全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6235篇 |
免费 | 193篇 |
国内免费 | 61篇 |
专业分类
耳鼻咽喉 | 64篇 |
儿科学 | 106篇 |
妇产科学 | 53篇 |
基础医学 | 1263篇 |
口腔科学 | 90篇 |
临床医学 | 262篇 |
内科学 | 160篇 |
皮肤病学 | 14篇 |
神经病学 | 1091篇 |
特种医学 | 160篇 |
外科学 | 217篇 |
综合类 | 288篇 |
预防医学 | 256篇 |
眼科学 | 1872篇 |
药学 | 414篇 |
1篇 | |
中国医学 | 143篇 |
肿瘤学 | 35篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 15篇 |
2023年 | 78篇 |
2022年 | 119篇 |
2021年 | 182篇 |
2020年 | 163篇 |
2019年 | 294篇 |
2018年 | 292篇 |
2017年 | 157篇 |
2016年 | 144篇 |
2015年 | 129篇 |
2014年 | 272篇 |
2013年 | 354篇 |
2012年 | 217篇 |
2011年 | 310篇 |
2010年 | 232篇 |
2009年 | 280篇 |
2008年 | 256篇 |
2007年 | 252篇 |
2006年 | 233篇 |
2005年 | 205篇 |
2004年 | 183篇 |
2003年 | 190篇 |
2002年 | 135篇 |
2001年 | 146篇 |
2000年 | 156篇 |
1999年 | 141篇 |
1998年 | 87篇 |
1997年 | 89篇 |
1996年 | 79篇 |
1995年 | 85篇 |
1994年 | 66篇 |
1993年 | 62篇 |
1992年 | 55篇 |
1991年 | 60篇 |
1990年 | 37篇 |
1989年 | 56篇 |
1988年 | 49篇 |
1987年 | 39篇 |
1986年 | 40篇 |
1985年 | 83篇 |
1984年 | 66篇 |
1983年 | 50篇 |
1982年 | 56篇 |
1981年 | 53篇 |
1980年 | 32篇 |
1979年 | 77篇 |
1978年 | 30篇 |
1977年 | 19篇 |
1976年 | 21篇 |
1975年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有6489条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
M. Hishinuma T. Yamaguchi 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1990,82(3):597-605
Summary The descending pathways responsible for eliciting forelimb stepping are located in the lateral funiculus (Yamaguchi 1986). In order to determine into which spinal segments the descending pathways project and to know the projections and functions of the other descending system, the ventral funicular pathways, we placed various lesions in the cervical spinal cord of decerebrate cats with the lower thoracic cord transected and studied their effects on forelimb stepping evoked by stimulation of the midbrain locomotor region. (1) The lateral funiculus was transected on one side. The operation removes descending input to all the segments caudal to the lesion. Experiments with serial transections from the caudal to rostral segment revealed that stepping activity of the limb on the lesioned side is reduced when the lesion is placed at the level between the C6 and C7 segment and then between C5 and C6. A slight reduction of activity was also observed after a lesion placed between C7 and C8. (2) Consistently, bilateral transection of the lateral funiculus at the level between C5 and C6 abolished stepping movements of both forelimbs. (3) The cervical cord was split in the parasagittal plane through the dorsal root entry. The operation removes the descending input to the segment in which the lesion is placed. The parasagittal lesions from the C1 to C6 did not abolish stepping activity, although a lesion placed between C5 and C6 could slightly affect stepping. The results, (1)–(3) suggest that the lateral funicular pathways project into the spinal segments mainly at the C6–C7 level with some rostrocaudal extension into C5 and C8. (4) Complete transections of the medial part of the spinal cord cut extensor bursts short and raised stepping frequency. Nevertheless, if the lesion at C1–C5 spared the ventromedial part of the ventral funiculus, it did not result in such high-frequency stepping or in weakened extensor activity. In the case of segments caudal to C6, medial transections which spared the corresponding region could result in such stepping. It is suggested that the pathways descending through the ventromedial part of the ventral funiculus in the rostral segments provide extensor activity during stepping. They may change their course in the more dorsal part of the ventral funiculus below the C6 and presumably project into the grey matter of more caudal segments. 相似文献
22.
The third derivative of motion or the change in the rate of acceleration (also known as jerk) is examined in terms of its applicability to the study of psychophysiological function. An algorithm of this third derivative is presented to show that jerk can be derived from the arithmetic difference of two slopes which constitute the portion of motion being differentiated. By modifying the algorithm, a new parameter termed “proportional jerk’ or PJ is formulated whereby one slope is measured relative to the other slope; this PJ provides information about the smoothness of movement without being influenced by the velocity as is the traditional jerk measure. A practical application of the PJ to waking saccades and REMs in 11 human subjects revealed that REMs are significantly “jerkier’ or less smooth than waking calibration eye movements. Whereas the waking eye movements had a well-defined negative phase of the PJ, the REMs did not show such stereotypical behavior. This is in accord with previous work which showed that waking saccade velocity increases to a maximum and then decreases whereas REMs maintain their peak or near peak velocities for varying periods. It was suggested that PJ can be useful in detecting subtle temporal changes in the course of movements and may be used as a parameter of motion even when the absolute amplitude is unknown. 相似文献
23.
