首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   717篇
  免费   31篇
  国内免费   96篇
耳鼻咽喉   2篇
儿科学   33篇
基础医学   112篇
口腔科学   21篇
临床医学   107篇
内科学   141篇
皮肤病学   7篇
神经病学   92篇
特种医学   109篇
外科学   22篇
综合类   35篇
预防医学   36篇
眼科学   11篇
药学   81篇
肿瘤学   35篇
  2023年   3篇
  2021年   7篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   4篇
  2017年   4篇
  2016年   6篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   9篇
  2013年   23篇
  2012年   12篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   32篇
  2009年   31篇
  2008年   17篇
  2007年   67篇
  2006年   20篇
  2005年   27篇
  2004年   10篇
  2003年   14篇
  2002年   13篇
  2001年   19篇
  2000年   15篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   48篇
  1997年   41篇
  1996年   30篇
  1995年   30篇
  1994年   28篇
  1993年   38篇
  1992年   14篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   17篇
  1989年   29篇
  1988年   27篇
  1987年   21篇
  1986年   23篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   16篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   11篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   12篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   3篇
  1971年   5篇
  1970年   3篇
排序方式: 共有844条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
53.

Purpose

To compare axial length (AL) growth curves in East Asian (EA) and non-EA emmetropes.

Methods

A meta-regression of 28 studies with emmetrope-specific AL data (measured with optical biometry) was performed. Emmetropia was defined as spherical equivalent refraction (SER) between −0.50 and +1.25 D, determined under cycloplegia if the mean age was ≤20 years. The AL growth curve (mean AL vs. mean age) was first fitted to the full dataset using a weighted nonlinear mixed-effects model, before refitting the model with ethnicity as a two-level grouping variable (EA vs. non-EA). Ethnic differences in growth curve parameters were tested using the Wald test.

Results

A total of 3331 EA and 1071 non-EA emmetropes (mean age: 6.5–23.1 years) were included. There was no evidence of an ethnic difference in either final AL (difference: 0.15 mm, 95% CI: −0.04 to 0.35 mm, p = 0.15) or initial AL, as represented by the amount that the final AL needed to be offset to obtain the y-intercept (difference: −2.77 mm, 95% CI: −10.97 to 5.44, p = 0.51). Likewise, AL growth rate (curve steepness) did not differ between ethnic groups (difference: 0.09, 95% CI: −0.13 to 0.31, p = 0.43). Collectively, AL growth rate decreased from 0.24 mm/year at 6 years of age to around 0.05 mm/year at 11 years of age, after which it dipped below the repeatability of optical biometry (±0.04 mm) and practically plateaued around 16 years of age (final AL: 23.60 mm).

Conclusions

EA and non-EA emmetropes have comparable AL growth curves.  相似文献   
54.
黄柏及中成药中小檗碱和巴马亭的高效液相色谱法测定   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
本文以正相高效液相色谱法,用窗口图解技术对色谱条件进行了优化。对黄柏及其中成药中的有效成分——小檗碱、巴马亭的提取、测定条件、标准曲线进行了研究。并对两种含黄柏的中成药样品进行了分析。其中小檗碱的回收率均在97%以上,巴马亭的回收率均在96%以上。  相似文献   
55.
56.
Adverse reactions to xenon-enhanced CT cerebral blood flow determination   总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7  
Latchaw  RE; Yonas  H; Pentheny  SL; Gur  D 《Radiology》1987,163(1):251-254
Fourteen institutions performed 1,830 computed tomographic (CT) cerebral blood flow (CBF) examinations with 32% inhaled stable xenon. Respiratory rate delay greater than 10 seconds occurred in 3.6% of patients, with 83% of the delays lasting 10-15 seconds. There was no incident of prolonged respiratory difficulty. Headache (0.4%), seizures (0.2%), nausea and vomiting (0.2%), and change in neurologic status (0.1%) were uncommon, and there were no transient ischemic attacks. The CT CBF method with 32% inhaled stable xenon is thus associated with an acceptably low incidence of adverse reactions.  相似文献   
57.
58.
Blindsight patients can detect fast moving stimuli presented within their blind field even when they deny any phenomenal visual experience. Although mounting evidence suggests the presence of different mechanisms and separate neural substrates underlying the processing of first-order (luminance-defined) and second-order (contrast-defined) motion, the perception of second-order motion in blindsight has scarcely been explored. In the present study, we investigated whether two blindsighted patients (GY and MS) can detect a variety of first- and second-order moving stimuli, and by using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), we assessed the role of V5/MT+ and V3+ in coherent motion processing. The hemianopes and four control subjects performed a two-interval forced-choice task in which they judged whether a pattern of coherently moving first-order or second-order textured squares moved in the first or second interval. They were not asked to report the direction of motion because neither of them could do so better than expected by chance. The results showed that MS, who has extensive destruction of the ventral cortical visual pathway as well as his V1 lesion, could not process second-order motion at all, whereas GY could perform second-order tasks but only at high-contrast modulation. This may have introduced first-order components in second-order moving stimuli and provided artifactual cues to motion. Moreover, rTMS delivered over area V5/MT+ impaired detection of both first- and second-order motion in undamaged control subjects, whereas rTMS over V3+ did not impair their performance in any of the stimuli employed. On the other hand, rTMS over V3+ did impair GY’s detection of first-order motion and high-contrast second-order moving textured squares that are likely to contain artifactual luminance cues. rTMS over V5/MT+ impaired first-order motion detection in MS. Overall, the results suggest that neither of the blindsight patients can detect artifact-free second-order motion.  相似文献   
59.
Multimodal representation of peripersonal or near space has been demonstrated in the brain of the nonhuman primate through invasive electrophysiological experiments. Representation of peripersonal space in the human brain has been inferred from extinction experiments and functional imaging studies. We present a unique case of lower limb myoclonus in a patient with common variable immunodeficiency which is sensitive to visual stimuli in the peripersonal space and light touch. This case provides further evidence for near space representation in the human brain. We hypothesize that somatopically organized multimodal areas exist in the human brain which code for peripersonal space. © 2008 Movement Disorder Society  相似文献   
60.
"Priming of pop-out" is a form of implicit memory that facilitates detection of a recently inspected search target. Repeated presentation of a target's features or its spatial position improves detection speed (feature/spatial priming). This study investigated a role for the human frontal eye fields (FEFs) in the priming of color pop-out. To test the hypothesis that the FEFs play a role in short-term memory storage, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was applied during the intertrial interval. There was no effect of TMS on either spatial or feature priming. To test whether the FEFs are important when a saccade is being programmed to a repeated target color or location, TMS was applied during the search array. TMS over the left but not the right FEFs abolished spatial priming, but had no effect on feature priming. These findings demonstrate functional specialization of the left FEFs for spatial priming, and distinguish this role from target discrimination and saccade-related processes. The results suggest that the left FEFs integrate a spatial memory signal with an evolving saccade program, which facilitates saccades to a recently inspected location.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号