全文获取类型
收费全文 | 717篇 |
免费 | 31篇 |
国内免费 | 96篇 |
专业分类
耳鼻咽喉 | 2篇 |
儿科学 | 33篇 |
基础医学 | 112篇 |
口腔科学 | 21篇 |
临床医学 | 107篇 |
内科学 | 141篇 |
皮肤病学 | 7篇 |
神经病学 | 92篇 |
特种医学 | 109篇 |
外科学 | 22篇 |
综合类 | 35篇 |
预防医学 | 36篇 |
眼科学 | 11篇 |
药学 | 81篇 |
肿瘤学 | 35篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 4篇 |
2017年 | 4篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 4篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 23篇 |
2012年 | 12篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 31篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 67篇 |
2006年 | 20篇 |
2005年 | 27篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 15篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 48篇 |
1997年 | 41篇 |
1996年 | 30篇 |
1995年 | 30篇 |
1994年 | 28篇 |
1993年 | 38篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 29篇 |
1988年 | 27篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1986年 | 23篇 |
1985年 | 18篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有844条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
Fabian SL Yii 《Ophthalmic & physiological optics》2023,43(6):1412-1418
Purpose
To compare axial length (AL) growth curves in East Asian (EA) and non-EA emmetropes.Methods
A meta-regression of 28 studies with emmetrope-specific AL data (measured with optical biometry) was performed. Emmetropia was defined as spherical equivalent refraction (SER) between −0.50 and +1.25 D, determined under cycloplegia if the mean age was ≤20 years. The AL growth curve (mean AL vs. mean age) was first fitted to the full dataset using a weighted nonlinear mixed-effects model, before refitting the model with ethnicity as a two-level grouping variable (EA vs. non-EA). Ethnic differences in growth curve parameters were tested using the Wald test.Results
A total of 3331 EA and 1071 non-EA emmetropes (mean age: 6.5–23.1 years) were included. There was no evidence of an ethnic difference in either final AL (difference: 0.15 mm, 95% CI: −0.04 to 0.35 mm, p = 0.15) or initial AL, as represented by the amount that the final AL needed to be offset to obtain the y-intercept (difference: −2.77 mm, 95% CI: −10.97 to 5.44, p = 0.51). Likewise, AL growth rate (curve steepness) did not differ between ethnic groups (difference: 0.09, 95% CI: −0.13 to 0.31, p = 0.43). Collectively, AL growth rate decreased from 0.24 mm/year at 6 years of age to around 0.05 mm/year at 11 years of age, after which it dipped below the repeatability of optical biometry (±0.04 mm) and practically plateaued around 16 years of age (final AL: 23.60 mm).Conclusions
EA and non-EA emmetropes have comparable AL growth curves. 相似文献54.
55.
56.
Fourteen institutions performed 1,830 computed tomographic (CT) cerebral blood flow (CBF) examinations with 32% inhaled stable xenon. Respiratory rate delay greater than 10 seconds occurred in 3.6% of patients, with 83% of the delays lasting 10-15 seconds. There was no incident of prolonged respiratory difficulty. Headache (0.4%), seizures (0.2%), nausea and vomiting (0.2%), and change in neurologic status (0.1%) were uncommon, and there were no transient ischemic attacks. The CT CBF method with 32% inhaled stable xenon is thus associated with an acceptably low incidence of adverse reactions. 相似文献
57.
58.
Pavan A Alexander I Campana G Cowey A 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2011,214(2):261-271
Blindsight patients can detect fast moving stimuli presented within their blind field even when they deny any phenomenal visual
experience. Although mounting evidence suggests the presence of different mechanisms and separate neural substrates underlying
the processing of first-order (luminance-defined) and second-order (contrast-defined) motion, the perception of second-order
motion in blindsight has scarcely been explored. In the present study, we investigated whether two blindsighted patients (GY
and MS) can detect a variety of first- and second-order moving stimuli, and by using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation
(rTMS), we assessed the role of V5/MT+ and V3+ in coherent motion processing. The hemianopes and four control subjects performed a two-interval forced-choice task in which
they judged whether a pattern of coherently moving first-order or second-order textured squares moved in the first or second
interval. They were not asked to report the direction of motion because neither of them could do so better than expected by
chance. The results showed that MS, who has extensive destruction of the ventral cortical visual pathway as well as his V1
lesion, could not process second-order motion at all, whereas GY could perform second-order tasks but only at high-contrast
modulation. This may have introduced first-order components in second-order moving stimuli and provided artifactual cues to
motion. Moreover, rTMS delivered over area V5/MT+ impaired detection of both first- and second-order motion in undamaged control subjects, whereas rTMS over V3+ did not impair their performance in any of the stimuli employed. On the other hand, rTMS over V3+ did impair GY’s detection of first-order motion and high-contrast second-order moving textured squares that are likely to
contain artifactual luminance cues. rTMS over V5/MT+ impaired first-order motion detection in MS. Overall, the results suggest that neither of the blindsight patients can detect
artifact-free second-order motion. 相似文献
59.
Sharavanan Parasivam MBBS FRACP Max Cowey BSc Bruce Day MBBS FRACP Dominic Thyagarajan MBBS MD FRACP 《Movement disorders》2009,24(3):422-425
Multimodal representation of peripersonal or near space has been demonstrated in the brain of the nonhuman primate through invasive electrophysiological experiments. Representation of peripersonal space in the human brain has been inferred from extinction experiments and functional imaging studies. We present a unique case of lower limb myoclonus in a patient with common variable immunodeficiency which is sensitive to visual stimuli in the peripersonal space and light touch. This case provides further evidence for near space representation in the human brain. We hypothesize that somatopically organized multimodal areas exist in the human brain which code for peripersonal space. © 2008 Movement Disorder Society 相似文献
60.
"Priming of pop-out" is a form of implicit memory that facilitates detection of a recently inspected search target. Repeated presentation of a target's features or its spatial position improves detection speed (feature/spatial priming). This study investigated a role for the human frontal eye fields (FEFs) in the priming of color pop-out. To test the hypothesis that the FEFs play a role in short-term memory storage, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was applied during the intertrial interval. There was no effect of TMS on either spatial or feature priming. To test whether the FEFs are important when a saccade is being programmed to a repeated target color or location, TMS was applied during the search array. TMS over the left but not the right FEFs abolished spatial priming, but had no effect on feature priming. These findings demonstrate functional specialization of the left FEFs for spatial priming, and distinguish this role from target discrimination and saccade-related processes. The results suggest that the left FEFs integrate a spatial memory signal with an evolving saccade program, which facilitates saccades to a recently inspected location. 相似文献