Hamzei F Dettmers C Rijntjes M Glauche V Kiebel S Weber B Weiller C 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2002,146(3):273-281
The aim of this functional magnetic resonance imaging study was to investigate differences in visuomotor control with increasing task complexity. Twelve right-handed volunteers were asked to perform their signature under different degrees of visual control: internally generated movement with closed eyes, signing with open eyes, tracking the line of the projected signature forwards, and tracking the line of the projected signature backwards. There was a gradual onset and disappearance of activation within a distributed network. Parietal, lateral and medial frontal brain areas were activated during all conditions, confirming the involvement of a parieto-frontal system. The weight of activation shifted with increasing task complexity. Internally generated movements activated predominantly the inferior parietal lobule and the ventral premotor cortex, as well as the rostral cingulate area, pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) and SMA proper. Opening the eyes reduced SMA and cingulate activation and activated increasingly the occipito-parietal areas with higher task complexity. Visually guided movements produced an activation predominantly in the superior parietal lobule and dorsal premotor cortex. This study bridges human activation studies with the results of neurophysiological studies with monkeys. It confirms a gradual transition of visuomotor control with increasing task complexity within a distributed parieto-frontal network. 相似文献
24.
Dietrich L. Meyer Dr. Detlef Bonnemann Prof. Dr. Kurt -Peter Schaefer 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1973,18(5):505-511
Summary Turning a rabbit on a turn-table for a few degrees induces compensatory eye-movements and results in an asymmetry of tonus in the optomotor system. If the visual input is discontinued (darkness), this asymmetry decays and the eyes drift back to the mid-position within 12–18 sec. The equalization of such asymmetries of tonus under normal conditions and under curare is described. Tonus asymmetries induced by tilting the animals about the longitudinal axis are neither compensated under visual, nor under non-visual, conditions. Recordings were taken from oculomotor neurons, and changes of their firing frequencies were used as a measure for eye movements.A preliminary report was given at the spring meeting of the German Physiological Society 1973.Supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, SFB 33. 相似文献
25.
Rapid eye movements (REMs), EEG alpha, and tonic heart rate (HR) were measured during 6 types of cognitive tasks—imagining a liked person, suppressing thoughts of the person, searching one's mind for alternative solutions, arithmetic involving little concentration, problems involving high concentration, and choosing a preferred activity. The latter 3 required verbalization, the former 3 did not. Only suppression and search did not differ significantly from each other on at least one physiological variable. Imagining, suppression, and search yielded few REMs, high alpha, and low HR. High concentration yielded many REMs, low alpha, and high HR. Choice yielded many REMs, low alpha, and intermediate HR. Low concentration yielded few REMs, low alpha, and high HR. Suppression produced somewhat less alpha than imagining but did not differ significantly in REMs. 相似文献
26.
MacDougall HG Brizuela AE Curthoys IS 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2003,148(2):166-175
Recent studies have shown that, although responses to long-duration, constant-current surface galvanic vestibular stimulation
(GVS) show substantial interindividual variability, individual subjects show a reliable, repeatable, idiosyncratic oculomotor
response pattern to GVS. It follows that GVS may be a more reliable stimulus than may have been anticipated from the literature.
The aim of the present study was to examine the metrics of 3D eye-movement responses to maintained (120 s), unilateral and
bilateral surface GVS. Eye movements were measured using computerised video-oculography. Two experiments were conducted: Experiment
1 examined whether the normal response is linear over increasing levels of current; and Experiment 2 examined (1) whether
the normal response to surface GVS is symmetrical when comparing stimulated sides, (2) whether the normal response to surface
GVS is symmetrical when the polarity of the stimulating current was reversed, and (3) whether there is additivity in the normal
response to combinations of unilateral/bilateral surface GVS. Five subjects participated in Experiment 1 and eight subjects
participated in Experiment 2. In both experiments, the onset of stimulation produced characteristic eye-movement responses:
changes in torsional position with the upper pole of both eyes rolling towards the anode and away from the cathode; together
with horizontal and torsional nystagmus with slow phases towards the anode and away from the cathode; and negligible vertical
nystagmus. These responses reversed direction at stimulus offset. In the fixation condition of Experiment 1, the magnitude
of ocular torsional position (OTP) and torsional nystagmus responses showed a linear relationship over conditions of increasing
current strength, as did OTP, torsional and horizontal nystagmus responses in darkness. The results of Experiment 2 showed
that responses to unilateral stimulation are symmetrical between stimulated sides, symmetrical between stimulating polarities,
and additive (with respect to responses to bilateral stimulation). The principles derived from these findings, as well as
those of recent studies, provide a foundation for future work investigating eye-movement responses to surface GVS in patients
with known types of vestibular dysfunction.
