全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8572篇 |
免费 | 199篇 |
国内免费 | 58篇 |
专业分类
耳鼻咽喉 | 34篇 |
儿科学 | 243篇 |
妇产科学 | 22篇 |
基础医学 | 1947篇 |
口腔科学 | 31篇 |
临床医学 | 1042篇 |
内科学 | 257篇 |
皮肤病学 | 3篇 |
神经病学 | 3032篇 |
特种医学 | 242篇 |
外科学 | 373篇 |
综合类 | 400篇 |
一般理论 | 1篇 |
预防医学 | 450篇 |
眼科学 | 19篇 |
药学 | 491篇 |
20篇 | |
中国医学 | 216篇 |
肿瘤学 | 6篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 54篇 |
2024年 | 132篇 |
2023年 | 149篇 |
2022年 | 231篇 |
2021年 | 342篇 |
2020年 | 275篇 |
2019年 | 287篇 |
2018年 | 296篇 |
2017年 | 257篇 |
2016年 | 224篇 |
2015年 | 198篇 |
2014年 | 417篇 |
2013年 | 585篇 |
2012年 | 312篇 |
2011年 | 391篇 |
2010年 | 274篇 |
2009年 | 351篇 |
2008年 | 364篇 |
2007年 | 362篇 |
2006年 | 334篇 |
2005年 | 284篇 |
2004年 | 264篇 |
2003年 | 193篇 |
2002年 | 180篇 |
2001年 | 166篇 |
2000年 | 215篇 |
1999年 | 165篇 |
1998年 | 119篇 |
1997年 | 110篇 |
1996年 | 100篇 |
1995年 | 103篇 |
1994年 | 83篇 |
1993年 | 80篇 |
1992年 | 74篇 |
1991年 | 84篇 |
1990年 | 61篇 |
1989年 | 59篇 |
1988年 | 63篇 |
1987年 | 64篇 |
1986年 | 43篇 |
1985年 | 59篇 |
1984年 | 50篇 |
1983年 | 28篇 |
1982年 | 34篇 |
1981年 | 29篇 |
1980年 | 35篇 |
1979年 | 44篇 |
1978年 | 38篇 |
1977年 | 29篇 |
1976年 | 26篇 |
排序方式: 共有8829条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Dr. Christopher G. Goetz Glenn T. Stebbins Lucy M. Blasucci Mitchell S. Grobman 《Movement disorders》1997,12(6):1039-1041
Patient on-off diaries are used in clinical trials, but a method to assure agreement between patient and examiner has never been developed. We tested whether a patient-teaching tape increased the rate of agreement between patient diary ratings and simultaneous neurologic assessment by a trained professional. A total of 32 consecutive patients who had Parkinson's disease with motor fluctuations independently completed a 4-h on-off diary (nine ratings) at the same time as an examiner. Those with <80% agreement with the examiner (n = 20) were randomized to view either a training tape that showed motor fluctuations (experimental group) or another videotape of general patient educational material (control group). All patients then underwent the same 4-h assessment of motor fluctuations. To test for long-term retention, they returned 1 month later and, without reviewing the videotape, underwent a final 4-h correlation assessment. After the training tape, the experimental group showed significant improvement, whereas the control group showed no improvement. Furthermore, another month later, the improvement in the experimental group was retained. Based on these findings, we suggest that future clinical trials assessing motor fluctuations incorporate this tape into their basic methodology. 相似文献
2.
In order to define precisely the relation between descending monoaminergic systems and the motor system, we measured in the ventral horn of spinal cord of adult rats the variations of extracellular concentrations of 5-HT, 5-HIAA, DA and MHPG. Measurements were performed during rest, endurance running on a treadmill, and a post-exercise period, with microdialysis probes implanted permanently for 45 days. We found a slight decrease in both 5-HT and 5-HIAA during locomotion with a more marked decrease during the post-exercise period compared to the mean of rest values. In contrast, the concentration of DA and MHPG increased slightly during the exercise and decreased thereafter. These results, when compared with those of a previous study, which measured monoamines in the spinal cord white matter [C. Gerin, D. Bécquet, A. Privat, Direct evidence for the link between monoaminergic descending pathways and motor activity: I. A study with microdialysis probes implanted in the ventral funiculus of the spinal cord, Brain Res. 704 (1995) 191–201], highlight the complex regulation of the release of monoamines that occurs in the ventral horn. 相似文献
3.
Ellis RR Flanagan JR Lederman SJ 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1999,125(2):109-114
Visual size illusions have been shown to affect perceived object size but not the aperture of the hand when reaching to those
same objects. Thus, vision for perception is said to be dissociated from vision for action. The present study examines the
effect of visual-position and visual-shape illusions on both the visually perceived center of an object and the position of
a grasp on that object when a balanced lift is required. The results for both experiments show that although the illusions
influence both the perceived and the grasped estimates of the center position, the grasp position is more veridical. This
partial dissociation is discussed in terms of its implications for streams of visual processing.
Received: 17 November 1997 / Accepted: 11 September 1998 相似文献
4.
