首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   5354篇
  免费   488篇
  国内免费   72篇
耳鼻咽喉   35篇
儿科学   46篇
妇产科学   20篇
基础医学   1436篇
口腔科学   43篇
临床医学   226篇
内科学   625篇
皮肤病学   44篇
神经病学   967篇
特种医学   129篇
外国民族医学   1篇
外科学   176篇
综合类   235篇
预防医学   787篇
眼科学   348篇
药学   308篇
  1篇
中国医学   22篇
肿瘤学   465篇
  2024年   19篇
  2023年   121篇
  2022年   157篇
  2021年   224篇
  2020年   188篇
  2019年   200篇
  2018年   162篇
  2017年   168篇
  2016年   156篇
  2015年   186篇
  2014年   273篇
  2013年   312篇
  2012年   201篇
  2011年   276篇
  2010年   220篇
  2009年   246篇
  2008年   270篇
  2007年   240篇
  2006年   244篇
  2005年   178篇
  2004年   169篇
  2003年   169篇
  2002年   114篇
  2001年   108篇
  2000年   80篇
  1999年   80篇
  1998年   96篇
  1997年   85篇
  1996年   91篇
  1995年   74篇
  1994年   58篇
  1993年   70篇
  1992年   59篇
  1991年   47篇
  1990年   60篇
  1989年   55篇
  1988年   43篇
  1987年   39篇
  1986年   33篇
  1985年   77篇
  1984年   64篇
  1983年   51篇
  1982年   37篇
  1981年   28篇
  1980年   20篇
  1979年   22篇
  1978年   11篇
  1977年   13篇
  1974年   6篇
  1973年   5篇
排序方式: 共有5914条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
Objective: Effective material exchange between blood and tissue depends on the heterogeneity of microvascular flow. The objective was to address inconsistencies between intravital studies regarding this dependency. We tested the hypothesis that heterogeneity of red blood cell velocity (VRBC) in capillary beds varies with the strength of metabolic stimulus and with capillary bed geometry. Methods: We used videomicroscopy to measure VRBC in a bed of 10–24 capillaries at the surface of extensor digitorum longus (EDL) muscle in anesthetized rats. The coefficient of variation (CV = standard deviation/mean; an index of spatial heterogeneity) was computed in the same bed before and after (i) 1, 2, 4, or 8 Hz supramaximal muscle contraction or (ii) adenosine superfusion (10?7–10?3 M). Beds with or without arteriolar—venular capillary shunts were used. Results: Although control VRBC differed between beds (shunt: 232 μm/s; no shunt: 130 μm/s), the percentage increases in postcontraction VRBC did not (range: 111–326%). In both beds, control CV varied greatly (overall range: 28–117%) and 2–8 Hz muscle contractions reduced CV significantly by 25%. Similar results were obtained for adenosine. In confirmatory experiments using the rat cremaster muscle, contractions (4 Hz) and adenosine (10?4 M) also reduced CV. Based on all data, CV = 63–0.022 VRBC (r = 0.82, P < 0.001). Conclusions: The heterogeneity of VRBC decreased with metabolic stress, regardless of capillary bed geometry. We propose that both the large variability in control CV and the relatively shallow dependence of CV on velocity could be responsible for the present inconsistencies between intravital studies.  相似文献   
22.
A new non-invasive method to measure the optical properties of biological tissue is described. This method consists of illuminating the investigated sample with light which is spatially periodically modulated in intensity. The spatial modulation of the backscattered light and the diffuse reflectivity of the sample, both detected with an imaging technique, are used to deduce the absorption and reduced scattering coefficient from a table generated by Monte Carlo simulations. This principle has three major advantages: Firstly, it permits the immediate acquisition of the average values of the optical coefficients over a relatively large area (typ. 20 mm in diameter), thus avoiding the perturbations generated by small tissue heterogeneities; It also provides good flexibility for measuring the optical coefficients at various wavelengths and it does not require the use of a detector with a large dynamic range. The method was first validated on phantoms with known optical properties. Finally, we measured the optical properties of human skin at 400 nm, 500 nm, 633 nm and 700 nm in vivo.  相似文献   
23.
