首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1630篇
  免费   216篇
  国内免费   38篇
耳鼻咽喉   4篇
儿科学   11篇
妇产科学   10篇
基础医学   203篇
口腔科学   19篇
临床医学   118篇
内科学   241篇
皮肤病学   6篇
神经病学   104篇
特种医学   78篇
外科学   93篇
综合类   186篇
预防医学   494篇
眼科学   39篇
药学   149篇
  2篇
中国医学   74篇
肿瘤学   53篇
  2024年   8篇
  2023年   39篇
  2022年   70篇
  2021年   86篇
  2020年   72篇
  2019年   68篇
  2018年   68篇
  2017年   63篇
  2016年   76篇
  2015年   55篇
  2014年   108篇
  2013年   108篇
  2012年   89篇
  2011年   95篇
  2010年   82篇
  2009年   71篇
  2008年   92篇
  2007年   77篇
  2006年   60篇
  2005年   64篇
  2004年   60篇
  2003年   57篇
  2002年   50篇
  2001年   29篇
  2000年   29篇
  1999年   25篇
  1998年   28篇
  1997年   21篇
  1996年   13篇
  1995年   15篇
  1994年   12篇
  1993年   13篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   9篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   7篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   6篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1971年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1884条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
益气通腑汤治疗女性直肠前突随机平行对照研究   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
[目的]观察益气通腑汤治疗女性直肠前突疗效。[方法]使用随机平行对照方法,将123例随机分为两组,对照组60例麻仁丸口服,治疗组60例益气通腑汤化裁。3个月为1疗程,治疗1疗程(90d)判定疗效。[结果]治疗组临床治愈6例,有效40例,无效14例,总有效率76.67%。对照组临床治愈1例,有效21例,无效38例,总有效率36.67%。治疗组疗效优于对照组(P<0.01)。[结论]中医药以整体观念为原则,以调理脏腑机能为根本,弥补了西医治疗的不足,值得临床推广。  相似文献   
22.
目的明确同一时间段先后分离自同一病房2例患者的两株黏质沙雷菌是否属于同源菌。方法应用随机扩增DNA多态性(RAPD)技术对同一时间段先后自同一病房2例患者痰液标本中分离得的2株黏质沙雷菌进行RAPD基因多态性分析。结果2株黏质沙雷菌扩增出2条不同的电泳带谱。结论2株黏质沙雷菌为不同的RAPD基因型,非同源菌。  相似文献   
23.
Conformist and anticonformist transmission of dichotomous cultural traits (i.e., traits with two variants) have been studied both experimentally, in many species, and theoretically, with mathematical models. Signatures of types of conformity to polychotomous traits (with more than two variants; e.g., baby names and syllables in bird song) have been inferred from population-level data, but there are few models that include individual-level biases among more than two discrete variants. We generalize the standard dichotomous trait conformity model by Boyd and Richerson to incorporate n3 role models and m2 variants. Our analysis shows that in the case of n=3 role models, under anticonformity, the central polymorphic equilibrium p*=(1m,,1m) is globally stable, whereas under conformity, if initially the frequencies of variants are all equal to the maximum variant frequency in the population, there is global convergence to an equilibrium in which the frequencies of these variants are all 1 and all other variants are absent. With a general number n of role models, the same result holds with conformity, whereas under anticonformity, global convergence is not guaranteed, and there may be stable frequency cycles or chaos. If both conformity and anticonformity occur for different configurations of variants among the n role models, a variety of novel polymorphic equilibria may exist and be stable. Future empirical studies may use this formulation to directly quantify an individual’s level of (anti)conformist bias to a polychotomous trait.

Cultural traits, such as tools, ideas, components of speech, or behaviors (1), can have different types, known as variants. Dichotomous cultural traits have two variants, such as pro vs. con opinions or the presence vs. absence of behaviors. Polychotomous cultural traits have three or more variants; examples include baby names, fashions, and components of birdsong. The transmission of dichotomous and polychotomous traits may be nonrandom, or biased, and two biases that have been widely studied are conformity and anticonformity. Conformity entails that a more common cultural variant is adopted at a rate greater than its population frequency, while under anticonformity, the adoption rate is less than its frequency (2).Many empirical studies of conformity and anticonformity have explored dichotomous cultural traits. For example, in ref. 3, children were presented with two arrays with dots and asked which had more dots, and in ref. 4, individuals were shown two shapes rotated at different angles and asked whether they were the same or different shapes. Participants conformed to the answers of others in the mental rotation task (4), and in ref. 3, young children anticonformed to others’ choices, whereas older children conformed. In nonhuman animals, conformity to a dichotomous trait was exhibited by sticklebacks choosing between feeders (5), great tits deciding whether to push a puzzle box slider to the left or right (6, 7), and female fruit flies copulating with pink or green males (8).In ref. 9, people were shown two lines (a dichotomous trait) or more than two lines, with a maximum of six (a polychotomous trait), and were asked which line was longest. The authors found that the strength of conformity increased as the number of lines increased. They explained (ref. 9, p. 11):
consider a world with only 2 [variants]—black and white shirts. The presence of black shirts at anything above 50% suggests that people are selecting black shirts above chance. However, in a world with four [variants]—black, white, green, and red shirts—black shirts need only be present above 25% to suggest selection above chanceall current models and experiments may have been underestimating the strength of the conformist bias, because there are often more than 2 [variants] in the real world.”
