首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   530篇
  免费   9篇
  国内免费   4篇
耳鼻咽喉   2篇
儿科学   3篇
妇产科学   9篇
基础医学   140篇
口腔科学   9篇
临床医学   51篇
内科学   15篇
皮肤病学   2篇
神经病学   56篇
特种医学   10篇
外科学   23篇
综合类   42篇
预防医学   24篇
眼科学   136篇
药学   17篇
中国医学   3篇
肿瘤学   1篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   15篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   9篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   19篇
  2013年   28篇
  2012年   15篇
  2011年   30篇
  2010年   20篇
  2009年   35篇
  2008年   36篇
  2007年   27篇
  2006年   24篇
  2005年   22篇
  2004年   22篇
  2003年   16篇
  2002年   7篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   7篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   8篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   7篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   10篇
  1984年   11篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   8篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   2篇
  1975年   2篇
  1972年   3篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   2篇
排序方式: 共有543条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
21.
目的 探讨手术消毒前超声定位在甲状腺疾病的手术治疗中的临床实用价值.方法 对80例甲状腺多发结节患者,于手术消毒前由术者行超声定位,观测病灶个数、病灶于上下极分布情况以及距后包膜的距离;并对病灶手术切除率、手术探查时间、声嘶、低钙并发症发生率进行统计,与未行超声定位的80例甲状腺多发结节患者进行对照.结果 80例患者术中按超声所观测数据确定的位置均能迅速找到病灶,病灶切除率为93.75%(75/80),多发性结节患者能缩短探查时间,结节距离后包膜5 mm以上者无需费时显露保护喉返神经,仅70.00%(56/80)显露喉返神经.未使用超声定位组对5 mm以下位置非表浅结节,均只能留作随访,病灶切除率76.25%(61/80),对临床可疑恶性、探查无法触及者,仅能做大范围切除活检(同侧叶次全切除),喉返神经显露达92.50%(74/80).在手术探查时间、病灶切除率和喉返神经显露率方面两组间的差异有统计学意义(P<0.01).两组中除1例因癌结节位于喉返神经入喉处术后出现声音略低外,均未发生严重并发症.结论 甲状腺疾病麻醉后消毒前超声定位,有助于了解甲状腺病灶的数量及与周围组织的三维关系,有利于术中迅速找到并准确切除病灶,是一种既能提高手术精确性、提高病灶切除率,又能降低手术难度的实用性很强的方法.  相似文献   
22.
杜芳  马兰  孙涛  张荫光  王昊 《北京医学》2008,30(9):519-520
目的 探讨曲面体层机中侧位纵向体层程序P21~23的投照技术及其在前牙区埋伏牙定位中的应用价值.方法 选取50例经曲面断层片或根尖片诊断为埋伏牙的患者,应用程序P21~23投照.分析埋伏牙的准确位置,并与埋伏牙拔除术中所见对比.结果 侧位纵向体层程序P21~23定位埋伏牙50例,其中偏唇侧13例,偏腭侧30例,居中7例,患牙的X线诊断与术中所见一致,准确率为100%.结论 侧位纵向体层程序P21~23是前牙区埋伏牙定位中更直接有效的方法 .  相似文献   
23.
非政府组织(NGO)在全球健康治理中发挥重要角色,已成为欧美发达国家和联合国系统全球健康战略的重要组成部分,联合国可持续发展目标也提出要强化NGO作为全球健康治理合作者的角色。本研究以全球健康战略比较成熟的世界卫生组织和美英两国为例,综述它们在全球健康战略中对NGO的定位和合作,为中国全球健康战略的制定以及与NGO的合作及定位提供参考建议。研究发现,世界卫生组织、美国和英国在其全球健康战略中均给予NGO明确定位,即加强相互合作,来扩大其在全球健康领域的影响力。因此,中国应将NGO纳入全球健康战略,建立NGO的管理和合作机制,使其成为全球健康的重要补充力量;严格筛选国际NGO进行合作,努力培育和支持本土NGO参与中国全球健康项目的实施;制定NGO合作清单和负面清单;同时加强监管和审核,规避NGO可能带来的风险。  相似文献   
24.