Sex differences in home range size and spatial ability are predictive of sex differences in the relative size of the hippocampus in rodents. Such differences in behavior and hippocampal volume are presumed to be, in part, the result of differences in perinatal exposure to hormones. We predicted from differences in the size of home ranges of male and female Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) in the wild that the hippocampus of male gerbils would be relatively larger than that of females. We examined the effect of prenatal hormonal influences on hippocampal size by comparing hippocampal volume of males and females from 2F and 2M intrauterine positions to that of randomly selected males and females. We found that, as predicted, randomly selected males had a significantly larger hippocampus, relative to telencephalon, than did randomly selected females. However, males and females from 2F and 2M intrauterine positions did not differ in relative hippocampal size. Possible explanations for the absence of a sex difference in hippocampal size in male and female gerbils from 2F and 2M intrauterine positions are discussed.  相似文献   
24.
The effects of hemicholinium-3 (HC-3) on spatial discriminaton learning were studied. Rats were equipped with indwelling cannulae in the right lateral ventricle and, following recovery, were trained on a two platform spatial discrimination task in a water maze. In this task a visible escape platform remains in a fixed position in the pool during a single training session, whilst the location of an identical float (which affords no escape) is randomly varied. For each session the location of the fixed escape platform was changed and the rats were retrained to criterion following pretreatment either with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) or HC-3 (2.5, 5.0 g/rat/ICV) 1 h before training. Each rat received every treatment according to a latin square design. The results showed that spatial learning was dose dependently impaired by HC-3, choice accuracy being reduced to chance levels by the higher dose. There was no evidence of motoric difficulty, as choice latencies were not significantly increased. Experiments were then conducted to test for reversal of the deficit using a range of psychotropic drugs. Rats were treated with CSF or HC-3 (5 g/rat ICV) 60 min prior to testing and test drugs were injected 15 min before testing. Some doses of physostigmine (46–460 g/kg/SC) and tetrahydroaminoacridine (THA) (2.2–10 mg/kg/SC) reversed the spatial learning deficit. The muscarinic agonists arecoline (0.046–1 mg/kg/SC), aceclidine (1–10 mg/kg/SC), oxotremorine (30–100 g/kg/SC) and RS-86 (0.46, 1.0 g/kg/SC) were also effective. Pilocarpine (0.22–2.2 mg/kg/SC) showed marginal activity and isoarecoline (4.6–10 mg/kg/SC) was inactive. Nicotine (0.32, 1, 3.2 mg/kg/SC) and piracetam (10, 30, 100 mg/kg IP) were also inactive. The 2 agonist, clonidine (46, 100 g/kg SC) and the antagonist idazoxan (32, 100 g/kg SC) were also inactive. Learning deficits were not reversed by haloperidol (20, 60 g/kg), amphetamine (0.1, 0.46 mg/kg), the selective 5-HT1A agonist 8-OH-DPAT (30, 100 g/kg) or by the benzodiazapine antagonist ZK 93426 (1, 3.2, 10 mg/kg). The results show that forebrain Ach depletion by HC-3 impairs spatial discrimination learning and these deficits are reversed by cholinesterase inhibitors and some muscarinic receptor agonists. Some degree of pharmacological selectivity is indicated by the failure of a range of other drugs to reverse the impairments.  相似文献   
25.
The changes of spatial EEG synchronisation during brisk and slow voluntary self-paced movements of the right and left index finger were analysed in 12 right-handed and 11 left-handed subjects. EEG was recorded from the left and right sensorimotor area using 24 closely spaced electrodes. A novel measure of spatial EEG synchronisation, -complexity, was computed separately for the left and right sensorimotor area in 64 overlapping one-second epochs representing 4.5 s of the pre-movement and 3.5 s of the post-movement period. -complexity was higher, hence spatial synchronisation was lower, in slow than in brisk movements, especially in the right-handed. A sustained increase of -complexity was observed during execution of a slow movement. A decrease of -complexity which was often associated with a brief burst of spatially synchronised 10-Hz oscillations occurred at the onset of extensor muscle contraction. We suggest that increased spatial EEG synchronisation at movement onset may prevent spillover of excitation from the sensorimotor hand area to other cortical regions. During movement, the cortical neuronal assemblies subserve distinct, specialised functions manifesting in increased -complexity.  相似文献   
26.
A technique that allowsin vivo measurements of the specific impedance of brain structures is analysed and implemented. This technique uses the 4-electrode method. The system includes an optically isolated current source and an interchangeable electrode module. Tests on the measuring system, including the electrodes and calibration procedures, are presented. Experiments in media of known resistivity and in cat brains define the present limits of the spatial resolution.  相似文献   
27.
