首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   7937篇
  免费   847篇
  国内免费   4篇
耳鼻咽喉   74篇
儿科学   189篇
妇产科学   129篇
基础医学   1118篇
口腔科学   286篇
临床医学   935篇
内科学   1582篇
皮肤病学   102篇
神经病学   600篇
特种医学   233篇
外科学   1201篇
综合类   208篇
一般理论   6篇
预防医学   808篇
眼科学   194篇
药学   633篇
中国医学   1篇
肿瘤学   489篇
  2021年   88篇
  2020年   59篇
  2019年   102篇
  2018年   118篇
  2017年   121篇
  2016年   121篇
  2015年   131篇
  2014年   132篇
  2013年   239篇
  2012年   353篇
  2011年   380篇
  2010年   228篇
  2009年   195篇
  2008年   310篇
  2007年   348篇
  2006年   342篇
  2005年   373篇
  2004年   298篇
  2003年   350篇
  2002年   305篇
  2001年   250篇
  2000年   280篇
  1999年   234篇
  1998年   91篇
  1997年   102篇
  1996年   86篇
  1995年   88篇
  1994年   98篇
  1993年   83篇
  1992年   194篇
  1991年   174篇
  1990年   162篇
  1989年   163篇
  1988年   155篇
  1987年   154篇
  1986年   138篇
  1985年   158篇
  1984年   114篇
  1983年   104篇
  1982年   73篇
  1981年   71篇
  1980年   59篇
  1979年   110篇
  1978年   69篇
  1977年   74篇
  1973年   85篇
  1972年   74篇
  1971年   73篇
  1970年   61篇
  1968年   59篇
排序方式: 共有8788条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
A considerable body of clinical literature discusses patient adjustment to spinal cord injury (SCI) but there are few empirical data, particularly about postrehabilitation hospital adjustment. The present study targets changes in psychologic adjustment after initial rehabilitation hospitalization. Thirty-six persons with SCI completed a questionnaire designed to study such adjustment at 3 weeks, 3 months, and 1 year postdischarge. Twenty-nine able-bodied controls completed the same questionnaire at similar time intervals. Outcome measures used were the Beck Depression Inventory, the Wiggins Hostility Scale, and the Handicap Problems Inventory. Moderately increased depression and hostility in the SCI group compared to the able-bodied were found immediately postdischarge, but these differences were gone within a year. On a specific measure of adjustment to disability, the SCI group showed significantly increasing comfort with disability status over the same period. While there are reports of psychologic adjustment difficulties for persons with SCI postdischarge, the severity of these difficulties may have been overestimated and our findings suggest that problems appearing immediately after discharge appear to resolve rapidly.  相似文献   
32.

Introduction

Ambulance diversion is a problem in many communities. When patients are diverted prompt and appropriate medical care may be delayed.

Objective

Compare diversion hours and drop-off times before and after a dramatic change in diversion policy restricting each hospital to 1 hour out of every 8.

Methods

This study was a retrospective study in a county of 600 000 people and 10 hospitals from September 2004 to February 2006. A countywide diversion protocol was implemented in March 2005 that limited diversion hours to 1 hour out of every 8 (maximum of 90 h/mo). No other changes were implemented during the study period. Pretrial (9/04-2/05), interim (3/05-8/05), and posttrial (9/05-2/06) periods were compared. The main outcome measures were ambulance diversion hours and emergency medical service (EMS) drop-off times. Results were compared using analysis of variance and a Tukey post hoc analysis. P < .05 was considered significant.

Results

There was no significant difference in the number of monthly transports comparing the posttrial vs pretrial periods; however, a significant decrease in monthly ambulance diversion hours (difference, 251 hours; 95% CI, 136-368) and significant increase in additional time that EMS crews required to transport patients (drop-off times) (difference, 178 hours; 95% CI, 74-283) were observed. Posttrial diversion hours decreased to 18% of the pretrial values (from 305 to 54).

