首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   988篇
  免费   70篇
  国内免费   3篇
耳鼻咽喉   9篇
儿科学   32篇
妇产科学   36篇
基础医学   116篇
口腔科学   16篇
临床医学   81篇
内科学   240篇
皮肤病学   8篇
神经病学   81篇
特种医学   47篇
外科学   150篇
综合类   9篇
一般理论   1篇
预防医学   123篇
眼科学   7篇
药学   63篇
中国医学   3篇
肿瘤学   39篇
  2023年   10篇
  2022年   18篇
  2021年   37篇
  2020年   19篇
  2019年   30篇
  2018年   29篇
  2017年   23篇
  2016年   41篇
  2015年   30篇
  2014年   47篇
  2013年   50篇
  2012年   60篇
  2011年   82篇
  2010年   43篇
  2009年   48篇
  2008年   49篇
  2007年   51篇
  2006年   45篇
  2005年   64篇
  2004年   45篇
  2003年   46篇
  2002年   56篇
  2001年   15篇
  2000年   16篇
  1999年   16篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   8篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   4篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   8篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   8篇
  1980年   2篇
  1977年   2篇
  1961年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1061条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
We describe a simple fluorometric method for determining aluminum in serum samples by monitoring the rate of reaction of 2-hydroxy-1-naphthaldehyde-p-methoxybenzoylhydrazone with aluminum ions. The emission of the resulting fluorescent metal-chelate formed is measured at 475 nm. Aluminum was measured in the supernate of serum after proteins were removed by precipitation with concentrated nitric acid, and calculations were based on the technique of standard additions. Within-run precision (CV) was 7.8% and 4.8% at mean aluminum concentrations of 7.7 and 60.7 micrograms/L, respectively (n = 10); between-run precision (CV) was 8.9% and 5.7% at mean aluminum concentrations of 23.3 and 46.8 micrograms/L, respectively (n = 10). The standard curve for the method is linear over the range of 0-250 micrograms of aluminum per liter. Samples from 49 patients were analyzed for aluminum by the proposed method (y) and by electrothermal atomic absorption spectroscopy (x). Linear regression analysis of the results yielded the equation y = 0.98x + 2.3 (r = 0.989, Syx = 6.7). The proposed method is comparable in sensitivity to the well-accepted atomic absorption spectrometric method but is simpler and less expensive.  相似文献   
2.
3.
4.
AIM: The G20210A mutation of the prothrombin gene is a genetic risk factor for venous thromboembolism (VTE). Variability exists in the mutation prevalence in both normal individuals and VTE patients. The aim of this study was to determine the mutation prevalence in Northwestern Greece and evaluate its association with VTE. METHODS: Presence of the G20210A mutation was investigated using DNA analysis in 176 consecutive patients with a history of venous thrombosis or pulmonary embolism and in 300 healthy controls, all Caucasian residents of Northwestern Greece. RESULTS: The mutation was present 12 patients (6.8%) and 8 controls (2.7%). The odds ratio for presence of the mutation versus the normal genotype in VTE was 2.7 (95% CI: 1.1 to 6.7), which was statistically significant. The prevalence of the G20210A prothrombin gene mutation in Northwestern Greece is 2.7% (95% CI: 0.8% to 4.4%) with an allele frequency of 1.3% (95% CI: 0.4% to 2.3%). CONCLUSION: The G20210A mutation of the prothrombin gene is associated with VTE in the Caucasian residents of this geographic region.  相似文献   
5.
A randomized trial was conducted to compare the efficacy and safety of fluconazole versus that of amphotericin B in the treatment of candidemia in non-neutropenic adults. Enrollment was stratified by disease severity (APACHE II score). Patients were randomized (1:1) to receive amphotericin B 0.6 mg/kg/day (cumulative dose 8 mg/kg) or fluconazole 800 mg intravenous loading dose, then 400 mg daily for four weeks (intravenous for at least 10 days). Patients were monitored for six months. A total of 106 patients were enrolled. A protocol amendment implemented midway through the trial required patients to be removed from the study and treated with amphotericin B if species identification indicated candidemia due toCandida glabrata orCandida krusei. Baseline characteristics were similar for the two groups; 103 patients (fluconazole, 50; amphotericin B, 53) met the major enrollment criteria. The intention-to-treat analysis indicated successful therapy in 50% of fluconazole recipients compared to 58% of the amphotericin B group (p=0.39; one-sided 95% Cl, –8 to 24%). The efficacy analysis included 84 patients (fluconazole, 42; amphotericin B, 42); successful outcomes were observed in 57% and 62% of cases in the fluconazole and amphotericin B groups, respectively (p=0.66: one-sided 95% Cl, –12 to 22%). The mortality at day 14 for the fluconazole group was 26% and for the amphotericin B group 21% (p=0.52; chi-square test) and remained similar throughout the course of follow-up. Drug-related adverse events were more frequent with amphotericin B than with fluconazole and prompted switching of therapy for two (4%) and zero cases, respectively. Fluconazole and amphotericin B were associated with similar clinical response rates and survival in the treatment of candidemia among non-neutropenic patients; however, drug-related adverse events were more frequent with amphotericin B.  相似文献   
6.