Manipulation of objects around the head requires an accurate and stable internal representation of their locations in space, also during movements such as that of the eye or head. For far space, the representation of visual stimuli for goal-directed arm movements relies on retinal updating, if eye movements are involved. Recent neurophysiological studies led us to infer that a transformation of visual space from retinocentric to a head-centric representation may be involved for visual objects in close proximity to the head. The first aim of this study was to investigate if there is indeed such a representation for remembered visual targets of goal-directed arm movements. Participants had to point toward an initially foveated central target after an intervening saccade. Participants made errors that reflect a bias in the visuomotor transformation that depends on eye displacement rather than any head-centred variable. The second issue addressed was if pointing toward the centre of a wide-field expanding motion pattern involves a retinal updating mechanism or a transformation to a head-centric map and if that process is distance dependent. The same pattern of pointing errors in relation to gaze displacement was found independent of depth. We conclude that for goal-directed arm movements, representation of the remembered visual targets is updated in a retinal frame, a mechanism that is actively used regardless of target distance, stimulus characteristics or the requirements of the task.  相似文献   
28.
Summary Age-related cognitive impairments were studied in rats kept in semi-enriched conditions during their whole life, and tested during ontogeny and adult life in various classical spatial tasks. In addition, the effect of intrahippocampal grafts of fetal septal-diagonal band tissue, rich in cholinergic neurons, was studied in some of these subjects. The rats received bilateral cell suspensions when aged 23–24 months. Starting 4 weeks after grafting, they were trained during 5 weeks in an 8-arm maze made of connected plexiglass tunnels. No age-related impairment was detected during the first eight trials, when the maze shape was that of a classical radial maze in which the rats had already been trained when young. The older rats were impaired when the task was made more difficult by rendering two arms parallel to each other. They developed an important neglect of one of the parallel tunnels resulting in a high amount of errors before completion of the task. In addition, the old rats developed a systematic response pattern of visits to adjacent arms in a sequence, which was not observed in the younger subjects. None of these behaviours were observed in the old rats with a septal transplant. Sixteen weeks after grafting, another experiment was conducted in a homing hole board task. Rats were allowed to escape from a large circular arena through one hole out of many, and to reach home via a flexible tube under the table. The escape hole was at a fixed position according to distant room cues, and olfactory cues were made irrelevant by rotating the table between the trials. An additional cue was placed on the escape position. No age-related difference in escape was observed during training. During a probe trial with no hole connected and no proximal cue present, the old untreated rats were less clearly focussed on the training sector than were either the younger or the grafted old subjects. Taken together, these experiments indicate that enriched housing conditions and spatial training during adult life do not protect against all age-related deterioration in spatial ability. However, it might be that the considerable improvement observed in the grafted subjects results from an interaction between the graft treatment and the housing conditions.  相似文献   
29.
Male Sprague-Dawley rats with combined lesions to the dorsal and lateral aspects of the entorhinal cortex in one hemisphere and of the contralateral dorsolateral hippocampal formation were compared with both operated and unoperated controls on three different behaviors, monitored across a 53 day postoperative period. The rats with the combined entorhinal cortex-hippocampal lesions (EH) showed transient hyperactivity in the open field, transient reduction in spontaneous alternation levels in an unbaited T-maze and persistent deficits in learning spatial maze problems. The results of the present experiment are discussed in comparison with those from experiments on rats with bilateral hippocampal lesions and those from experiments on rats with bilateral entorhinal cortex lesions. Although some similarities among these findings suggest that these two brain regions probably function in a coordinated fashion with respect to these behaviors, differences in the various syndromes are also discussed.  相似文献   
30.
Research on visual attention has demonstrated that covert attention can be focused on particular locations within one hemifield, but that a specific "meridian" cost may also be found for shifting attention between hemifields. These issues have received less consideration for audition, even though reliable behavioral measures for the effects of spatial attention on hearing are now available. We examined the spatial distribution of covert attention in an auditory task following spatially non-predictive peripheral auditory cues (which should induce exogenous attention shifts), or following symbolic central cues that predicted the likely location for the auditory target (to induce endogenous attention shifts). In both cases, we found that attention can be focused not only on one hemifield versus another, but also within one hemifield in an auditory task. However, there was no unequivocal evidence for a meridian effect in audition. Electronic Publication  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号