首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2808篇
  免费   182篇
  国内免费   4篇
耳鼻咽喉   30篇
儿科学   109篇
妇产科学   97篇
基础医学   435篇
口腔科学   88篇
临床医学   267篇
内科学   478篇
皮肤病学   77篇
神经病学   303篇
特种医学   169篇
外科学   348篇
综合类   40篇
一般理论   3篇
预防医学   203篇
眼科学   35篇
药学   179篇
中国医学   4篇
肿瘤学   129篇
  2022年   20篇
  2021年   49篇
  2020年   27篇
  2019年   59篇
  2018年   57篇
  2017年   41篇
  2016年   45篇
  2015年   64篇
  2014年   62篇
  2013年   105篇
  2012年   153篇
  2011年   163篇
  2010年   90篇
  2009年   72篇
  2008年   166篇
  2007年   163篇
  2006年   135篇
  2005年   109篇
  2004年   111篇
  2003年   93篇
  2002年   91篇
  2001年   68篇
  2000年   70篇
  1999年   70篇
  1998年   42篇
  1997年   26篇
  1996年   28篇
  1995年   37篇
  1994年   27篇
  1993年   31篇
  1992年   49篇
  1991年   43篇
  1990年   40篇
  1989年   49篇
  1988年   47篇
  1987年   32篇
  1986年   29篇
  1985年   36篇
  1984年   33篇
  1983年   26篇
  1980年   20篇
  1979年   21篇
  1978年   16篇
  1977年   19篇
  1976年   24篇
  1974年   23篇
  1973年   19篇
  1972年   20篇
  1971年   17篇
  1970年   19篇
排序方式: 共有2994条查询结果,搜索用时 703 毫秒
991.
To gain insight into the genomic basis of diffuse large B-cell lymphoma (DLBCL), we performed massively parallel whole-exome sequencing of 55 primary tumor samples from patients with DLBCL and matched normal tissue. We identified recurrent mutations in genes that are well known to be functionally relevant in DLBCL, including MYD88, CARD11, EZH2, and CREBBP. We also identified somatic mutations in genes for which a functional role in DLBCL has not been previously suspected. These genes include MEF2B, MLL2, BTG1, GNA13, ACTB, P2RY8, PCLO, and TNFRSF14. Further, we show that BCL2 mutations commonly occur in patients with BCL2/IgH rearrangements as a result of somatic hypermutation normally occurring at the IgH locus. The BCL2 point mutations are primarily synonymous, and likely caused by activation-induced cytidine deaminase-mediated somatic hypermutation, as shown by comprehensive analysis of enrichment of mutations in WRCY target motifs. Those nonsynonymous mutations that are observed tend to be found outside of the functionally important BH domains of the protein, suggesting that strong negative selection against BCL2 loss-of-function mutations is at play. Last, by using an algorithm designed to identify likely functionally relevant but infrequent mutations, we identify KRAS, BRAF, and NOTCH1 as likely drivers of DLBCL pathogenesis in some patients. Our data provide an unbiased view of the landscape of mutations in DLBCL, and this in turn may point toward new therapeutic strategies for the disease.  相似文献   
992.
993.
994.
Despite a characteristic indolent course, a substantial subset of follicular lymphoma (FL) patients has an early relapse with a poor outcome. Cells in the microenvironment may be a key contributor to treatment failure. We used a discovery and validation study design to identify microenvironmental determinants of early failure and then integrated these results into the FLIPI. In total, 496 newly diagnosed FL grade 1–3 A patients who were prospectively enrolled into the MER cohort from 2002 to 2012 were evaluated. Tissue microarrays were stained for CD4, CD8, FOXP3, CD32b, CD14, CD68, CD70, SIRP-α, TIM3, PD-1, and PD-L1. Early failure was defined as failing to achieve event-free survival at 24 months (EFS24) in immunochemotherapy-treated patients and EFS12 in all others. CyTOF and CODEX analysis were performed to characterize intratumoral immunophenotypes. Lack of intrafollicular CD4 expression was the only predictor of early failure that replicated with a pooled OR 2.37 (95%CI 1.48–3.79). We next developed a bio-clinical risk model (BioFLIPI), where lack of CD4 intrafollicular expression moved patients up one FLIPI risk group, adding a new fourth high-risk group. Compared with BioFLIPI score of 1, patients with a score of 2 (OR 2.17; 95% CI 1.08–4.69), 3 (OR 3.53; 95% CI 1.78–7.54), and 4 (OR 8.92; 95% CI 4.00–21.1) had increasing risk of early failure. The favorable intrafollicular CD4 T cells were identified as activated central memory T cells, whose prognostic value was independent from genetic features. In conclusion, lack of intrafollicular CD4 expression predicts early failure in FL and combined with FLIPI improves identification of high-risk patients; however, independent validation is warranted.Subject terms: Cancer microenvironment, CD4-positive T cells  相似文献   
995.