7.
Insertion of transposable elements is a major cause of genomic expansion in eukaryotes. Less is understood, however, about mechanisms underlying contraction of genomes. In this study, we show that retroelements can, in rare cases, be precisely deleted from primate genomes, most likely via recombination between 10- to 20-bp target site duplications (TSDs) flanking the retroelement. The deleted loci are indistinguishable from pre-integration sites, effectively reversing the insertion. Through human-chimpanzee-Rhesus monkey genomic comparisons, we estimate that 0.5%-1% of apparent retroelement "insertions" distinguishing humans and chimpanzees actually represent deletions. Furthermore, we demonstrate that 19% of genomic deletions of 200-500 bp that have occurred since the human-chimpanzee divergence are associated with flanking identical repeats of at least 10 bp. A large number of deletions internal to Alu elements were also found flanked by homologies. These results suggest that illegitimate recombination between short direct repeats has played a significant role in human genome evolution. Moreover, this study lends perspective to the view that insertions of retroelements represent unidirectional genetic events.  相似文献   
8.
4'-Methyl-alpha-pyrrolidinopropiophenone (MPPP) is a new drug of abuse. It is believed to have an abuse potential similar to that of amphetamines. Previous studies with Wistar rats had shown that MPPP was metabolized mainly by hydroxylation in position 4' followed by dehydrogenation to the corresponding carboxylic acid. The aim of the study presented here was to identify the human hepatic cytochrome p450 (p450) enzymes involved in the biotransformation of MPPP to 4'-hydroxymethyl-pyrrolidinopropiophenone. Baculovirus-infected insect cell microsomes and human liver microsomes were used for this purpose. Only CYP2C19 and CYP2D6 catalyzed this hydroxylation. The apparent Km and Vmax values for the latter were 9.8 +/- 2.5 microM and 13.6 +/- 0.7 pmol/min/pmol p450, respectively. CYP2C19 was not saturable over the tested substrate range (2-1000 microM) and interestingly showed a biphasic kinetic profile with apparent Km,1 and Vmax,1 values of 47.2 +/- 12.5 microM and 8.1 +/- 1.4 pmol/min/pmol p450, respectively. Experiments with pooled human liver microsomes also revealed biphasic nonsaturable kinetics with apparent Km,1 and Vmax,1 values of 57.0 +/- 20.9 microM and 199.7 +/- 59.7 pmol/min/mg of protein for the high affinity enzyme, respectively. Incubation of 2 microM MPPP with 3 microM of the CYP2D6-specific inhibitor quinidine resulted in significant (p < 0.01) turnover inhibition (11.8 +/- 1.6% of control). Based on kinetic data corrected for the relative activity factors, CYP2D6 is the enzyme mainly responsible for MPPP hydroxylation, confirmed by CYP2D6 inhibition studies.  相似文献   
9.
To increase their chances of survival, prey often behave unpredictably when escaping from predators. However, the response of predators to, and hence the effectiveness of, such tactics is unknown. We programmed interactive prey to flee from an approaching fish predator (the blue acara, Andinoacara pulcher) using real-time computer vision and two-wheeled robots that controlled the prey’s movements via magnets. This allowed us to manipulate the prey’s initial escape direction and how predictable it was between successive trials with the same individual predator. When repeatedly exposed to predictable prey, the predators adjusted their behavior before the prey even began to escape: prey programmed to escape directly away were approached more rapidly than prey escaping at an acute angle. These faster approach speeds compensated for a longer time needed to capture such prey during the subsequent pursuit phase. By contrast, when attacking unpredictable prey, the predators adopted intermediate approach speeds and were not sensitive to the prey’s escape angle but instead showed greater acceleration during the pursuit. Collectively, these behavioral responses resulted in the prey’s predictability having no net effect on the time taken to capture prey, suggesting that unpredictable escape behavior may be advantageous to prey in fewer circumstances than originally thought. Rather than minimizing capture times, the predators in our study appear to instead adjust their behavior to maintain an adequate level of performance during prey capture.