Thus, modeling (anti)conformity to a polychotomous trait may help empiricists adequately assess levels of conformity rather than relying on dichotomous trait models. The authors also found that the strength of conformity and the reliance on social learning increased with group size for the dichotomous but not for the polychotomous trait, perhaps because as the number of variants increases, “larger groups are required for group size to have a discernible effect” (ref. 9, p. 19).There is some evidence for (anti)conformity to real-world polychotomous traits that have hundreds to thousands of variants. As directly measuring individual biases among a large number of variants can be difficult, many studies have inferred cultural transmission biases from signatures in population-level data. For example, in ref. 10, the turnover in baby names—i.e., the number of new names that enter the list of top names—was plotted as a function of top list size (top 5 names, top 10 names, etc.). A concave plot suggested anticonformity, while a convex plot suggested conformity or content-biased transmission (where the latter is a bias for a particular variant over another). Recent distributions of common baby names appeared to be under anticonformist bias, whereas in earlier decades, common male baby names appeared to be under conformist or content-biased transmission, although these could not be distinguished (10).Another population-level signature of conformity is the sigmoidal curve that can appear when the probability of adopting a variant is plotted against that variant’s population frequency (2). Acerbi et al. (11) contended that a sigmoidal curve could also be observed under content bias and demonstrator bias (the latter occurs if variants are copied randomly from a particular subset of individuals in the population) and questioned whether there was “strong support for the existence of conformist transmission at all” (ref. 11, p. 6). However, Smaldino et al. (12) reran the simulations in ref. 11 with different and possibly more realistic assumptions (12) and found that the sigmoidal curve no longer appeared under content bias, while with demonstrator bias only a slightly sigmoidal curve appeared under some conditions. They concluded that sigmoidal curves are much more likely to result from conformist transmission than content-biased or demonstrator-biased transmission. When models of conformity, content bias, and demonstrator bias were each fitted to empirical data on the distributions of syllables in swamp sparrow song (13), the distributions were most consistent with conformist bias.Table 1.n=3 role models and m=3 variants
Offspring probabilities P(Ai|x)
Role model state, x A 1 A 2 A 3 P(x)
(3, 0, 0)100 p13
(0, 3, 0)010 p23
(0, 0, 3)001 p33
(2, 1, 0) 23+D3 13D3 0 3p12p2
(2, 0, 1) 23+D3 0 13D3 3p12p3
(1, 2, 0) 13D3 23+D3 0 3p1p22
(1, 0, 2) 13D3 0 23+D3 3p1p32
(0, 2, 1)0 23+D3 13D3 3p22p3
(0, 1, 2)0 13D3 23+D3 3p2p32
(1,1,1) 13 13 13 6p1p2p3
Open in a separate windowDespite the prevalence of polychotomous traits, the majority of the many theoretical studies of conformity involve dichotomous traits. In refs. 1 and 2, two models of frequency-dependent transmission of a dichotomous cultural trait (with variants A and B, say) were proposed, both of which could incorporate conformity or anticonformity. In ref. 2, individuals randomly sample n role models from the previous generation, and there are different conformity coefficients for different samples of role models (e.g., individuals might conform more strongly if 60% of role models share a variant than if 90% do, or vice versa). Many subsequent theoretical studies involving conformity to dichotomous traits have taken n to be three (1421), in which case there is a single conformity coefficient, or have adopted another formulation that also includes a single conformity coefficient (18, 22, 23). Some models have added parameters for content bias (15, 24), payoff-dependent bias (16), individual learning (14, 1719, 21), or various forms of group selection (20, 22). In ref. 25, Boyd and Richerson’s (2) conformity models for a dichotomous trait with n > 3 role models and more than one conformity coefficient were reexamined and shown to exhibit dynamics that were not observed with n=3 role models, including stable asymmetric polymorphic equilibria, exact cycles in variant frequencies (if n5), and chaos (if n10). In ref. 26, further generalizations allowed conformity coefficients to vary stochastically over time.Fewer models of conformity have incorporated a polychotomous trait. Henrich (15) argues that a polychotomous trait can be approximated by a dichotomous trait in some contexts (ref. 15, p. 994): “In the typical diffusion of an innovation, tracking only two traits is sufficient to capture the essential process: Trait 1 represents the presence of the novel trait (the “innovation”), and Trait 2 indicates the absence of the trait. If we are, for example, studying the spread of a new nitrogen fertilizer, an individual possesses Trait 1 if he or she uses the fertilizer and possesses Trait 2 if he or she does not use the fertilizer.”However, consider the following example. Prior to the occurrence of the novel type, suppose there are two types of fertilizer, denoted by A and B, each frequency at 12. A new type, C, appears in the population at a small frequency, denoted by ε. Considering