首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4820篇
  免费   184篇
  国内免费   41篇
耳鼻咽喉   115篇
儿科学   70篇
妇产科学   64篇
基础医学   561篇
口腔科学   88篇
临床医学   364篇
内科学   1591篇
皮肤病学   34篇
神经病学   338篇
特种医学   98篇
外科学   799篇
综合类   11篇
预防医学   165篇
眼科学   64篇
药学   298篇
中国医学   5篇
肿瘤学   380篇
  2023年   31篇
  2022年   72篇
  2021年   145篇
  2020年   88篇
  2019年   112篇
  2018年   145篇
  2017年   90篇
  2016年   103篇
  2015年   106篇
  2014年   152篇
  2013年   199篇
  2012年   344篇
  2011年   323篇
  2010年   203篇
  2009年   193篇
  2008年   282篇
  2007年   322篇
  2006年   314篇
  2005年   320篇
  2004年   267篇
  2003年   253篇
  2002年   223篇
  2001年   47篇
  2000年   61篇
  1999年   62篇
  1998年   49篇
  1997年   29篇
  1996年   32篇
  1995年   15篇
  1994年   28篇
  1993年   16篇
  1992年   42篇
  1991年   18篇
  1990年   19篇
  1989年   25篇
  1988年   21篇
  1987年   13篇
  1986年   24篇
  1985年   25篇
  1984年   21篇
  1983年   26篇
  1982年   14篇
  1981年   15篇
  1979年   17篇
  1978年   15篇
  1977年   17篇
  1976年   13篇
  1975年   11篇
  1974年   19篇
  1973年   10篇
排序方式: 共有5045条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
992.
Giving the impact of complete response (CR) on outcome of multiple myeloma patients addressed to high-dose melphalan, we explored the role of a pre-transplant intensification with 3 months thalidome plus dexamethasone therapy (Thal-Dex), after pulse-VAD induction. Seventy-four multiple myeloma patients (MM pts) uniformly treated, were retrospectively studied. The response rate after pulse-VAD were: CR 6%, VGPR 40%, PR 23%, MR 23%, and progression 8%. The response rate after Thal-Dex were similar: CR 11%, VGPR 39%, PR 17%, MR 9%, and progression 24%. Giving no advantage in terms of response rate with an additive toxicity, Thal-Dex does not seem useful for intensification before transplant.  相似文献   
993.
994.
995.
996.
997.
A century-long debate on bodily states and emotions persists. While the involvement of bodily activity in emotion physiology is widely recognized, the specificity and causal role of such activity related to brain dynamics has not yet been demonstrated. We hypothesize that the peripheral neural control on cardiovascular activity prompts and sustains brain dynamics during an emotional experience, so these afferent inputs are processed by the brain by triggering a concurrent efferent information transfer to the body. To this end, we investigated the functional brain–heart interplay under emotion elicitation in publicly available data from 62 healthy subjects using a computational model based on synthetic data generation of electroencephalography and electrocardiography signals. Our findings show that sympathovagal activity plays a leading and causal role in initiating the emotional response, in which ascending modulations from vagal activity precede neural dynamics and correlate to the reported level of arousal. The subsequent dynamic interplay observed between the central and autonomic nervous systems sustains the processing of emotional arousal. These findings should be particularly revealing for the psychophysiology and neuroscience of emotions.