Rapid evasive responses are a vital tool prey use to minimize capture by predators (1, 2). Despite their ubiquity, it can be challenging to demonstrate the benefit of escape strategies, due to the difficulties involved in designing studies which integrate realistic predation with manipulation of prey behavior that experimentally controls for confounding effects. Studying the behavior of real predators is crucial when attempting to demonstrate the adaptive value of prey adaptations, especially when these are dependent on features of predator cognition (35). This applies particularly to unpredictable escape behavior by prey, which is thought to enhance prey survival by compromising the ability of predators to anticipate the movement of their target (6). Although unpredictable escape tactics are widespread taxonomically (7, 8), we know little about how real predators respond to unpredictability in prey escape strategies and whether this prevents predators from adjusting their behavior over multiple interactions with prey (9, 10).Controlled experiments in which human predators target continuously moving virtual prey have demonstrated that abrupt and unpredictable changes in direction reduce the accuracy of prey targeting (11, 12). However, it is unknown whether the survival advantage conferred by unpredictable motion also applies against nonhuman predators. Additionally, the escape responses of prey which are initially stationary are common in nature, as numerous prey taxa freeze once they have detected a potential threat or remain motionless to avoid detection by predators, before eventually fleeing only once a predator gets too close (1, 1315). One way for stationary prey to be unpredictable is to vary the initial escape angle from one encounter to the next (16). This is a distinct tactic to the unpredictable movements made by prey which move continuously regardless of the presence of a predatory threat (6) or the abrupt turns made by some prey in anticipation of a predator’s attack (17). Although theoretical models predict that for a predator of a given speed, prey should select a single optimal escape trajectory which maximizes the distance from an approaching predator (18, 19), predators might anticipate the movements of prey which repeatedly escape at a fixed angle relative to their approach (20). Contrary to expectations based on a single optimal escape path, a wide range of prey species show a substantial degree of variability in their initial escape angles (16), including amphibians (21), crustaceans (22, 23), fish (2427), insects (28, 29), mammals (30), and reptiles (31). Given that this variability is so widespread taxonomically, investigating whether it represents an antipredator strategy aimed at generating unpredictability could have major implications for our understanding of prey escape behavior (32, 33).Many predator-prey interactions are typified by feedback between the attacker and the target (34), making it difficult to disentangle the effects of prey defenses on predators from the impact of predator behavior on prey using a purely observational approach. One way to determine the importance of prey defensive tactics is to present real predators with standardized virtual prey, whose movements and behavior can be precisely controlled and experimentally manipulated (3539). However, previous experiments with virtual prey have used unresponsive prey items which do not react to predators, and do not allow the predator to capture prey and be rewarded, making it extremely challenging to study repeated interactions between predators and prey. These limitations can be overcome by using interactive robotic prey (40).To study the effect of unpredictability in prey escape on predators, we developed an experimental system [Fig. 1A; see also Swain et al. (41)], in which artificial robot-controlled prey were programmed to flee from blue acara cichlid (Andinoacara pulcher) predatory fish. Blue acaras are opportunistic predators with a broad diet but actively pursue highly evasive prey such as Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata) (42, 43). Prey initiated their escape response once the predator had approached within a threshold distance (Fig. 1B), mimicking the tendency of many prey to flee from a distant predator at submaximal speeds (14, 44). After an initial period in which groups of blue acaras were trained to attack the prey (the training period, SI Appendix, SI Methods), individual predators were repeatedly presented with prey which escaped either in predictable or unpredictable directions over 20 successive experimental trials (the test period). For individuals assigned to the predictable treatment (which acted as the control), prey escaped at the same angle relative to the predator’s approach from one trial to the next, whereas in the unpredictable treatment, prey were programmed to flee in random directions over successive trials (Fig. 1C). To successfully capture prey, pursuit predators must respond to changes in prey direction, which occur at the start of a chase (4547). Across trials with predictable prey, the predators had the opportunity to gain reliable information about the prey’s likely escape direction, in contrast to the unpredictable treatment where the prey’s escape angle in previous trials was not a reliable indicator of its escape direction in future encounters. If unpredictable escape behavior is adaptive, increased uncertainty about the prey’s likely escape direction in the unpredictable treatment should reduce the performance of the predator in these trials, with slower speeds of approach (i.e., before the prey respond), longer times taken to capture prey, and/or greater kinematic costs resulting from higher speeds, increased acceleration, and more turning during the pursuit.Open in a separate windowFig. 1.The robotic prey system. (A) Diagram (not to scale) showing a side view of the experimental system, with the Bluetooth-controlled robot situated on a platform underneath the experimental tank and the webcam used to monitor the predator’s movements suspended overhead. The prey’s movements are controlled by the robot via magnets, enabling the prey to escape from an attacking predator once the predator approaches within 27 cm of the prey’s starting position. See ref. 41 for a similar system designed for robotic predators attacking prey fish shoals. (B) Prey escape angles were defined relative to the predator’s approach direction. (C) In the predictable treatment, prey escaped at the same initial angle over successive trials (the escape angle varied between individual predators). In the unpredictable treatment, the prey’s initial escape angle varied randomly from trial to trial. While the experiment manipulated the prey’s initial escape angle, the prey’s subsequent escape trajectory was fixed as a straight-line path in both treatments.  相似文献   
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号