only two variants, namely, “C/not C” as in ref. 15, would lead one to conclude that under weak anticonformity, the frequency of C would be 12 at equilibrium (2, 25). However, this is incorrect because with three variants, a frequency 12 of one variant would render it common and thus disfavored under anticonformity; instead, a frequency 13 of all variants would be the intuitive equilibrium in this case. If instead there were conformist transmission when C was introduced at a low frequency ε, then in the dichotomous C/not C framework, the type that is not C would be expected to reach fixation (2, 25). However, a polychotomous conformity model is more informative because it can tell us which of A or B (or neither) will fix in the population. When C first appears, let the frequency of A become 12η and the frequency of B become 12δ, where η and δ are small. If η<δ, then A will fix; if η>δ, then B will fix; and if η=δ, both A and B will be favored over C, but neither A nor B will be favored over the other, so the frequencies of A and B will be 0.5 at equilibrium (shown in Result 5). Thus, a polychotomous trait model is more appropriate than a dichotomous trait model in such situations.Among the few theoretical studies of conformity with polychotomous traits, refs. 18, 23, 27 allowed social and individual learners in variable environments to acquire one of many variants, with either one (18, 23) or more (27) variants having a fitness advantage in a given environmental state. In these studies, social learners could be conformist, with a single conformity coefficient. A continuous trait was explored in refs. 2830, where cultural transmission was a function of a group’s mean trait value (among other variables). In refs. 29 and 30, “conformists” preferentially adopted trait values equal to or near this mean trait value. This definition of conformity differs from the definition for a discrete cultural trait: in the latter case, conformists preferentially adopt the most common variant (i.e., the modal variant).The present study explores conformist and anticonformist transmission of a discrete and selectively neutral polychotomous cultural trait (e.g., pottery motifs, dog breeds, or baby names) in a stable environment. Rather than incorporating a single conformity coefficient, we allow (anti)conformity to vary flexibly for different samples of role models provided n4 role models are observed. For example, if the name “Mary” is slightly more popular than other names, an individual might conform and name a child Mary, but it would be unrealistic to expect the same level of conformity if, say, 80% of all people that the individual observed were named Mary. Our previous studies (25, 26) focused on dynamical properties of the commonly studied model (2) of a dichotomous trait, which could include multiple conformity coefficients if n5 role models were sampled. Here we generalize ref. 25 to incorporate an arbitrary number m of cultural variants with n role models. If n is taken to be 3, as is often the case in models of conformity with dichotomous traits, then there is a single conformity coefficient, and there is global convergence of the population to one of two equilibria: equal frequencies of all variants initially present in the population, under anticonformity, or equal frequencies of the variant(s) that were initially present at the maximum frequency in the population, and zero frequency for all other variants, under conformity. With a general number n of role models and multiple conformity coefficients, conformity entails global convergence to the same equilibrium as with n = 3. However, with anticonformity and n role models, it is possible that no equilibrium is reached, and stable frequency cycles, or chaos, may arise. If individuals can conform or anticonform depending on the configuration of sampled role models, the dynamics can be much more complex. For example, a variety of novel asymmetric polymorphic equilibria may exist and be stable.  相似文献   
24.
As the core component of satellite navigation, the hydrogen maser needs a high vacuum environment to maintain the stability of the frequency signal. The getter pump, composed of various non-evaporable getters, plays an important role in maintaining the high vacuum. In this paper, the Zr100-xCux (x = 0, 2, 4, 6)/Zr56.97V35.85Cr7.18 getter was studied and the contradiction between sorption performance and mechanical properties was solved. The Zr-V-Cr getter, a better candidate for getter pump, exists for problems which will destroy the high vacuum and affect the service life of the hydrogen maser. To solve the problem of dropping powder from Zr-V-Cr getter, Zr-Cu films were coated on the surface of Zr-V-Cr matrix to obtain the pore gradient structure. After vacuum sintering, the interface showed gradient structure and network change in pore structure from Zr-Cu film to Zr-V-Cr matrix. These characteristic structures made Zr-V-Cr getter have good absorption properties, which is better than a similar product of SAES company and mechanical properties. Because the Zr-Cu film on Zr-V-Cr matrix effectively prevented dropping powders from the matrix, (Zr-Cu)/(Zr-V-Cr) getter solved the problem of dropping powder. The self-developed new getter with pore gradient structure is of great significance for maintaining the high vacuum of hydrogen maser in the future.  相似文献   
25.