A. W. English O. I. Weeks 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1984,56(2):361-368
Summary The distribution of muscle fibers in single muscle units in cat lateral gastrocnemius (LG) muscle was analyzed using the method of glycogen depletion. Single, type FF motor units in LG were identified by stimulation of dissected ventral rootlets and then activated to produce depletion of glycogen in their innervated muscle fibers. Serial histological sections were made of the entire triceps surae muscles and then reacted for the demonstration of glycogen. Projected tracings of the sections were used to compare the distribution of glycogen depleted muscle fibers to the heads of the LG muscle. Results of analysis of single muscle units indicate that the muscle fibers innervated by single cat LG type FF motor units are distributed to the same regions of muscle which are innervated by primary branches of the nerve to the LG muscle. The single units are thus compartmentalized. These results support the hypothesis that each of the neuromuscular compartments of LG contains a unique population of motor units. It is suggested that compartmentalization of muscles about primary branches of their muscle nerves may from a major organizational principle in the design of skeletal muscles. 相似文献
5.
Schieppati M. Crenna P. 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1985,59(2):249-256
Summary We studied the potential contribution of postsynaptic mechanisms to the depression of reflex excitability which occurs immediately after a voluntary release from tonic muscle contraction. The excitability of the Soleus (Sol) motor pool was tested at rest and after voluntary muscle relaxation. In both cases the Sol H-reflex was conditioned by 1. a single shock to the peroneal nerve, in order to activate the Ia interneurones (INs) mediating the reciprocal inhibition via a peripheral input, or by 2. a short-lasting voluntary contraction of the Tibialis Anterior (TA) muscle, to activate the Ia INs via a central command. Changes in excitability of Renshaw cells were also tested at rest and after release, to assess the role of recurrent inhibition in the release-induced inhibition of the Sol H-reflex. It was demonstrated that: 1. the excitability of the INs mediating the reciprocal inhibition was only slightly enhanced in comparison with resting conditions; 2. the H-reflex of the antagonist muscle (TA) evoked after Sol release was not consistently facilitated with respect to rest; 3. the command to contract the TA muscle reduced the H-reflex of the Sol muscle during rest but not after Sol release; 4. recurrent inhibition did not increase its effect in the post-release period. Such features suggest that recurrent and reciprocal post-synaptic inhibitions do not play a major role in reducing the reflex excitability of a relaxing muscle; rather, the command to release prevents the reciprocal inhibitory effect which accompanies the contraction of the antagonist muscle. The findings support the concept that release-induced reflex depression is mediated mainly by presynaptic inhibition of autogenetic spindle afferences (Schieppati and Crenna 1984).Supported by Italian M.P.I. 相似文献
6.
Kumar S Ferrari R Narayan Y Vieira ER 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2005,167(3):345-351
The purpose of this study was to determine the response of the cervical muscles to increasing low-velocity, whiplash-type
lateral impacts when the occupant is seated out of the recommended driving position (neutral posture). Twenty healthy volunteers
were subjected to left lateral impacts of 4.1, 7.7, 10.5, and 13.7 m/s2 acceleration, with their trunk flexed by 45° and laterally flexed to the right and left also by 45° at the time of impact.
Bilateral electromyograms of the sternocleidomastoids, trapezii, and splenii capitis were recorded. Under these conditions
of trunk-flexed postures, in a left lateral impact, muscle responses were of generally low magnitude with the trunk flexed
to either the left or right. Even at the highest acceleration of 13.7 m/s2, all muscles generated less than 37% of their known maximal voluntary contraction electromyogram. Also, in these left lateral
impacts, the right splenius capitis showed a greater EMG response than the left splenius capitis regardless of whether the
subject was flexed to the right or left at the time of impact. The right splenius capitis (the one contralateral to the left
lateral impact direction) was more active than its counterpart. Compared to what is known for EMG responses with an occupant
in the neutral posture, the right sternocleidomastoid (usually the most active muscle in a left lateral collision) was significantly
less-active with trunk flexion than with neutral posture conditions (P<0.01). In the absence of bodily impact, the flexed trunk posture does not produce a biomechanical response that would increase
the likelihood of cervical muscle injury in low velocity lateral impacts, and may lessen the risk of injury for some muscles. 相似文献
7.