Summary Twelve golden hamsters learned visual discriminations in a Y-maze for a food reward. After initial training on a light/dark task, the hamsters learned to discriminate a grating from a uniform grey field of the same mean luminance as the grating; grating orientation was varied among hamsters. Those animals trained with the grating vertical or horizontal learned significantly faster than those trained on obliques. Acuity, measured by varying the spatial frequency of the grating according to the descending method of limits or the method of constant stimuli, was determined to be about 0.7 c/deg at 50% correct or 0.5 c/deg at 70% correct for all orientations tested (0 °, 45 °, 90 °, 135 °). Acuity was relatively constant within the human photopic range, but decreased to about 0.35 c/deg at 5 × 10-4cd/m2. The change in acuity as a function of luminance suggests that the hamster has a rod-dominated retina.  相似文献   
25.
Summary 1. Previous experiments have demonstrated that eye movements, acting through the extraocular muscle (EOM) proprioceptive afferents, are necessary for the development of orientation selectivity in the cells of the kitten visual cortex. New experiments were carried out to study the effect of the plane of eye movements on the preferred orientation acquired by the visual cortical cells. 2. Darkreared (DR) kittens were operated on at 5–6 weeks of age. In the first series of experiments, 4 out of the 6 EOMs were removed bilaterally in such a way that both eyes could only move in a single plane, either vertical or horizontal. In the second series of experiments, the same operation was performed on one eye which was also sutured shut and, on the other side, the EOM were deafferented by intracranial section of the ophthalmic branch of Vth nerve and the eye left open. 3. 1–4 days after surgery the kittens were given 6 h of visual experience and 12 h later were prepared for visual cell recording in Area 17. 4. In kittens of the first series: orientation selectivity developed in the majority (60–65%) of visual cells, most of which encoded horizontal orientations when the eyes had moved in the vertical plane and vertical orientations when the eyes had moved in the horizontal plane. These results show that the plane of eye movements during early visual experience influences the distribution of preferred orientations with an orthogonal relation. Ocular dominance histograms were strabismic like. 5. In kittens of the second series: orientation selectivity developed in 40–50% of cells, about half of which were tuned for the orientation orthogonal to the direction of movement of the occluded eye, as in experiment I. The seeing, deafferented eye, presumably would have sent normal visual inputs centrally, corresponding to displacements on the retina in every direction since the ocular motility of that eye had not been disturbed. However, proprioceptive information about its movements was suppressed. As only some of the EOMs of the occluded eye were still present and connected, the conclusion is that the observed influence of the plane of eye movements acts through the proprioceptive afferents. The ocular dominance histograms showed: 1) a powerful change in favour of the seeing eye after only 6 h of monocular visual experience; 2) a larger effect of monocular visual experience in the hemisphere contralateral to the seeing eye; 3) a linkage between acquisition of orientation selectivity and shift in ocular dominance. 6. Our results suggest that normal development of orientation selectivity in visual cortical cells results from the close association of visual and EOM afferent inputs. It is suggested that these two signals must occur with a precise temporal relationship.  相似文献   
26.
用新型激光衍射仪研究了铅对人红细胞在低粘切变流场中取向的影响,并观察了尼克酰胺的抗铅作用,结果表明,铅使人红细胞在C=0轨道上取向的数目下降,且有明显的剂量--效应关系,尼克酰胺有抗铅致人红细胞取向变化的作用。  相似文献   
27.
