首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   6906篇
  免费   927篇
  国内免费   282篇
耳鼻咽喉   45篇
儿科学   84篇
妇产科学   48篇
基础医学   477篇
口腔科学   135篇
临床医学   1156篇
内科学   576篇
皮肤病学   36篇
神经病学   682篇
特种医学   131篇
外科学   666篇
综合类   950篇
一般理论   5篇
预防医学   1109篇
眼科学   65篇
药学   723篇
  6篇
中国医学   608篇
肿瘤学   613篇
  2024年   52篇
  2023年   205篇
  2022年   255篇
  2021年   313篇
  2020年   365篇
  2019年   331篇
  2018年   347篇
  2017年   354篇
  2016年   345篇
  2015年   323篇
  2014年   501篇
  2013年   626篇
  2012年   436篇
  2011年   518篇
  2010年   371篇
  2009年   304篇
  2008年   353篇
  2007年   350篇
  2006年   297篇
  2005年   274篇
  2004年   224篇
  2003年   252篇
  2002年   125篇
  2001年   105篇
  2000年   72篇
  1999年   43篇
  1998年   42篇
  1997年   50篇
  1996年   37篇
  1995年   48篇
  1994年   27篇
  1993年   25篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   20篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   14篇
  1988年   13篇
  1987年   13篇
  1986年   11篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   2篇
  1975年   1篇
  1970年   1篇
排序方式: 共有8115条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Two compatible conversion factors for converting diameter measurements taken at different breast heights were derived for Pinus radiata using taper data from more than 3000 trees. The two breast heights used for conversion were 1.3 and 1.4 m above ground, as defined in Australia and New Zealand, two major radiata-growing countries in the world. The conversion factors were estimated through three alternative statistical methods including simple least squares regression, seemingly unrelated regression and errors-in-variables models. The three sets of estimates were almost identical and had similar conversion accuracy, although the second method was slightly better. The conversion factors were more accurate than overbark taper equations used for the same purpose. The factor was 0.9916 for converting diameter measured at 1.3 to that at 1.4 m above ground, and the inverse of this value, 1.0084, was for the vice versa. When calculating tree and stand volume and biomass using equations with diameter at a different breast height as a predictor to that of the input data, the bias, either over or under estimation, could be between 1.67% and 2.00% without conversion. These conversion factors will facilitate the sharing of data among radiata growing countries with different definitions of breast height, but more importantly it will help correct the bias in stand volume and biomass estimation caused by the seemingly negligible difference in breast height when software for forest resource management and decision support developed in one country is applied in another. Such bias when accumulated over a large management area may not be financially insignificant for an astute forest management agency.  相似文献   
102.
103.
104.
105.
106.
The gene encoding the small subunit rRNA serves as a prominent tool for the phylogenetic analysis and classification of Bacteria and Archaea owing to its high degree of conservation and its fundamental function in living organisms. Here we show that the 16S rRNA genes of not-yet-cultivated large sulfur bacteria, among them the largest known bacterium Thiomargarita namibiensis, regularly contain numerous self-splicing introns of variable length. The 16S rRNA genes can thus be enlarged to up to 3.5 kb. Remarkably, introns have never been identified in bacterial 16S rRNA genes before, although they are the most frequently sequenced genes today. This may be caused in part by a bias during the PCR amplification step that discriminates against longer homologs, as we show experimentally. Such length heterogeneity of 16S rRNA genes has so far never been considered when constructing 16S rRNA-based clone libraries, even though an elongation of rRNA genes due to intervening sequences has been reported previously. The detection of elongated 16S rRNA genes has profound implications for common methods in molecular ecology and may cause systematic biases in several techniques. In this study, catalyzed reporter deposition-fluorescence in situ hybridization on both ribosomes and rRNA precursor molecules as well as in vitro splicing experiments were performed and confirmed self-splicing of the introns. Accordingly, the introns do not inhibit the formation of functional ribosomes.  相似文献   
107.
