首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8811篇
  免费   383篇
  国内免费   117篇
耳鼻咽喉   149篇
儿科学   103篇
妇产科学   80篇
基础医学   1872篇
口腔科学   247篇
临床医学   571篇
内科学   401篇
皮肤病学   24篇
神经病学   1659篇
特种医学   285篇
外科学   772篇
综合类   520篇
一般理论   1篇
预防医学   343篇
眼科学   1686篇
药学   303篇
中国医学   153篇
肿瘤学   142篇
  2023年   152篇
  2022年   308篇
  2021年   367篇
  2020年   327篇
  2019年   280篇
  2018年   228篇
  2017年   264篇
  2016年   214篇
  2015年   223篇
  2014年   432篇
  2013年   585篇
  2012年   384篇
  2011年   564篇
  2010年   462篇
  2009年   444篇
  2008年   488篇
  2007年   394篇
  2006年   382篇
  2005年   284篇
  2004年   236篇
  2003年   231篇
  2002年   136篇
  2001年   122篇
  2000年   118篇
  1999年   107篇
  1998年   81篇
  1997年   94篇
  1996年   65篇
  1995年   93篇
  1994年   65篇
  1993年   70篇
  1992年   73篇
  1991年   67篇
  1990年   56篇
  1989年   48篇
  1988年   50篇
  1987年   55篇
  1986年   64篇
  1985年   91篇
  1984年   79篇
  1983年   43篇
  1982年   60篇
  1981年   61篇
  1980年   42篇
  1979年   63篇
  1978年   42篇
  1977年   54篇
  1976年   29篇
  1974年   25篇
  1972年   19篇
排序方式: 共有9311条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Summary and Conclusions In six dark reared, 4-weak-old kittens visual experience was restricted to contours of a single orientation, horizontal or vertical, using cylindrical lenses. Subsequently, the deoxyglucose method was used to determine whether these artificial raising conditions had affected the development of orientation columns in the visual cortex. After application of the deoxyglucose pulse one hemifield was stimulated with vertical, the other with horizontal contours. Thus, from interhemispheric comparison, changes in columnar systems corresponding to experienced and inexperienced orientations could be determined. The following results were obtained: (1) Irrespective of the restrictions in visual experience, orientation columns develop in areas 17, 18, 19 and in the visual areas of the posterior suprasylvian sulcus. (2) Within area 17, spacing between columns encoding the same orientations is remarkably regular (1 mm), is not influenced by selective experience and shows only slight interindividual variation. (3) In non-striate areas the spacing of columns is less regular and the spatial frequency of the periodicity is lower. (4) The modifiability of this columnar pattern by selective experience is small within the granular layer of striate cortex but substantial in non-granular layers: Within layer IV columns whose preference corresponds to the experienced orientation are wider and more active than those encoding the orthogonal orientation but the columnar grid remains basically unaltered. Outside layer IV the columnar system is maintained only for columns encoding the experienced orientations. The deprived columns by contrast frequently fail to extend into non-granular layers and remain confined to the vicinity of layer IV. (5) These modifications in the columnar arrangement are more pronounced in striate cortex than in non-striate visual areas and, within the former, more conspicuous in the central than in the peripheral representation of the visual field. It is concluded that within layer IV the blue print for the system of orientation columns is determined by genetic instructions: first order cells in layer IV develop orientation selectivity irrespective of experience whereby the preference for a particular orientation is predetermined by the position in the columnar grid. Dependent on experience is, however, the expansion of the columnar system from layer IV into non-granular layers. It is argued that all distortions following selective rearing can be accounted for by competitive interactions between intracortical pathways, the mechanisms being identical to those established for competitive processes in the domain of ocular dominance columns. It is proposed that such experience dependent modifiability of connections between first and second order cells is a necessary prerequisite for the development of orientation selectivity in cells with large and complex receptive fields.This work has partially been supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, SFB 50, A14Dedicated to Prof. D. Ploog on the occasion of his 60th anniversaryResearch Fellow of the Alexander-von Humboldt-Stiftung  相似文献   
72.
