首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1421篇
  免费   104篇
  国内免费   3篇
耳鼻咽喉   38篇
儿科学   30篇
妇产科学   34篇
基础医学   225篇
口腔科学   62篇
临床医学   132篇
内科学   375篇
皮肤病学   33篇
神经病学   131篇
特种医学   31篇
外科学   155篇
综合类   2篇
一般理论   1篇
预防医学   107篇
眼科学   11篇
药学   83篇
中国医学   4篇
肿瘤学   74篇
  2024年   3篇
  2023年   13篇
  2022年   29篇
  2021年   54篇
  2020年   29篇
  2019年   56篇
  2018年   58篇
  2017年   42篇
  2016年   49篇
  2015年   52篇
  2014年   45篇
  2013年   74篇
  2012年   125篇
  2011年   153篇
  2010年   61篇
  2009年   71篇
  2008年   89篇
  2007年   97篇
  2006年   64篇
  2005年   81篇
  2004年   58篇
  2003年   58篇
  2002年   53篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   10篇
  1999年   9篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   4篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   5篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   2篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   3篇
  1969年   1篇
  1967年   2篇
排序方式: 共有1528条查询结果,搜索用时 26 毫秒
71.
72.
OBJECTIVE: Osteoprotegerin (OPG) is an inhibitor of osteoclastogenesis, which has been recently involved in atherosclerosis. The relationship between coronary atherosclerosis and OPG has never been studied in asymptomatic type 2 diabetic patients. RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS: This is a nested case-control study; 162 asymptomatic type 2 diabetic patients were evaluated for silent myocardial ischemia using stress myocardial perfusion imaging; of 50 patients with positive results, 37 underwent coronary angiography, 20 of whom showed significant coronary artery disease (CAD group). Of 112 patients without silent myocardial ischemia, 20 subjects (NO-CAD group) were selected and matched by age and sex to patients with CAD. OPG, C-reactive protein, adiponectin, lipoprotein(a), albuminuria, and classical risk factors were measured. RESULTS: The percentages of subjects with OPG levels above median and with nephropathy were higher in the CAD group than in the NO-CAD group (70 vs. 25%, P = 0.004 and 50 vs. 5%, P = 0.001, respectively). LDL cholesterol levels were higher and HDL cholesterol levels lower in the CAD compared with the NO-CAD group (P = 0.033 and P = 0.005, respectively). No other variables were associated with CAD. Logistic regression analysis showed that OPG values above median (odds ratio 8.31 [95% CI 1.18-58.68], P = 0.034) and nephropathy (21.98 [1.24-388.36], P = 0.035) were significant independent predictors of asymptomatic CAD in type 2 diabetic patients. CONCLUSIONS: Our investigation reports the first evidence of an independent association of OPG with asymptomatic CAD in type 2 diabetic patients. The results of this nested case- control study with 20 cases need to be confirmed in a larger population.  相似文献   
73.
The physical characterization of active pharmaceutical substances is crucial to the successful development of the final drug product. The different solid forms and variations in the degree of crystallinity can lead to significantly different physical and chemical properties, including color, morphology, stability, dissolution and bioavailability. In the case of omeprazole sodium (OMS), its chemical structures contain a specific number of water molecules (hydrate). The behavior of pharmaceutical hydrates has become the object of increasing attention over the past decade, primarily due to the potential impact of hydrates on the development process and dosage form performance. The present study was designed to characterize and evaluate the crystallinity of omeprazole sodium, dehydrated omeprazole sodium (DOMS) and omeprazole free base (OM) using a variety of techniques including thermal analysis (thermogravimetry/derivative thermogravimetry (TG/DTG) and differential scanning calorimetry (DSC)), diffuse reflectance infrared Fourier transform (DRIFT) spectroscopy, scanning electron microscopy (SEM) and X-ray powder diffraction (XRPD). Furthermore, an NMR spectroscopy study was also carried out to clarify the conformation and crystal structure.  相似文献   
74.
75.
Adults can learn to identify nonnative speech sounds with training, albeit with substantial variability in learning behavior. Increases in behavioral accuracy are associated with increased separability for sound representations in cortical speech areas. However, it remains unclear whether individual auditory neural populations all show the same types of changes with learning, or whether there are heterogeneous encoding patterns. Here, we used high-resolution direct neural recordings to examine local population response patterns, while native English listeners learned to recognize unfamiliar vocal pitch patterns in Mandarin Chinese tones. We found a distributed set of neural populations in bilateral superior temporal gyrus and ventrolateral frontal cortex, where the encoding of Mandarin tones changed throughout training as a function of trial-by-trial accuracy (“learning effect”), including both increases and decreases in the separability of tones. These populations were distinct from populations that showed changes as a function of exposure to the stimuli regardless of trial-by-trial accuracy. These learning effects were driven in part by more variable neural responses to repeated presentations of acoustically identical stimuli. Finally, learning effects could be predicted from speech-evoked activity even before training, suggesting that intrinsic properties of these populations make them amenable to behavior-related changes. Together, these results demonstrate that nonnative speech sound learning involves a wide array of changes in neural representations across a distributed set of brain regions.