Recent advances in social neuroscience research have unveiled the neurophysiological correlates of race and intergroup processing. However, little is known about the neural mechanisms underlying intergroup empathy. Combining event‐related fMRI with measurements of pupil dilation as an index of autonomic reactivity, we explored how race and group membership affect empathy‐related responses. White and Black subjects were presented with video clips depicting white, black, and unfamiliar violet‐skinned hands being either painfully penetrated by a syringe or being touched by a Q‐tip. Both hemodynamic activity within areas known to be involved in the processing of first and third‐person emotional experiences of pain, i.e., bilateral anterior insula, and autonomic reactivity were greater for the pain experienced by own‐race compared to that of other‐race and violet models. Interestingly, greater implicit racial bias predicted increased activity within the left anterior insula during the observation of own‐race pain relative to other‐race pain. Our findings highlight the close link between group‐based segregation and empathic processing. Moreover, they demonstrate the relative influence of culturally acquired implicit attitudes and perceived similarity/familiarity with the target in shaping emotional responses to others' physical pain. Hum Brain Mapp 34:3168–3181, 2013. © 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.  相似文献   
108.
Composition is an important topic in visual art. The literature suggests a bias for objects on the right side (Levy, 1976) and two additional biases with respect to positioning of objects within a rectangular frame: a Centre bias and an Inward bias (Palmer, Gardner, & Wickens, 2008). We analysed images of animals from three datasets of works of art: two datasets were from artists well known for their portraits of animals (Bewick, Stubbs) and the third was a medieval bestiary. There was no overall displacement of the subject to the right or to the left of the picture. However, we found a bias consisting of more space in front compared to behind the animal, consistent with Palmer at al.'s findings and with their definition of an Inward bias. Because our animals never face towards the centre we use the term Anterior bias. In addition, we found a modulation of this bias on the basis of the facing direction of the animal, consisting of a stronger Anterior bias for left-facing animals. This asymmetry may originate from a combination of an Anterior bias and a Right bias. Finally, with respect to size we found that the size of the animals predicted the proportion of the picture occupied, an effect known as “canonical size”.  相似文献   
109.
Age-related changes in visual spatial biases in children, young adults, and older adults were studied with unilateral and bilateral stimulus conditions in fast-paced linguistic and non-linguistic attention tasks. Only rightward spatial biases were observed. The incidence of the biases changed as a function of age: in childhood and in old age the rightward spatial biases were more common than in young adulthood. The present rightward spatial biases were similar to those observed in the corresponding auditory spatial linguistic and non-linguistic attention tests (Takio, Koivisto, Laukka, & Hämäläinen, 2011) and in the dichotic listening forced-attention task (Takio et al., 2009). We suggest that the multimodal rightward spatial bias observed under intensive attentional load is related to a right hemispace preference and modulated by age-dependent changes in executive functions.  相似文献   
110.
Abstract

Objectives. People with eating disorders (EDs) have difficulties with social functioning. One explanatory mechanism is a problem with over-sensitivity to rejection and/or low sensitivity to social reward. The aim of this study is to investigate attentional bias to facial stimuli in people with a lifetime diagnosis of EDs and healthy controls (HCs) and to test whether these attentional biases are linked to adverse early experiences. Methods. Forty-six participants with a current diagnosis of EDs (29 with anorexia nervosa (AN) and 17 with bulimia nervosa (BN)), 22 participants recovered from an eating disorder (13 with past AN and nine with past BN) and 50 HCs completed a dot-probe task with faces expressing rejection and acceptance. Participants reported on parental style and adverse early experiences. Results. People with a lifetime diagnosis of EDs show an attentional bias to rejecting faces and a difficulty disengaging attention from these stimuli. Also, they had a sustained attentional avoidance of accepting faces. HCs demonstrated the opposite attentional pattern. The attentional bias to rejection was correlated with adverse childhood experiences. Conclusions. People with an EDs show vigilance to rejection and avoidance of social reward. This may contribute to the causation or maintenance of the illness.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号