The biological role of monocytes and macrophages in B-cell non-Hodgkin lymphoma (NHL) is not fully understood. We have previously reported that monocytes from patients with B-cell NHL have an immunosuppressive CD14+HLA-DRlow/− phenotype that correlates with a poor prognosis. However, the underlying mechanism by which CD14+HLA-DRlow/− monocytes develop in lymphoma is unknown. In the present study, we found that interleukin (IL)-10, which is increased in the serum of patients with B-cell NHL, induced the development of the CD4+HLA-DRlow/− population. Using peripheral blood samples from patients with B-cell NHL, we found that absolute numbers of CD14+ monocytic cells with an HLA-DRlow/− phenotype were higher than healthy controls and correlated with a higher International Prognostic Index score. IL-10 serum levels were elevated in lymphoma patients compared with controls and were associated with increased peripheral monocyte counts. Treatment of monocytes with IL-10 in vitro significantly decreased HLA-DR expression and resulted in the expansion of CD14+HLA-DRlow/− population. We found that lymphoma B cells produce IL-10 and supernatants from cultured lymphoma cells increased the CD14+HLA-DRlow/− population. Furthermore, we found that IL-10-induced CD14+HLA-DRlow/− monocytes inhibited the activation and proliferation of T cells. Taken together, these results suggest that elevated IL-10 serum levels contribute to increased numbers of immunosuppressive CD14+HLA-DRlow/− monocytes in B-cell NHL.  相似文献   
996.
997.
998.
999.
1000.
Early caregiver–infant interactions are critical for infants’ socioemotional and cognitive development. Several hormones and neuromodulators, including oxytocin, affect these interactions. Exogenous oxytocin promotes social behaviors in several species, including human and nonhuman primates. Although exogenous oxytocin increases social function in adults—including expression recognition and affiliation—it is unknown whether oxytocin can increase social interactions in infants. We hypothesized that nebulized oxytocin would increase affiliative social behaviors and such effects would be modulated by infants’ social skills, measured earlier in development. We also hypothesized that oxytocin’s effects on social behaviors may be due to its anxiolytic effects. We tested these hypotheses in a blind study by nebulizing 7- to 14-d-old macaques (n = 28) with oxytocin or saline. Following oxytocin administration, infants’ facial gesturing at a human caregiver increased, and infants’ salivary oxytocin was positively correlated with the time spent in close proximity to a caregiver. Infants’ imitative skill (measured earlier in development: 1–7 d of age) predicted oxytocin-associated increases in affiliative behaviors—lip smacking, visual attention to a caregiver, and time in close proximity to a caregiver—suggesting that infants with higher propensities for positive social interactions are more sensitive to exogenous oxytocin. Oxytocin also decreased salivary cortisol, but not stress-related behaviors (e.g., scratching), suggesting the possibility of some anxiolytic effects. To our knowledge, this study provides the first evidence that oxytocin increases positive social behaviors in newborns. This information is of critical importance for potential interventions aimed at ameliorating inadequate social behaviors in infants with higher likelihood of developing neurodevelopmental disorder.Oxytocin is a neuropeptide that has wide-ranging effects on social behaviors and social perception, including increased emotion recognition and prosocial behavior (1, 2). Animal studies present convergent evidence of oxytocin’s positive effects on social behavior (26), including humans (1, 7). In recent years, an increasing literature on human and nonhuman primates suggests an association between oxytocin levels—either endogenous or exogenously administered—and prosocial behaviors (810). In both humans and macaques, exogenous oxytocin appears to enhance social attention, prosocial behaviors, sensitivity to gaze, and sensitivity to facial expressions (for reviews, see refs. 1 and 2).Oxytocin, therefore, may be a tool for promoting social behaviors, especially in clinical populations in which social faculties are compromised (8, 1113). In the last few years, in fact, oxytocin has been tested in autistic individuals, and it appears to increase social attention and improve emotion recognition (e.g., refs. 1419; although see ref. 20; for a recent review, see ref. 21). Given the importance of early assessments in the diagnosis of autism (22), studies clarifying the role of oxytocin in early development are critically important. For example, human infants actively participate in face-to-face caregiver–infant interactions; failure to engage with caregivers in this way can disrupt the development of healthy emotion regulation and socioemotional skills (2325). In both caregivers and neonates, complex cortical and limbic brain networks are prepared to sustain such exchanges (2628), and several hormones and neuromodulators regulate the affective components of face-to-face