Electronic Publication 相似文献
27.
Knox PC Davidson JH Anderson D 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2005,165(1):1-7
Quantitative analysis of eye movements is a useful tool for examining the behavioural effects of ageing. Although the effect of ageing on saccadic eye movement has been examined in some detail, the effect of ageing on a second class of eye movement, smooth pursuit (SP), has received less attention. We examined the initiation of SP in a group of fifteen healthy older people (mean age 72 years) and compared their performance with that of ten young controls (mean age 21 years). Although their performance was qualitatively similar, pursuit latency was increased in the older group. Investigation of the gap effect on pursuit revealed that, while the gap effect was present in the older group, it seemed to be directionally asymmetrical. When the longer absolute latencies were taken into account, although the gap effect in the two groups was identical for leftward tasks, for rightward tasks it was reduced in the older group, although this did not reach statistical significance. The difference between the old and young groups was driven by some of the older subjects. At the longest gap duration employed (400 ms), while there was a clear gap effect for leftward tasks in these subjects, there was no reduction in latency, or increases in latency, for rightward tasks. This asymmetry was not related to chronological age within the older group. These results suggest an age-related alteration in SP initiation that is more complex than general slowing of information processing in ageing. They may be indicative of additional ageing effects specific to the oculomotor or closely related systems. 相似文献
28.
We describe a simple method for computer quantification of eye movement (EM) potentials during REM sleep. This method can be applied by investigators using either period-amplitude (PA) or Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) spectral EEG analysis without special hardware or computer programming. It provides good correlations with visual ratings of EM in baseline sleep and after administration of GABAergic hypnotics. We present baseline data for both PA and FFT measures for 16 normal subjects, studied for 5 consecutive nights. Both visually rated and computer-measured EM density (EMD) showed high night-to-night correlations across baseline and drug nights and the computer measures detected the EMD suppression that is produced by GABAergic drugs. Measurement of EM in addition to stage REM provides biologically significant information and application of this simple computer method, which does not require pattern recognition algorithms or special hardware, could provide reliable data that can be compared across laboratories. 相似文献
29.
Rats trained to reach for food pellets into a narrow tubular feeder consistently prefer to perform this stereotype instrumental movement with either the left or right forepaw. In 16 rats with established handedness electrodes were implanted into both lateral hypothalami. The animals were rewarded by intracranial self stimulation (ICSS, 300 msec, 50 Hz, 20-60 microA) for reaching into a modified feeder for a plastic ball operandum, the movement of which between the bottom and entrance of the feeder was monitored by mechanical contacts. The rats readily continued to reach when ICSS was delivered immediately after the photoelectrically detected reach or after the displacement of the operandum. Most rats learned in a single session to modify the movement when ICSS delivery was made contingent upon holding the operandum between the bottom and entrance of the feeder for 256 or 512 msec. The efficiency of reaching (ratio of successful reaches to all reaches) decreased with increasing holding time; only a few animals were able to master a 1024 msec delay. Reaching was supported by ICSS of either lateral hypothalamus. Whereas in 8 rats the strongly expressed forepaw preference was not changed by lateralized ICSS, in 8 latently ambidextrous animals stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus ipsilateral to the preferred forepaw increased reaching with the normally non-preferred forepaw from 15% to 60%. Stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus contralateral to the preferred forepaw did not change the preference. The preference shift was equally well expressed in simple and difficult versions of the task. It is concluded that lateralization of motivational influences can be reflected in the asymmetry of the neural mechanisms processing the lateralized sensory signals and/or elaborating the lateralized motor output. 相似文献
30.
There are a number of reports suggesting an association between profusion of eye movements and active dreaming. It has however been suggested that this relationship might only be evident in comparisons across the night and would not be evident in comparisons within one REM period. Data from 20 subjects taking placebo, amylobarbitone, and nitrazepam were used to test this. Dream reports were collected from REM awakenings and rated blind as visually active or passive. Eye movement profusion (number of 2 sec epochs) was assessed for each REM period. Correlation between dream content and eye movement was low but significant in comparisons including the whole night, and including data from drug, withdrawal, and placebo conditions. A significant correlation was not consistently obtained, however, when data from each REMP were considered separately. Correlations based on data from non-drug nights only were also small and could have been due to chance effects alone. The low correlations were not explicable solely by poor reliability of content ratings. It is concluded that the relationship between visually active dreaming and eye movement is slight, and may not hold when time of night is adequately controlled. 相似文献