首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   18672篇
  免费   1936篇
  国内免费   133篇
耳鼻咽喉   88篇
儿科学   709篇
妇产科学   265篇
基础医学   1768篇
口腔科学   375篇
临床医学   3553篇
内科学   1757篇
皮肤病学   53篇
神经病学   3123篇
特种医学   149篇
外科学   1030篇
综合类   1426篇
一般理论   24篇
预防医学   4763篇
眼科学   51篇
药学   892篇
  57篇
中国医学   140篇
肿瘤学   518篇
  2024年   56篇
  2023年   498篇
  2022年   541篇
  2021年   971篇
  2020年   1014篇
  2019年   1061篇
  2018年   942篇
  2017年   1010篇
  2016年   878篇
  2015年   873篇
  2014年   1230篇
  2013年   2306篇
  2012年   979篇
  2011年   973篇
  2010年   675篇
  2009年   735篇
  2008年   762篇
  2007年   705篇
  2006年   638篇
  2005年   511篇
  2004年   433篇
  2003年   406篇
  2002年   337篇
  2001年   264篇
  2000年   231篇
  1999年   188篇
  1998年   188篇
  1997年   161篇
  1996年   119篇
  1995年   116篇
  1994年   90篇
  1993年   107篇
  1992年   79篇
  1991年   78篇
  1990年   65篇
  1989年   69篇
  1988年   54篇
  1987年   45篇
  1986年   64篇
  1985年   50篇
  1984年   33篇
  1983年   31篇
  1982年   32篇
  1981年   18篇
  1980年   22篇
  1979年   22篇
  1978年   16篇
  1977年   17篇
  1976年   13篇
  1975年   12篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
951.
ObjectivesThree sets of analyses are performed in our study. First, following Erikson (1959), we hypothesized that the relationship between age and meaning becomes progressively stronger at successively older ages (i.e., the relationship is nonlinear). Second, following Carstensen (1992), we predicted that the relationship between age and social support (received support and satisfaction with support) becomes progressively stronger at successively older ages (i.e., these relationships are nonlinear, as well). Third, we proposed that the nonlinear relationship between age and meaning is mediated by the nonlinear social support constructs (i.e., received support and satisfaction with support).MethodsOur data were obtained from online interviews with a nationwide sample of adults of all ages (N = 2, 245).ResultsOur findings suggest there is a nonlinear relationship between age and meaning in life as well as a nonlinear relationship between age and each social support measure. Our data also indicate that the nonlinear effects of the social support measures mediate the nonlinear relationship between age and meaning in life.DiscussionFinding a sense of meaning may be facilitated by the supportive social networks that older people maintain.  相似文献   
952.
Human immunodeficiency virus/acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (HIV/AIDS) has a profound impact not only on the infected individuals, but also on their families. Children of the HIV-infected parents are particularly affected. The present study examined the relationship between social support, resilience, posttraumatic growth (PTG), hopelessness, and depression among 195 children of HIV-infected parents in mainland China. Results showed that 35.4% of the sample scored above the cutoff of the Children's Depression Inventory. Results from structural equation modeling reported that social support had a significant positive relationship with resilience and PTG. Higher levels of resilience and PTG were associated with lower level of hopelessness which in turn, was associated with lower level of depression. The overall model achieved satisfactory fit. Interventions are needed to improve social support of the children affected by HIV so as to improve their mental health.  相似文献   
953.
Willis Omondi Odek 《AIDS care》2014,26(8):1042-1049
People living with human immunodeficiency virus (PLHIV) in developing countries can live longer due to improved treatment access, and a deeper understanding of determinants of their quality of life is critical. This study assessed the link between social capital, operationally defined in terms of social networks (group-based and personal social networks) and access to network resources (access to material and non-material resources and social support) and health-related quality of life (HRQoL) among 554 (55% female) adults on HIV treatment through South Africa's public health system. Female study participants were involved with more group-based social networks but had fewer personal social networks in comparison to males. Access to network resources was higher among females and those from larger households but lower among older study participants. Experience of social support significantly increased with household economic status and duration at current residence. Social capital indicators were unrelated to HIV disease status indicators, including duration since diagnosis, CD4 count and viral load. Only a minority (13%) of study participants took part in groups formed by and for predominantly PLHIV (HIV support groups), and participation in such groups was unrelated to their mental or physical health. Personal rather than group-linked social networks and access to network resources were significantly associated with mental but not physical health, after controlling for sociodemographic characteristics. The findings of limited participation in HIV support groups and that the participation in such groups was not significantly associated with physical or mental health may suggest efforts among PLHIV in South Africa to normalise HIV as a chronic illness through broad-based rather than HIV-status bounded social participation, as a strategy for deflecting stigma. Further research is required to examine the effects of HIV treatment on social networking and participation among PLHIV within both rural and other urban settings of South Africa.  相似文献   
954.
