首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   9189篇
  免费   683篇
  国内免费   44篇
耳鼻咽喉   73篇
儿科学   251篇
妇产科学   55篇
基础医学   1175篇
口腔科学   176篇
临床医学   950篇
内科学   1895篇
皮肤病学   147篇
神经病学   1202篇
特种医学   737篇
外科学   1603篇
综合类   47篇
一般理论   4篇
预防医学   437篇
眼科学   157篇
药学   420篇
中国医学   6篇
肿瘤学   581篇
  2024年   23篇
  2023年   129篇
  2022年   207篇
  2021年   387篇
  2020年   245篇
  2019年   288篇
  2018年   329篇
  2017年   291篇
  2016年   361篇
  2015年   353篇
  2014年   469篇
  2013年   555篇
  2012年   846篇
  2011年   834篇
  2010年   470篇
  2009年   394篇
  2008年   597篇
  2007年   588篇
  2006年   492篇
  2005年   377篇
  2004年   343篇
  2003年   295篇
  2002年   290篇
  2001年   95篇
  2000年   62篇
  1999年   66篇
  1998年   46篇
  1997年   21篇
  1996年   23篇
  1995年   19篇
  1994年   16篇
  1993年   18篇
  1992年   39篇
  1991年   28篇
  1990年   32篇
  1989年   27篇
  1988年   26篇
  1987年   26篇
  1986年   15篇
  1985年   20篇
  1984年   8篇
  1983年   12篇
  1982年   11篇
  1981年   8篇
  1979年   11篇
  1978年   14篇
  1976年   11篇
  1973年   10篇
  1972年   12篇
  1922年   14篇
排序方式: 共有9916条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
Human eyes serve two key functions in face-to-face social interactions: they provide cues about a person’s emotional state and attentional focus (gaze direction). Both functions critically rely on the morphologically unique human sclera and have been shown to operate even in the absence of conscious awareness in adults. However, it is not known whether the ability to respond to social cues from scleral information without conscious awareness exists early in human ontogeny and can therefore be considered a foundational feature of human social functioning. In the current study, we used event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to show that 7-mo-old infants discriminate between fearful and nonfearful eyes (experiment 1) and between direct and averted gaze (experiment 2), even when presented below the perceptual threshold. These effects were specific to the human sclera and not seen in response to polarity-inverted eyes. Our results suggest that early in ontogeny the human brain detects social cues from scleral information even in the absence of conscious awareness. The current findings support the view that the human eye with its prominent sclera serves critical communicative functions during human social interactions.Eyes play a key role in human social encounters, as they are critically involved in perceiving others as having minds (1), in attributing mental states to others (2), and in social coordination during face-to-face interactions (3). The presence of eyes has also been shown to increase cooperative behavior in laboratory and in real-world contexts (2, 47). The human eye is unique in that it is characterized by a prominent white sclera several times larger than that of other primates (8, 9), which allows for the efficient communication and detection of social information. It is thought that the human sclera is adapted to facilitate interpersonal communication and cooperative interactions among humans (10). When humans observe others’ faces, eyes are typically the first features that are scanned for information (11), and, compared with other primates, humans show a stronger focus on the eye region than on other parts of the face when scanning faces (12, 13). Conversely, failure to devote special attention to the eye region during face perception has been linked to severe social deficits that can, for instance, be observed in autism spectrum disorder (14).One reason why human eyes have such prime importance is that emotion perception, as a vital part of any social interaction, heavily relies on information from the eye region (14). This is especially important for the detection of fear in others, as one of the most basic forms of identifying threatening situations. Fear detection has been observed in response to eyes alone (15, 16). This mechanism operates exceptionally fast (17) and occurs irrespective of conscious awareness (15). On a neural level, the processing of fearful eyes critically depends on the amygdala. Depending on the context (18, 19), fearful eyes can elicit an enhanced activation of the amygdala (20), even if not perceived consciously (15). Patients with bilateral amygdala lesions show deficits in recognizing fear, which disappear when they are instructed to focus on the eye region (21). Furthermore, there is recent evidence to show that the amygdala is involved in reflexively directing attention to the eyes and in predicting gaze to fearful eyes (22, 23).Another important social cue conveyed by the eye is the direction of gaze. Eye gaze can inform us about another person’s attentional focus, thereby providing clues about future behavior (24). Critically, eye gaze and emotion perception have been shown to powerfully interact. For example, fearful eyes elicit stronger behavioral and neural responses when averted from than when directed at an observer (25, 26). This presumably relates to the fact that averted fearful eyes inform an observer about a potential danger in the environment (clear threat), whereas directed fearful eyes signal fear of the observer (ambiguous threat). At the brain level, this also relies on the amygdala, as reflected in a differential activation for direct compared with averted gaze (25, 27). Furthermore, behavioral studies suggest that similar to emotion processing, eye gaze discrimination operates even in the absence of conscious awareness (28).Attending and responding to eyes is thought to play a vital role in the early development of social skills (29). From birth, infants respond sensitively to human eyes: Newborns prefer direct gaze faces over averted gaze faces (30) and even show a rudimentary form of gaze following (31). Newborns’ sensitivity to eyes has been shown to be specific to the human sclera, as behavioral preferences disappear when the contrast polarity of the eye is reversed (32). Nevertheless, the ability to attend to the eyes and follow gaze improves considerably over the course of the first year of life and is viewed as an important marker of healthy social development (33, 34). Indeed, orienting to the eyes is present early in infancy but declines between 2 and 6 mo in infants later diagnosed with autism (29). With respect to responding to emotional information, newborns also show a basic sensitivity to (familiar) emotional facial expressions for which they may also use eye cues (30, 35, 36). However, it is not until the age of 7 mo that they show a robust attentional bias to fear, as reflected in their neural and behavioral responses (3741). The developmental emergence of this fear bias has been linked to the maturation of frontolimbic circuits (4245) and occurs at a point in development when infants begin to first experience fear themselves (46, 47). Despite our growing understanding of the developmental origins of emotion and gaze processing in humans, some fundamental questions concerning the exact nature of this ability remain unanswered.In the present study we therefore addressed two key questions, which are essential to understanding the mechanisms that underpin sensitive responding to human eyes in infants. First, we asked whether infants’ detection of social cues such as fear and gaze from eyes occurs in the absence of conscious awareness. Second, we examined whether the detection of these social cues can be seen in response to scleral information alone. To address these questions, we conducted two experiments, based on an established paradigm from adult literature (15), which investigated whether the infant brain can discriminate between fearful and nonfearful eyes (experiment 1) and between direct and averted fearful eyes (experiment 2), even if the stimuli are not perceived consciously. We hypothesized that if the eyes indeed serve a critical function in human social communication, then the unconscious detection of social cues from scleral information should be evident early in ontogeny. Specifically, according to our hypothesis, using event-related brain potentials (ERPs), we expected infants to show evidence for neural discrimination between fearful and nonfearful human eyes (experiment 1) and direct and averted gaze (experiment 2).  相似文献   
112.
113.
114.
115.