Conclusion

This novel ambulance diversion protocol dramatically reduced diversion hours at the cost of increasing EMS drop-off times in a large community.  相似文献   
33.
34.
35.
36.
Methods:We performed a matched-cohort study comparing 16 single-port robotic-assisted adrenalectomies with 16 patients from a pool of 148 laparoscopic adrenalectomies, matched for age, gender, operative side, pathology, and body mass index. All were operated on by 1 surgeon.Results:The pathology included aldosteronoma in 44% of patients, adrenocorticotropic hormone–dependent Cushing syndrome (bilateral adrenalectomy) in 19%, pheochromocytoma in 13%, and other pathology in 24%. The operative time was 183 ± 33 minutes for single-port robotic-assisted adrenalectomy and 173 ± 40 minutes for laparoscopic adrenalectomy (P = .58). The total time in the operating room was 246 ± 33 minutes for single-port robotic-assisted adrenalectomy and 240 ± 39 minutes for laparoscopic adrenalectomy (P = .57). There was 1 conversion to open adrenalectomy (6%) in each group, both because of bleeding on the right side during bilateral adrenalectomy. Two right-sided single-port robotic-assisted adrenalectomy patients required conversion to laparoscopic adrenalectomy, one because of poor visualization. There were no deaths. Complications occurred in 2 patients in each group (intensive care unit admission, prolonged ileus). Both groups had similar pain scores (mean of 3.7 on a scale from 1 to 10) on postoperative day 1, and patients in the single-port robotic-assisted adrenalectomy group used less narcotic pain medication in the first 24 hours after surgery (43 mg vs 84 mg in laparoscopic adrenalectomy group, P < .001). The differences between the single-port robotic-assisted adrenalectomy group and laparoscopic adrenalectomy group in length of stay (2.3 ± 0.5 days vs 3.1 ± 0.9 days, P = .23), percentage of patients discharged on postoperative day 1 (56% vs 31%, P = .10), and hospital cost (16% lower in single-port robotic-assisted adrenalectomy group, P = .17) did not reach statistical significance.Conclusion:Single-port robotic adrenalectomy is feasible; patients require less narcotic pain medication whereas costs appear equivalent compared with laparoscopic adrenalectomy.  相似文献   
37.
Recent theoretical work emphasizes the role of expectation in neural processing, shifting the focus from feed-forward cortical hierarchies to models that include extensive feedback (e.g., predictive coding). Empirical support for expectation-related feedback is compelling but restricted to adult humans and nonhuman animals. Given the considerable differences in neural organization, connectivity, and efficiency between infant and adult brains, it is a crucial yet open question whether expectation-related feedback is an inherent property of the cortex (i.e., operational early in development) or whether expectation-related feedback develops with extensive experience and neural maturation. To determine whether infants’ expectations about future sensory input modulate their sensory cortices without the confounds of stimulus novelty or repetition suppression, we used a cross-modal (audiovisual) omission paradigm and used functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to record hemodynamic responses in the infant cortex. We show that the occipital cortex of 6-month-old infants exhibits the signature of expectation-based feedback. Crucially, we found that this region does not respond to auditory stimuli if they are not predictive of a visual event. Overall, these findings suggest that the young infant’s brain is already capable of some rudimentary form of expectation-based feedback.Over the past two decades, theoretical focus has shifted from predominantly feed-forward hierarchies of cortical function, where sensory cortex propagates information to higher level analyzers on the way to decision and motor control areas, to models in which feedback connections to lower-level cortical regions allow extensive top-down functional modulation based on expectation (1). An influential model for incorporating feedback is predictive coding, which compares expectations or predictions to input at each level of the processing hierarchy (24). There is extensive and compelling evidence for expectation-based modulation even at the earliest levels of sensory processing in both humans (57) and nonhuman animals (8). Because complex, naturalistic sensory input is characterized by temporal, spatial, and contextual regularities, the ability to modulate early sensory function as a result of expectations is believed to support adaptive perceptual abilities (4).Empirical evidence for expectation-based feedback, however, is restricted to adult humans and nonhuman animals. With their extensive experience, adults already have developed sophisticated internal models of the environment and have a highly interconnected brain and efficient neural processing. Comparatively, human infants are born with a demonstratively immature behavioral repertoire, have underdeveloped sensorimotor and cognitive capacities, and lack sophisticated internal models of the environment. These internal models undergo substantial postnatal development even in the early sensory cortex. For example, spontaneous activity of primary visual cortex (V1) in ferrets converges with evoked visual responses to naturalistic stimuli across development but does not converge with evoked responses to nonnaturalistic stimuli (9). Thus, statistics in sensory input progressively create an adaptive, internal model of the environment that is evident in V1, and, therefore, the neonate brain does not have access to sophisticated expectations