 The effects of stimuli falling outside the ’classical receptive field’ and their influence on the orientation selectivity of cells in the cat primary visual cortex are still matters of debate. Here we examine the variety of effects of such peripheral stimuli on responses to stimuli limited to the receptive field. We first determined the extent of the classical receptive field by increasing the diameter of a circular patch of drifting grating until the response saturated or reached a maximum, and by decreasing the diameter of a circular mask in the middle of an extended grating, centred on the receptive field, until the cell just began to respond. These two estimates always agreed closely. We then presented an optimum grating of medium-to-high contrast filling the classical receptive field while stimulating the surround with a drifting grating that had the same parameters as the central stimulus but was varied in orientation. For all but five neurons (of 37 tested), surround stimulation produced clear suppression over some range of orientations, while none showed explicit facilitation under these conditions. For 11 cells (34% of those showing suppression), the magnitude of suppression did not vary consistently with the orientation of the surround stimulus. In the majority of cells, suppression was weakest for a surround grating oriented orthogonal to the cell’s optimum. Nine of these cells (28%) exhibited maximum inhibition at the optimum orientation for the receptive field itself, but for 12 cells (38%) there was apparent ’release’ from inhibition for surround gratings at or near the cell’s optimum orientation and direction, leaving inhibition either maximal at angles flanking the optimum (9 cells) or broadly distributed over the rest of the orientation range (3 cells). This implies the existence of a subliminal facilitatory mechanism, tightly tuned at or near the cell’s optimum orientation, extending outside the classical receptive field. For just two cells of 13 tested the preferred orientation for a central grating was clearly shifted towards the orientation of a surrounding grating tilted away from the cell’s optimum. The contrast gain for central stimulation at the optimal orientation was measured with and without a surround pattern. For nine of 25 cells tested, surround stimulation at the cell’s optimum orientation facilitated the response to a central grating of low contrast (≤0.1) but inhibited that to a higher-contrast central stimulus: the contrast-response gain is reduced but the threshold contrast is actually decreased by surround stimulation. Hence the receptive field is effectively larger for low-contrast than for high-contrast stimuli. Inhibition from the periphery is usually greatest at or around the cell’s optimum, while suppression within the receptive field has been shown to be largely non-selective for orientation. Inhibition by orientations flanking the optimum could serve to sharpen orientation selectivity in the presence of contextual stimuli and to enhance orientational contrast; and it may play a part in orientation contrast illusions. Received: 28 July 1996 / Accepted: 30 January 1997  相似文献   
28.
Summary Psychophysical experiments on contrast vision were made using a modified Hermann grid which was graded in two directions. This pattern was composed of intersecting (IG) and intersected stripes (ID) representing 15 shades of a gray scale, and was viewed against 5 uniform backgrounds (BGD) ranging from white to black. Illusory light and dark patches at intersections were essentially limited to IG's that ranged in reflectance between BGD and ID. The illusion was most distinct when the contrast ratio between IG and BGD was minimum, while that between IG and ID was maximum; it was much reduced on intersections formed by equireflectant stripes, such as those in the original Hermann grids. Brighter responses were evoked by a white BGD and darker responses by a black BGD. Gray BGD's elicited both kinds of responses. The contrast effects were more pronounced for vertical and horizontal than for diagonal grid orientation. An optimum grid was devised that shows maximum brightness changes at almost every intersection, even when fixated foveally. The observations are interpreted in terms of Baumgartner's receptive field hypothesis.This work was supported (in part) by PHS Grant EY00227 from the National Eye Institute (to Dr. C.L. Schepens) and by a grant from the John A. Hartford Foundation, Inc. (to Dr. H.L. Teuber).JL worked under the summer student program of Chelsea High School  相似文献   
29.
Sex differences are consistently reported in human navigation. Indeed, to orient themselves during navigation women are more likely to use landmark-based strategies and men Euclidean-based strategies. The difference could be due to selective social pressure, which fosters greater spatial ability in men, or biological factors. And the great variability of the results reported in the literature could be due to the experimental setting more than real differences in ability. In this study, navigational behaviour was assessed by means of a place-learning task in which a modified version of the Morris water maze for humans was used to evaluate sex differences. In using landmarks, sex differences emerged only during the learning phase. Although the men were faster than the women in locating the target position, the differences between the sexes disappeared in delayed recall.  相似文献   
30.
Summary Single unit recording from visual cortex areas 17 and 18 and in the border region between them was performed on adult cats with unilateral optic tract section (OTX) either on the day of the operation (acute) or three to six months postoperatively (chronic). Visual activity from both hemispheres was analyzed with respect to the responsiveness level, the ocular dominance distribution and selectivity to orientation and direction. The results showed almost a complete absence of visual responsiveness in the deafferented hemisphere and a considerable reduction of responsiveness in the intact hemisphere. Following surgery an increase in visually responsive cells was found in the intact hemisphere as postoperative recovery continued. In addition, a reduction in the proportion of cells selective for orientation and direction was also found in the intact hemisphere of the OTX animals as compared to the control cats. Furthermore, cortical binocularity was not affected in the intact hemisphere of all OTX cats.We conclude that an almost total absence of interhemispheric callosal transfer of visual functions from the intact to the deafferented hemisphere is induced as a result of the unilateral OTX in adult cats. Moreover, the fact that the absence of cortical binocularity in the hemisphere receiving direct geniculate input was not disrupted, indicates the absence of callosal transfer from the deafferented to the intact hemisphere.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号