We investigated changes in the P100 latency of the visual evoked potential (VEP) and the saccadic reaction time (SRT) in relation to the degree of activity of the shoulder girdle elevators. Muscle force was set in 10% increments from 0% to 50% of the maximal voluntary contraction (MVC). The VEP was derived from a midline occipital electrode with reference electrodes on the ears when the right retina was stimulated through the eyelid by light emitting diodes while the eyes were closed. The P100 latency of the VEP was defined as the time from the stimulus onset to the main positive peak. The SRT was defined as the latency until the beginning of eye movement toward the lateral target, which was moved at random time-intervals. P100 latency was shortened until 30% of the MVC, and which it lengthened. The SRT changed in a pattern similar to that observed for the P100 latency. The ratio of the shortening in P100 latency relative to that of the SRT was approximately 20%. All data is presented as the mean value, plus the standard deviation. We believe that the information processing time in the neural pathway from the retina to the visual cortex was shortened up to a certain muscle force of the shoulder girdle elevators, and then this processing time lengthened. These findings indicate that shortening of information processing time in the neural pathway beyond the visual cortex is included in the shortening of the SRT.  相似文献   
73.
Summary The responsiveness of 254 simple and complex striate cortical cells to various forms of static and dynamic textured visual stimuli was studied in cats, lightly anaesthetised with N2O/O2 mixtures supplemented with pentobarbitone.Simple cells were unresponsive to all forms of visual noise presented alone, although about 70% showed a change in responsiveness to conventional bar stimuli when these were presented on moving, rather than stationary, static-noise backgrounds. Bar responses were depressed by background texture motion in a majority of cells (54%), but were actually enhanced in a few instances (16%).In contrast, all complex cells were to some extent responsive to bars of static visual noise moving over stationary backgrounds of similar texture, or to motion of a whole field of static noise. The optimal velocity for noise was generally lower than for bar stimuli.Since moving noise backgrounds were excitatory for complex cells, they tended to reduce specific responses to bar stimulation; in addition, directional bias could be modified by direction and velocity of background motion.Complex cells fell into two overlapping groups as regards their relative sensitivity to light or dark bars and visual noise. Extreme examples were insensitive to conventional bar or edge stimuli while responding briskly to moving noise.In many complex cells, the preferred directions for motion of noise and of an optimally oriented black/white bar were dissimilar.The ocular dominance and the degree of binocular facilitation of some complex cells differed for bar stimuli and visual texture.Preliminary evidence suggests that the deep-layer complex cells (those tolerant of misalignment of line elements; Hammond and MacKay, 1976) were most sensitive to visual noise. Superficial-layer complex cells (those preferring alignment) were less responsive to noise.Only complex-type hypercomplex cells showed any response to visual noise.We conclude that, since simple cells are unresponsive to noise, they cannot provide the sole input to complex cells. The differences in the response of some complex cells to rectilinear and textured stimuli throw a new light on their rôle in cortical information-processing. In particular, it tells against the hypothesis that they act as a second stage in the abstraction of edge-orientation.  相似文献   
74.
Peak Identification in Visual Evoked Potentials   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Waveform patterns evoked by 4 intensities of flash in normal subjects were studied in relation to intersubject variability. Time-frequency distribution curves of all peaks occurring between 11 and 280 msec after flash onset and meeting minimal criteria were obtained from 46 males. These distributions closely corresponded to similar data reported by others for single intensity stimulation. An algorithm was developed which identified in 67 to 100% of instances a single “peak event’ within the time ranges of each of 6 peak distributions. Many peak events appeared and disappeared within the 4 intensity sets of individuals. Latencies were obtained for these peak events. Application of the algorithm to a replicate sample of 29 Ss, which included 8 females, indicated generalizability. Test-retest data on 15 Ss showed its reliability. The data suggest that methodology significantly contributes to the variability of peak identification among subjects. This may be reduced by employing multiple intensities of stimulation.  相似文献   
75.