Gribova A Donchin O Bergman H Vaadia E Cardoso De Oliveira S 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2002,144(3):322-335
It has been established that repeated presentation of a transient target motion stimulus such as a constant-velocity ramp leads to the build up of steady state (SS), anticipatory smooth pursuit eye movements after two or three presentations. Each SS response is then composed of the anticipatory component of nonvisual origin, a visual component associated with the stimulus presentation and another nonvisual component that represents the decay of the response after extinction of the stimulus. Here we investigated the interactions that occur when each motion stimulus was itself a sequence containing more than one ramp component. Ramp components had a velocity of 15 degrees /s or 30 degrees /s to left or right and were separated by gaps of 200 ms duration. In an initial experiment, responses to 2-ramp stimuli were examined and compared with responses to the single-ramp stimuli from which they were constituted. We present evidence that the anticipatory, nonvisual components of the double-ramp response result from the linear summation of the nonvisual components of the responses to the constituent single-ramp components. In a 2nd experiment, we examined responses to a wide variety of 4-ramp sequences and again found evidence that, in the SS, the responses were formed from the linear summation of the constituent single-ramp components. Regression analysis performed on the velocity at onset of each ramp component indicated that this nonvisual part of the response was predictive of the upcoming ramp component. To confirm this, unexpected changes were introduced into single ramp components of the 4-ramp sequence after at least five prior presentations of the sequence had allowed a SS response to be established. Subjects continued to initiate a response to the modified component that was appropriate in velocity and direction for the corresponding part of the previous sequence and inappropriate for the newly modified stimulus. This preprogrammed response persisted unmodified for more than 170 ms after onset of the modified ramp component. In contrast, in the second presentation of the new sequence, the anticipatory component of the response was highly correlated with the SS response of the new sequence, but not with that of the prior sequence, showing that the preprogrammed response had been modified very rapidly. Similar behaviour was observed whichever of the 4-ramp components was modified, indicating that the velocity and direction of the anticipatory response to each component had been preprogrammed. The results suggest that velocity information related to at least four elements of a sequence can be temporarily stored and subsequently released with appropriate temporal order to form an anticipatory response throughout the whole sequence. 相似文献
8.
Dr. H. Gemba K. Sasaki 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1984,55(1):26-32
Summary A monkey was trained to lift a lever by wrist extension in response to a light stimulus. During the learning process of the task over several months, field potentials related not only to the task performance but also to substitution and stimulation experiments were recorded with chronically implanted electrodes on the surface and at a depth of 2.5–3.0 mm in the prefrontal, premotor, motor and prestriate cortices. In the substitution experiment, an examiner lifted a lever for the monkey so that it was watching the light and rewarded without the hand movement. In the stimulation experiment, the same light stimulus was simply delivered to the monkey. In a naive monkey which lifted the lever independently of the stimulus, stimulus-locked potentials were evoked by the task experiment in those cortices except the motor cortex, but none was elicited by the substitution or stimulation experiment. In a welltrained monkey, the substitution and stimulation experiments induced almost the same potentials as those prior to the task movement in respective cortices except the motor cortex, in which the component of cerebellar-induced premovement potential was not observed during the substitution and stimulation experiments. At an intermediate stage of learning, the situation was intermediate between the naive and well-trained stages and most premovement potentials except those in the motor cortex were elicited by the substitution experiment in reduced sizes, but nothing by the stimulation experiment.The present study suggests that the neuronal circuits for the operantly conditioned movement are functionally organized and gradually consolidated in the learning process, and that the consolidation is made earlier for the circuit involving association and premotor cortices than the circuit including the motor cortex in the process. The circuit to the motor cortex via the cerebro-cerebellar interconnection is recruited only on the execution of movement. 相似文献
9.
Franz EA Packman T 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2004,157(2):174-180
This study examined the hypothesis that the mirror reflection of one hands movement directly influences motor output of the other (hidden) hand, during performance of bimanual drawing. A mirror was placed between the two hands during bimanual circle drawing, with one hand and its reflection visible and the other hand hidden. Bimanual spatial coupling was enhanced by the mirror reflection, as shown by measures of circle size. Effects of the mirror reflection differed significantly from effects of vision to one hand alone, but did not differ from a control task performed in full vision. There was no evidence of a consistent phase lead of the visible hand, which indicates that the observed effects on spatial coupling were immediate and not based on time-consuming feedback processes. We argue that visual mirror symmetry fools the brain into believing it sees both hands moving rather than one. Consequently, the spatial properties of movement of the two hands become more similar through a process that is virtually automatic. 相似文献
10.
Ferrarin M Gironi M Mendozzi L Nemni R Mazzoleni P Rabuffetti M 《Medical & biological engineering & computing》2005,43(3):349-356
Cerebellar ataxia is a complex motor disturbance that involves the planning and execution of movements and reduces movement
accuracy and co-ordination. The quantification of ataxic signs is commonly realised through visual examination of motor tasks
performed by the patient and assignment of scores to specific items composing the international co-operative ataxia rating
scale (ICARS). The present work studied an experimental procedure to characterise specific aspects of motor disturbances in
ataxia objectively. Four tests belonging to the ICARS were considered: walking, knee-tibia test, finger-to-nose and finger-to-finger
test. Through a kinematic analysis performed during the above tests, specific indices were defined to quantify velocity, linearity,
asymmetry, tremor, instability and smoothness of movement or posture. The procedure was applied to five patients with cerebellar
ataxia and to ten healthy adult subjects. Results demonstrated that the patients moved significantly more slowly than the
healthy subjects (0.67 against 0.97 m s−1 and 0.81 against 1.02 m s−1, respectively, for straight walk and finger-to-nose tests) and showed poorer linearity and smoothness behaviour. Velocity,
linearity, tremor, smoothness and instability indices showed moderate to good correlation with the corresponding ICARS score.
Some of these indices can separately evaluate aspects that are combined in single ICARS subscores. It is concluded that the
combination of clinical assessments and instrumental evaluations allows a better insight into ataxic patients' motor disturbances
and is a useful tool for the definition and follow-up of rehabilitation programmes. 相似文献