caregiver–infant interactions (2932). However, to our knowledge, studies investigating the role of infants’ oxytocin levels in these early intersubjective exchanges have not been carried out. Only one study to date measured endogenous oxytocin levels in newborns and reported that higher levels of oxytocin in newborns’ cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) were associated with higher levels of social engagement, including actively seeking parental social interaction for soothing and a greater interest in social interaction (33). No studies to date, however, have administered oxytocin to infants to determine its effects on social behavior, despite the fact that a more thorough understanding of oxytocin and its behavioral consequences may provide a potential tool for interventions aimed at promoting social affiliation in individuals with social impairments (1115, 34). The necessity to fill this gap motivated the present study.Our first goal was to determine whether oxytocin influences newborn macaques’ behaviors during an interaction with a human caregiver. We predicted that oxytocin, compared with saline, would increase positive social behavior, including facial gestures [i.e., lip smacking (LPS) and tongue protrusion (TP)], visual attention to a human caregiver, and time spent in close proximity to a human caregiver (35). As adults, macaques display positive behavioral changes in response to exogenous oxytocin (26), as in humans (1).

Table 1.

Ethogram for 12 behaviors scored during imitation recognition
BehaviorOperational definition
EventsLPSLip smacking. Rapid opening and closing of the mouth
TPProtrusion and retraction of the tongue
StatesVis attnVisual attention. Looking at the face of the human caregiver model
ProxProximity. Infant torso is within 5 cm (infant arm’s reach) from cage front
EventsScratchCommon use
YawnCommon use
StatesSelf-suckInsertion into mouth of fingers/hands, toes/feet
Self-claspHand or foot closed on fur or some body part
SurrogateAny touching of surrogate mother
LocoLocomotion. Directed movement of torso (>15 cm within 5 s)
ExploreExploration. Manipulating toys or bedding
SleepInfant lying down with head on floor of cage
Open in a separate windowAn additional motivation for the present study was to examine individual differences in sensitivity to oxytocin. We predicted that individual differences in infants’ social skills might moderate the effects of oxytocin. In particular, in the first week of life, macaques, like humans, imitate facial gestures (36); this response reflects the emergence of infants’ early social skills in tuning their own behavior with that of their mothers (36). Despite large individual differences in imitative ability (37), the neurochemical mechanism mediating these responses remains unknown. Early imitative abilities are associated with some aspects of later social cognitive development (3739) and may reflect general social interest (for a review, see ref. 40). For example, macaque infants who consistently imitate in the first week of life, compared with those who do not, are better at recognizing human caregivers (38) and visually attend more to caregivers (39). Together, these lines of evidence suggest that the capacity to imitate at birth is associated with a range of social-cognitive skills, and that the interindividual differences in such skills may rely on neurobiological substrates mediated by oxytocin. Given that infants may vary in their social interest, and that oxytocin may enhance intrinsic social motivation (2, 4), we predicted infants’ imitative skill—a measure of social interest—may predict their sensitivity to exogenous oxytocin.A final motivation was to assess infants’ salivary oxytocin and cortisol levels, to determine the influence of inhaled oxytocin. Other studies report that administering oxytocin results in a dose-dependent decrease in plasma cortisol (41) and reduces anxiety, which increases affiliative motivation (42). We predicted that inhaled oxytocin would increase infants’ salivary oxytocin and decrease salivary cortisol. We also measured anxiolytic effects behaviorally by examining self-directed behaviors that have been associated with stress (43), including scratching, yawning, self-sucking, self-clinging, and interactions with the surrogate (SI Methods). In the second week of life, we carried out a procedure on 2 consecutive days, in which infants were nebulized with oxytocin or saline (one per day). One and 2 h following nebulization, infants were tested in an imitation recognition task in which a human experimenter imitated all of an infant’s mouth movements for 2 min, followed by 2 min of still face (i.e., neutral face), while trying to maintain eye contact with the infant. This paradigm was selected because of previous findings that monkeys recognize when they are being imitated (44) and display affiliation toward social partners who imitate them (45). We collected saliva samples 2 and 4 h after the end of nebulization to measure salivary oxytocin and cortisol levels (see SI Methods for details).  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号