“What Is an Emotion?” by William James (1), published more than a century ago, started the scientific debate on the nature of emotions. However, a shared and definitive theory of emotions is not in place yet, and the very definition of emotions and their nature is still a matter of debate. While more “classical” theories point to emotions as “the functional states of the brain that provide causal explanations of certain complex behaviors—like evading a predator or attacking prey” (2), other theories suggest how they are constructions of the world, not reactions to it (3). Namely, emotions are internal states constructed on the basis of previous experiences as predictive schemes to react to external stimuli.The role of bodily activity in emotions is often questioned. Despite the vast literature showing bodily correlates with emotions, a long-lasting debate about the relationship between bodily states and emotions persists (4). For instance, a feeling is defined as the subjective metarepresentation and labeling of physiological changes (such as an increase in heart rate, the increase of blood pressure, or changes in peristalsis) (5) that are strictly related to the body state on the one hand and to emotions on the other. To this extent, emotions are complex psychological phenomena in which feelings are interpreted and labeled. In a particular psychopathological condition known as alexithymia, individuals experience difficulties in experiencing and understanding emotions to various degrees (6). Indeed, some of these patients can perceive the physical changes connected to a feeling but are unable to label it as emotion, so that emotional experience is described only as its physical counterpart [e.g., described an experience as “I have my heart beating too fast” instead of “I’m fearful” (7)]. From a biological point of view the way in which physical changes become feelings and emotions is based on the interplay between the central and the autonomic nervous systems.The central nervous system (CNS) communicates with the autonomic nervous system (ANS) through interoceptive neural circuits that contribute to physiological functions beyond homeostatic control, from the emotional experience and the genesis of feelings (8) to decision making (9, 10). The debate about the role of the ANS in emotions can be condensed into two views: specificity or causation (4). The specificity view is related to the James–Lange theory, which states that bodily responses precede emotions’ central processing, meaning that bodily states would be a response to the environment, followed by an interpretation carried out by the CNS that would result in the feeling felt. However, causation theories represent an updated view of the James–Lange theory, suggesting that peripheral changes influence the conscious emotional experience; from a biological point of view this may reflect the fact that autonomic nervous signals from the body do influence perceptual activity in the brain (11, 12). In this regard, subjective perception may be influenced or shaped by ascending communication from visceral inputs to the brain (1315).Functional models of CNS and ANS interplay have described bidirectional dynamics in emotions (1618). In particular, the functional brain–heart interplay (BHI) involves brain structures that comprise the central autonomic network (CAN), which has been described as being in charge of autonomic control (19, 20). Moreover, the default mode network (DMN) has been found to be involved in autonomic control (21) and tasks of self-related cognition and interoception (22, 23), suggesting that the DMN participates in both ascending and descending communications with the heart. Finally, the constructed emotion theory suggests how DMN together with other intrinsic networks is crucial in the genesis of emotion and emotional experience (3).Psychophysiological studies have uncovered several correlates of different autonomic signals in the brain during emotional experiences (2427). To understand these correlations and the functional interactions between the heart and brain, various signal processing methods have been proposed to investigate functional BHI through noninvasive recordings (28). The study of emotions using these methods comprises the analysis of heartbeat-evoked potentials (29), nonlinear couplings (30), and information transfer modeling (31). However, the causative role of bodily inputs remains unknown (4) and, more specifically, the temporal and causal links between cortical and peripheral neural dynamics in both ascending and descending directions, i.e., from the brain to the body and from the body to the brain, are still to be clarified.In this study, we take a step forward in answering these scientific questions and investigate whether peripheral neural dynamics play a causal role in the genesis of emotions. We applied a mathematical model of functional BHI based on synthetic data generation (SDG) (32), estimating the directionality of the functional interplay using simultaneous electroencephalography (EEG) and electrocardiography (ECG) recordings gathered from healthy subjects undergoing emotion elicitations with video clips, the publicly available DEAP and MAHNOB datasets (33, 34). ECG series were analyzed to derive heart-rate variability (HRV) series, which result from the concurrent activity of the sympathetic and parasympathetic (vagal) branches of the ANS acting to regulate the heartbeat. We hypothesize that, from a neurobiological point of view, feelings and subsequent emotional experiences arise from the mutual interplay between brain and body, particularly in which the CNS integrates the afferent ANS information outflow, namely from-heart-to-brain interplay, which actually triggers a cascade of cortical neural activations that, in turn, modulate directed neural control onto the heart, namely from brain-to-heart interplay.  相似文献   
998.
Patients with non metastatic squamous cell lung cancer were treated with radiotherapy (RT) plus lonidamine (LND) or placebo (PLAC), according to a randomized double-blind study design.

Treatment with lonidamine 150 mg t.i.d (27 patients) or placebo (23 patients) started 3 days before RT,lasted up to 7 months. Partial responses were observed in 14 and 6 patients respectively in the LND+RT and PLAC+RT groups. Statistical analysis of the survival curves showed no significant difference between the LND+RT (median 311 days) and PLAC+RT (median 193 days) groups.

Stage III patients survived significantly longer (p<0.05) when treated with LND+RT (median 318 days) than with PLAC+RT (median 163 days). No synergistic toxic effects between radiation and LND were noted.

To confirm these data a new and larger multicentric study is now in progress.  相似文献   

999.
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号