Humans are finely attuned to the sounds in their native language (1, 2), driven by extensive experience hearing these sounds in many different contexts from different speakers (35). However, for nonnative sounds in unfamiliar languages, adult listeners often struggle to learn to recognize relatively simple contrasts (69). For example, although native English listeners understand how changes in vocal pitch indicate intonational prosody (e.g., statements versus questions; refs. 10 and 11), this does not translate to the ability to easily identify the syllable-level pitch patterns that define lexical tones in Mandarin Chinese (12, 13). Fundamentally, this difficulty may reflect a trade-off between maintaining stable representations of deeply engrained speech sounds and retaining enough plasticity to be able to continue to learn behaviorally relevant information throughout the lifespan (1419). Learning to identify nonnative speech sounds often requires long and intense periods of active training (12, 2022), consistent with the observation that speech circuits in the human brain are resistant to change following developmental critical periods (15, 17).However, even brief training periods can lead to an increased ability to identify novel speech sounds, albeit with highly variable performance across individuals (2325). Behavioral evidence has further shown that the way listeners perceive relevant auditory cues changes after speech training (14, 2527), which has led to the hypothesis that learning is rooted in more distinct neural representations of those sounds (17, 27). Consistent with this hypothesis, previous neuroimaging studies have shown that activation in frontotemporal areas increases following identification or discrimination tasks (13, 19, 2830). These increases in the magnitude of activation are further associated with greater neural separability among sound categories for both speech (3133) and nonspeech sounds (3436). However, recent evidence suggests a highly diverse set of speech representations even within areas like the superior temporal gyrus (STG; ref. 37). Currently, the extent to which learning-related changes vary at the level of local populations remains unknown due to the broad spatial scale of noninvasive methods, which may obscure more complex dynamics. In addition, it is unclear how learning-related changes evolve on a trial-by-trial basis, as listeners initially learn to use the stimulus dimensions that allow them to achieve increased accuracy on the task, since most previous work examines neural activity only at early and late stages of the task.Here, we examined the relationship between behavioral performance during the initial stage of nonnative speech sound learning and the trial-by-trial encoding of speech content in local neural populations in the human brain. English-speaking participants listened to unfamiliar Mandarin syllables and learned to identify tone categories (22, 38), while neural activity was recorded from electrocorticography (ECoG) arrays placed over lateral cortical areas. We hypothesized that, as listeners heard the same stimuli across multiple exposures, some neural populations would show responses to Mandarin speech sounds that track the trial-by-trial fluctuations in participants’ behavioral performance during learning, and we also asked whether these changes would be uniformly reflected in increased separability among tones. We further hypothesized that these learning-related neural populations would be distinct from other potential patterns of change across trials that do not directly correlate with learning (e.g., the number of exposures to a given token, independent from accuracy) and neural populations that show stable activity patterns across trials. To address the relationship to stimulus feature encoding (e.g., pitch representations for English intonational prosody; ref. 39), we also measured the extent to which neural responses to unfamiliar Mandarin speech sounds prior to training can be used to predict the emergence of learning-related changes during training.We found a subset of local populations across the cortical surface that track trial-by-trial accuracy, even when learning performance is relatively low and variable. These learning-related effects manifest as both increases and decreases in the amplitude of neural responses to specific speech sounds and are spatially interspersed and dissociable from those that arise simply as a function of repeated exposure. Furthermore, learning-related changes are associated with higher variability of the response amplitude across repeated exposures to the same acoustic stimulus, suggesting less robust emergent neural representations. Finally, we show that intrinsic properties of these neural populations are associated with whether they show learning-related effects during training, allowing us to predict whether these effects will occur based on responses to the novel speech sounds prior to training. Together, these results demonstrate that learning to identify novel speech sounds scaffolds on existing sensitivities to relevant features and that the initial stages of learning a new language involve a specific set of processes to fine-tune local speech representations in the brain. We propose that the learning-induced increased neural separability in frontotemporal regions arises from heterogeneous changes among local populations, which comprise those regions.  相似文献   
76.
Objective: In a clinical setting, theories of health behaviour change could help audiologists and other hearing health care professionals understand the barriers that prevent people with hearing problems to seek audiological help. The transtheoretical (stages of change) model of health behaviour change is one of these theories. It describes a person’s journey towards health behaviour change (e.g. seeking help or taking up rehabilitation) in separate stages: precontemplation, contemplation, preparation, action, and, finally, maintenance. A short self-assessment measure of stages of change may guide the clinician and facilitate first appointments. This article describes correlations between three stages of change measures of different lengths, one 24-item and two one-item. Design: Participants were recruited through an online hearing screening study. Adults who failed the speech-in-noise recognition screening test and who had never undergone a hearing aid fitting were invited to complete further questionnaires online, including the three stages of change measures. Study sample: In total, 224 adults completed the three measures. Results: A majority of the participants were categorised as being in one of the information- and help-seeking stage of change (contemplation or preparation). The three stages of change measures were significantly correlated. Conclusions Our results support further investigating the use of a one-item measure to determine stages of change in people with hearing impairment.  相似文献   
77.
78.
79.
80.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号