about sensory input. Moreover, converging evidence from both anatomical (10) and functional connectivity analyses (11) has demonstrated that human infants have a much less interconnected brain and instead have a predominance of local connections and a slow development of long-range interactions (12). Finally, human infants exhibit characteristically slower neural processing even for basic sensory stimuli (13).Given the considerable differences between infant and adult brains, it is unclear whether the infant brain is capable of the sophisticated expectation-based modulation of sensory cortices that has been observed in adult brains. Relatedly, the developmental process by which feedback comes to modulate neural function based on changing expectations is entirely unknown. One possibility is that infants do not exhibit expectation-based feedback, but rely on a predominantly feed-forward architecture. In this case, a feed-forward neural architecture would be a developmental precursor to the feedback mechanisms that would emerge after extensive postnatal experience and would be consistent with the immaturity of infant cognition and sensorimotor control. Another possibility is that the fundamental architecture that allows expectation-based feedback is present in early infancy and provides a scaffold for the development of efficient internal models of the environment. Here, we ask whether expectation-based feedback is evident in the infant brain: specifically, in the sensory cortex.Despite extensive behavioral evidence that infants can form expectations in many perceptual and cognitive domains (14, 15) and are able to quickly and robustly respond to new information in their environment (e.g., statistical learning abilities) (16, 17), it remains unclear how their expectations about future sensory input are instantiated neurally. The infant event-related potentials (ERPs) literature provides a glimpse but falls short of clarifying the underlying neural mechanism. The traditional oddball design, which presents a frequent stimulus and an infrequent stimulus (e.g., 80% and 20% respectively), reveals a robust novelty response in several waveform components (delayed version of the P300 and the mismatch negativity) (1820). However, the ERP is exquisitely sensitive to the properties of the stimuli, and so the enhanced response to the infrequent stimulus could be a prediction error, a nonspecific surprise effect, or a recovery to the greater repetition of the frequent stimulus (i.e., rebound from repetition suppression) (21). Using similar designs with similar interpretative limitations, recent work with functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) (an optical imaging method for noninvasively recording functional changes in the hemodynamics of the infant cortex while the infant is awake and behaving) (22, 23) has revealed increased neural responses to the presentation of novel acoustic stimuli (24) and evidence of stimulus anticipation (25).A more interpretively transparent design for assessing whether expectations can modulate responses in sensory cortex is the stimulus-omission paradigm. Here, two or more stimuli are presented in a predictable temporal order, and one of the expected stimuli is occasionally and unexpectedly omitted. Because there is no sensory input during stimulus omission, the response cannot be due to recovery from repetition suppression, and, if the effect is localized (i.e., does not occur in all cortical regions), it cannot be due to nonspecific surprise. This stimulus-omission paradigm has been used with mice to record from V1 with multielectrode arrays (8), with human adults to record from auditory and frontal areas with magnetoencephalography (MEG) (7), from V1 using fMRI (26), and with presurgical epilepsy patients to record from temporal-parietal areas with cortical electrodes (27). A particularly impressive variation is the ability of a recently learned cross-modal association (e.g., audiovisual stimuli) to generate responses to the unexpected omission of one of the previously paired stimuli, as recently demonstrated in human adults (5, 28).We used the cross-modal stimulus omission paradigm to determine whether a recently learned audiovisual association drives occipital cortex responses during an unexpected visual omission in young human infants. We presented 36 6-month-old infants (Materials and Methods) with novel sounds and visual stimuli such that a sound predicted the presentation of a visual stimulus. Infants can rapidly learn arbitrary audiovisual associations even from a young age (29, 30). Therefore, after a brief period of familiarization (less than 2 min, 18 audiovisual or A+V+ events), infants were presented with trials where the predictive auditory stimulus was presented and the visual stimulus was unexpectedly withheld (see Fig. 1 and Materials and Methods). These unexpected visual omission trials violate sensory expectations but do not present any sensory input. Therefore, any occipital cortex responses to unexpected visual omissions were evidence of a neural response generated as a result of a violation of sensory expectation and not a result of lower-level neural adaptation effects, such as repetition suppression or generalized novelty responses.Open in a separate windowFig. 1.Schematic of the two trial types: A+V+ (audiovisual trials) and A+V− (visual omission trials). After a period of familiarization to only A+V+ trials, infants saw visual stimuli accompanying auditory stimuli 80% of the time. Thus, infants expected to see the visual stimulus after each auditory stimulus.  相似文献   
38.
39.
40.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号