955.
956.
957.
958.
959.
960.
Emotional trauma is transmitted across generations. For example, children witnessing their parent expressing fear to specific sounds or images begin to express fear to those cues. Within normal range, this is adaptive, although pathological fear, such as occurs in posttraumatic stress disorder or specific phobias, is also socially transmitted to children and is thus of clinical concern. Here, using a rodent model, we report a mother-to-infant transfer of fear to a novel peppermint odor, which is dependent on the mother expressing fear to that smell in pups’ presence. Examination of pups’ neural activity using c-Fos early gene expression and 14C 2-deoxyglucose autoradiography during mother-to-infant fear transmission revealed lateral and basal amygdala nuclei activity, with a causal role highlighted by pharmacological inactivation of pups’ amygdala preventing the fear transmission. Maternal presence was not needed for fear transmission, because an elevation of pups’ corticosterone induced by the odor of the frightened mother along with a novel peppermint odor was sufficient to produce pups’ subsequent aversion to that odor. Disruption of axonal tracts from the Grueneberg ganglion, a structure implicated in alarm chemosignaling, or blockade of pups’ alarm odor-induced corticosterone increase prevented transfer of fear. These memories are acquired at younger ages compared with amygdala-dependent odor-shock conditioning and are more enduring following minimal conditioning. Our results provide clues to understanding transmission of specific fears across generations and its dependence upon maternal induction of pups’ stress response paired with the cue to induce amygdala-dependent learning plasticity. Results are discussed within the context of caregiver emotional responses and adaptive vs. pathological fears social transmission.Children, including infants, use their parents’ emotions to guide their behavior and learn about safety and danger (14). The infant’s ability to regulate behavior in novel situations using the caregiver’s emotional expression is known as social referencing and occurs in humans and nonhuman primates (1). Although parental physical presence itself or particular cues indicating parental presence, such as voice, touch, or smell typically signal safety for the child, infants are especially responsive to the caregiver’s communication during threats (35). This social learning is critical for enhancing survival through an adaptation to the environment but also provides transmission of pathological fears, such as occurs in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or in specific phobias (37).Despite existing evidence that children are sensitive to parental fear and anxiety, the neurobiological mechanisms for the transmission of parental specific fear to the offspring have remained elusive (27). Animal studies investigating the impact of parental stress on the offspring focused on the history of parental trauma, quality of maternal care, and resultant overall behavioral alterations in the offspring (7, 8). However, to develop efficient survival strategies, progenies must learn about specific environmental threats triggering parental fear (9).Most of what we know about fear learning comes from studies using fear conditioning (FC) (10). In FC, a neutral sensory cue [conditioned stimulus (CS)] is paired with a noxious event [unconditioned stimulus (US)]. Animal studies indicate that the amygdala’s lateral and basal nuclei (LBA) play an important role in FC (10). However, FC in infant rats is naturally attenuated until postnatal day (PND) 10 due to low levels of the stress hormone corticosterone (CORT) during the stress hyporesponsive period (1115). This fear suppression continues in older pups (until PND 16) in the mother’s presence due to social buffering (attenuation) of the shock-induced CORT increase (15).To study the intergenerational transmission of fear to specific triggers, we developed a mother-to-infant social fear learning paradigm. In social fear learning, an organism learns fear through an exposure to a conspecific expressing fear to a discrete CS. Social fear learning may thus serve as a model explaining how defense responses to specific triggers are transmitted between individuals. Social fear learning has been demonstrated in primates, including humans and in rodents, and involves the amygdala (1619).  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号