Twelve females were tested at four times during the menstrual cycle with a visual detection task and a visual pattern discrimination task. Mood levels and confidence ratings were evaluated for each session. In addition to the behavioral testing, plasma samples were collected and radioimmunoassayed for estradiol, progesterone, luteinizing hormone, and follicle stimulating hormone levels. Visual detection fluctuated significantly during the menstrual cycle with impaired performance occurring at the premenstrual session. In contrast to previous reports, the impaired performance was not related to lowered confidence ratings or to mood levels.  相似文献   
76.
Summary The cortical afferents to the cortex of the anterior ectosylvian sulcus (SEsA) were studied in the cat, using the retrograde axonal transport of horseradish peroxidase technique. Following injections of the enzyme in the cortex of both banks, fundus and both ends (postero-dorsal and anteroventral) of the anterior ectosylvian sulcus, retrograde labeling was found in: the primary, secondary, and tertiary somatosensory areas (SI, SII and SIII); the motor and premotor cortices; the primary, secondary, anterior and suprasylvian fringe auditory areas; the lateral suprasylvian (LS) area, area 20 and posterior suprasylvian visual area; the insular cortex and cortex of posterior half of the sulcus sylvius; in area 36 of the perirhinal cortex; and in the medial bank of the presylvian sulcus in the prefrontal cortex. Moreover, these connections are topographically organized. Considering the topographical distribution of the cortical afferents, three sectors may be distinguished in the cortex of the SEsA. 1) The cortex of the rostral two-thirds of the dorsal bank. This sector receives cortical projections from areas SI, SII and SIII, and from the motor cortex. It also receives projections from the anterolateral subdivision of LS, and area 36. 2) The cortex of the posterior third of the dorsal bank and of the posterodorsal end. It receives cortical afferents principally from the primary, secondary and anterior auditory areas, from SI, SII and fourth somatosensory area, from the anterolateral subdivision of LS, vestibular cortex and area 36. 3) The cortex of the ventral bank and fundus. This sulcal sector receives abundant connections from visual areas (LS, 20, posterior suprasylvian, 21 and 19), principally from the lateral posterior and dorsal subdivisions of LS. It also receives abundant connections from the granular insular cortex, caudal part of the cortex of the sylvian sulcus and suprasylvian fringe. Less abundant cortical afferents were found to arise in area 36, second auditory area and prefrontal cortex. The abundant sensory input of different modalities which appears to converge in the cortex of the anterior ectosylvian sulcus, and the consistent projection from this cortex to the deep layers of the superior colliculus, make this cortical region well suited to play a role in the control of the orientation movements of the eyes and head toward different sensory stimuli.Supported by FISSS grants 521/81 and 1250/84  相似文献   
77.
We studied the role of the nucleus of the optic tract (NOT) in adapting the gain of the angular vestibulo-ocular reflex (aVOR) in rhesus and cynomolgus monkeys using lesions and temporary inactivation with muscimol. The aVOR gain was adaptively reduced by forced sinusoidal rotation (0.25 Hz, 60 degrees/s) in a self-stationary visual surround, i.e., a visual surround that moved with the subject, or by wearing x0.5 reducing lenses during natural head movements. The aVOR gains dropped by 20-30% after 2 h and by about 30% after 4 h. Muscimol injections caused a loss of adaptation of contraversive-eye velocities induced by the aVOR, and their gains promptly returned to or above preadapted levels. The gains of the adapted ipsiversive and vertical eye velocities produced by the aVOR were unaffected by muscimol injections. Lesions of NOT significantly reduced or abolished the animals' ability to adapt the gain of contraversive aVOR-induced eye velocities, and the monkeys were unable to suppress these contraversive-eye velocities in a self-stationary surround. The lesions did not affect ipsiversive aVOR-induced eye velocities, and the animals were still able to suppress them. Lesions of NOT also affected the unadapted or "default" aVOR gains. After unilateral NOT lesions, gains of ipsiversive aVOR-induced eye velocity were reduced, while gains of contraversive aVOR-induced eye velocity were either unaffected or slightly increased. Consistent with this, muscimol injections into the NOT of unadapted monkeys slightly reduced the gains of ipsiversive and increased the gains of contraversive-eye velocities by about 8-10%. We conclude that each NOT processes ipsiversive retinal-slip information about visual surround movement relative to the head induced by the aVOR. In the presence of visual surround movement, the retinal-slip signal is suppressed, leading to adaptive changes in the gain of aVOR-induced contraversive horizontal eye velocities. NOT also has a role in controlling and maintaining the current state of the aVOR gains. Thus, it plays a unique role in producing and supporting adaptation of the gain of the horizontal aVOR that is likely to be important for stabilizing gaze during head movement. Pathways through the inferior olive are presumably important for this adaptation.  相似文献   
78.