叶杰胜 《中国药业》2011,20(19):27-28
目的运用近红外漫反射光谱分析技术建立别嘌醇片的一致性检验模型。方法采用一阶导数法在4 240~5 000 cm-1,5 600~6 900 cm-1和7 500~8 700 cm-1波数范围内对各厂家的别嘌醇片光谱进行预处理,并建立一致性检验模型。结果针对多厂家和单一厂家建立的一致性检验模型在CI=4时,均能达到各自的快速分析目标。结论近红外漫反射光谱法是一种很实用的分析技术,针对别嘌醇片建立的两种一致性检验模型具有快速、可靠的特点,适用于药品检测车对别嘌醇片的快速筛查。  相似文献   
26.
霍山石斛种内遗传稳定性的RAPD初探   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
目的 研究霍山石斛荚果内遗传稳定性及荚果间的差异大小。方法 利用从20个随机引物筛选出来的3个稳定性较好的10碱基引物对来自5个不同荚果的霍山石斛种群进行RAPD检测。结果 5个荚果内遗传相似系数较大,在92.5926%-100.00%,其中H1、H9、H10、H14存在一定程度的变异,H23是一个较为稳定的群体;5个荚果间H1、H9、H10、H23的遗传距离较小,H14与其他4个荚果的遗传距离则较大。结论 建立霍山石斛稳定的无性快繁体系是切实可行的,同时说明霍山石斛种内存在一定的变异,H14与其他荚果的差异最大。  相似文献   
27.
In this paper, the S32101 duplex stainless steel welded joints were produced by a K-TIG welding system. The weld geometry parameters under different welding speeds were analyzed by combining the morphological characteristics of the keyhole. The microstructure and impact toughness of the base metal and weld metal zone under different welding speeds were studied. The experiment results show that the welding speed has quite an effect on the geometry profile of the weld. In addition, the characteristic parameters of the keyhole can effectively predict the geometry profile of the weld. The test results prove that the microstructure, Σ3 coincidence site lattice grain boundary, and phase boundary of ferrite and austenite have an effect on the impact property of the weld metal zone. When the proportion of the austenite, Σ3 coincidence site lattice grain boundary and random phase boundary increased, the impact property of the weld metal zone also increased.  相似文献   
28.
耿仲乐  王静  张瑞  王鹏超 《中国药事》2012,26(8):898-901
目的 在不破坏药品包装的情况下,利用近红外光谱快速鉴别健胃消食片(江中药业股份有限公司).方法 在12000~4000 cm-1波段范围内,对同一厂家的健胃消食片进行全谱扫描,通过图谱比对,建立一致性检验模型.结果 正品健胃消食片与伪品健胃消食片的近红外光谱在5500~9000 cm-1波长范围内存在较大差别,通过建立该品种的一致性近红外模型,可对健胃消食片进行初筛.结论 该方法快速简便、准确有效,为基层打假提供有效方法.  相似文献   
29.
为适应医学教育的发展形势,积极与"全球医学教育最低标准"相接轨,安徽医科大学对临床医学专业课程设置进行了新的设计与整合,以培养具有多方面潜质、多方面能力和良好职业道德与人际沟通素质的人才为价值取向,按照"一个标准、一个原则、两个整合、两个环节、九个模块"的思路制定了新培养方案,为深化教学改革,培养创新医学专门人才明确了方向。  相似文献   
30.
People appear to vary in their susceptibility to lifestyle risk factors for cardiometabolic disease; determining a priori who is most sensitive may help optimize the timing, design, and delivery of preventative interventions. We aimed to ascertain a person’s degree of resilience or sensitivity to adverse lifestyle exposures and determine whether these classifications help predict cardiometabolic disease later in life; we pooled data from two population-based Swedish prospective cohort studies (n = 53,507), and we contrasted an individual’s cardiometabolic biomarker profile with the profile predicted for them given their lifestyle exposure characteristics using a quantile random forest approach. People who were classed as ‘sensitive’ to hypertension- and dyslipidemia-related lifestyle exposures were at higher risk of developing cardiovascular disease (CVD, hazards ratio 1.6 (95% CI: 1.3, 1.91)), compared with the general population. No differences were observed for type 2 diabetes (T2D) risk. Here, we report a novel approach to identify individuals who are especially sensitive to adverse lifestyle exposures and who are at higher risk of subsequent cardiovascular events. Early preventive interventions may be needed in this subgroup.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号