Aims

Only limited data exist about the role of point of care CYP2C19 testing in the acute setting in the early phase of acute coronary syndromes (ACS). Therefore, the present study was designed to investigate the impact of CYP2C19 loss-of–function point-of-care (POC) genotyping in patients presenting with acute coronary syndromes (ACS) and treated with dual antiplatelet therapy in the emergency setting.

Methods and Results

137 subjects with ACS scheduled for percutaneous coronary intervention were consecutively enrolled. Pre- and on-treatment platelet aggregation was assessed by multiple electrode aggregometry (MEA) after stimulation with adenosine diphosphate (ADP). Patients were loaded according to current guideline adherent indications and contraindications for use of P2Y12 inhibitors in ACS. POC genotyping for CYP2C19*2 was performed in the emergency room after obtaining a buccal swab using the Spartan RX CYP2C19 system and obtaining patient’s informed consent. Prasugrel and ticagrelor treated patients had significantly lower PR compared to clopidogrel-treated patients. The benefits of prasugrel and ticagrelor compared to clopidogrel treated patients in terms of platelet inhibition were more pronounced in CYP2C19*2 carriers. Non-carriers showed similar inhibition regardless of particular P2Y12 inhibitor treatment. Statistical analyses adjusting for factors associated with response (e.g. smoking) revealed that CYP2C19*2 allele carrier status and loading with different type of P2Y12 receptor blockers were significant predictors of on-treatment platelet reactivity in the early phase of ACS.

Conclusion

The results of this pilot study of treatment of patients in the early phase of ACS indicate that CYP2C19*2 POC genotyping might help to identify patients at risk with poor response to clopidogrel treatment, thereby benefiting from reloading and switching to alternative P2Y12 receptor inhibition.  相似文献   
116.
117.
118.
119.
120.
We have used X-ray diffraction on the rhombohedral phospholipid phase to reconstruct stalk structures in different pure lipids and lipid mixtures with unprecedented resolution, enabling a quantitative analysis of geometry, as well as curvature and hydration energies. Electron density isosurfaces are used to study shape and curvature properties of the bent lipid monolayers. We observe that the stalk structure is highly universal in different lipid systems. The associated curvatures change in a subtle, but systematic fashion upon changes in lipid composition. In addition, we have studied the hydration interaction prior to the transition from the lamellar to the stalk phase. The results indicate that facilitating dehydration is the key to promote stalk formation, which becomes favorable at an approximately constant interbilayer separation of 9.0 ± 0.5  for the investigated lipid compositions.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号