Neurons in the rostral superior colliculus (SC) of alert cats exhibit quasi-sustained discharge patterns related to the fixation of visual targets. Because some SC neurons also respond to auditory stimuli, we investigated whether there is a population of neurons in the rostral SC which is active in relation to fixation of both auditory and visual targets. We identified cells which were active with visual fixation and which continued to discharge if the fixation stimulus was briefly extinguished. The population of neurons exhibited similar discharge characteristics when the fixation stimulus was auditory. Few neurons were significantly more active during fixation of visual targets than during fixation of auditory targets. Most fixation neurons showed a diminished discharge rate during spontaneous (self-generated) saccadic eye movements away from a visual fixation stimulus, regardless of the direction of the saccade. this diminished discharge rate (or pause) typically began, on average, 12.2 ms before saccade onset and the duration of the pause was Ionger than the duration of the saccade. These observations are consistent with the hypothesis that increased discharge of these neurons is related to active fixation and that reductions in their activity are important for the generation of saccades. However, the lack of a precise relationship between pause duration and saccade duration implies that these neurons would be unlikely to project directly to the saccadic burst generator. The mean interval from the beginning of the pauses of fixation neurons to be beginning of the saccades away from fixation targets is also shorter than has been found in brainstem omnipause neurons. By analogy with the concept of a receptive field, agaze position error field depicts the range of gaze position error for which a cell is active. Although fixation neurons appear to encode the magnitude and direction of the error between visual targets and the visual axis, visual error fields at the end of fixating eye movements were significantly larger than those at stimulus onset. For auditory stimuli, this difference was not significant. These observations are compatible with a number of recent experiments indicating that neural signals of eye position are damped or delayed with respect to current eye position.  相似文献   
79.
Summary Three methods for analyzing the spatial organization of visual evoked potentials were compared. Pattern reversal visual evoked potentials were obtained from a single subject under three viewing conditions: stimulation of the left, right, and both visual fields. The scalp distribution of the VEP to 1 deg checks was displayed using three recording and analysis techniques: a conventional horizontal occipital array of electrodes, topographic mapping, and 3-dimensional evoked potentials. All three techniques revealed "paradoxical" lateralization of P100. The relative merits of each technique are discussed.We are grateful to Susan Hoffmann-Nader and Rebecca Clark-Bash for data collection. This research was supported in part by the Brain Research Foundation of the University of Chicago.  相似文献   
80.
Recent advances in the study of evoked responses settled a definition of transient and steady-state visual evoked responses (VER) and discovered the existence of Temporal Frequency Regions in humans. This paper reports data from visual responses evoked by low and high frequency stimuli. White flashes were performed in albino Sprague Dawley rats from 7 to 90 days of age. Frequency analysis technique offered the possibility to evaluate the amplitude parameters and their variations dependent on intensity of luminance and on development. The responses suggested the existence of two temporal frequency regions in the rat: a first one at about 8 Hz, related to high frequencies of EEG, a second one at frequencies ranging from 12 to 24 Hz, related to luminance sensitivity. The development of the second region is correspondent to the complete development of transient VER parameters.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号