首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2851篇
  免费   206篇
  国内免费   21篇
耳鼻咽喉   13篇
儿科学   130篇
妇产科学   109篇
基础医学   342篇
口腔科学   46篇
临床医学   320篇
内科学   559篇
皮肤病学   26篇
神经病学   225篇
特种医学   108篇
外科学   390篇
综合类   56篇
一般理论   4篇
预防医学   418篇
眼科学   21篇
药学   150篇
中国医学   1篇
肿瘤学   160篇
  2022年   21篇
  2021年   41篇
  2020年   16篇
  2019年   31篇
  2018年   35篇
  2017年   30篇
  2016年   42篇
  2015年   59篇
  2014年   72篇
  2013年   103篇
  2012年   133篇
  2011年   120篇
  2010年   75篇
  2009年   91篇
  2008年   119篇
  2007年   133篇
  2006年   135篇
  2005年   148篇
  2004年   140篇
  2003年   118篇
  2002年   118篇
  2001年   72篇
  2000年   69篇
  1999年   80篇
  1998年   40篇
  1997年   49篇
  1996年   45篇
  1995年   48篇
  1994年   38篇
  1993年   33篇
  1992年   57篇
  1991年   50篇
  1990年   38篇
  1989年   59篇
  1988年   47篇
  1987年   58篇
  1986年   45篇
  1985年   54篇
  1984年   35篇
  1983年   46篇
  1982年   18篇
  1981年   26篇
  1980年   24篇
  1979年   24篇
  1978年   31篇
  1977年   17篇
  1976年   21篇
  1974年   18篇
  1973年   17篇
  1969年   18篇
排序方式: 共有3078条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Objectives. We evaluated a Social Branding antitobacco intervention for “hipster” young adults that was implemented between 2008 and 2011 in San Diego, California.Methods. We conducted repeated cross-sectional surveys of random samples of young adults going to bars at baseline and over a 3-year follow-up. We used multinomial logistic regression to evaluate changes in daily smoking, nondaily smoking, and binge drinking, controlling for demographic characteristics, alcohol use, advertising receptivity, trend sensitivity, and tobacco-related attitudes.Results. During the intervention, current (past 30 day) smoking decreased from 57% (baseline) to 48% (at follow-up 3; P = .002), and daily smoking decreased from 22% to 15% (P < .001). There were significant interactions between hipster affiliation and alcohol use on smoking. Among hipster binge drinkers, the odds of daily smoking (odds ratio [OR] = 0.44; 95% confidence interval [CI] = 0.30, 0.63) and nondaily smoking (OR = 0.57; 95% CI = 0.42, 0.77) decreased significantly at follow-up 3. Binge drinking also decreased significantly at follow-up 3 (OR = 0.64; 95% CI = 0.53, 0.78).Conclusions. Social Branding campaigns are a promising strategy to decrease smoking in young adult bar patrons.Tobacco companies1 and public health authorities2–5 recognize young adulthood as a critical time when experimenters either quit or transition to regular tobacco use. Young adults are also aspirational role models for youths.1,6,7 Tobacco companies devote considerable resources to reaching young adults to encourage tobacco use,1,8–11 and young adults have a high prevalence of smoking.12 In California in 2011, young adults had the highest smoking prevalence of any age group, and the Department of Health estimated that 32% of California smokers started smoking between the ages of 18 and 26 years.13 Although they are more likely to intend to quit and successfully quit than older adults,14–17 young adults are less likely to receive assistance with smoking cessation.18,19 Although there are few proven interventions to discourage young adult smoking,20 cessation before age 30 years avoids virtually all of the long-term adverse health effects of smoking.21Tobacco companies have a long history of using bars and nightclubs to reach young adults and to encourage smoking.1,6,9–11,22–24 Bar attendance and exposure to tobacco bar marketing is strongly associated with smoking.25 The 1998 Master Settlement Agreement and Food and Drug Administration regulations that limit tobacco advertising to youths, explicitly permit tobacco marketing in “adult only” venues, including bars and nightclubs.26,27Aggressive tobacco marketing may actually be more intensive in smoke-free bars: a 2010 study of college students attending bars found that students in the community with a smoke-free bar law were more likely to be approached by tobacco marketers, offered free gifts, and to take free gifts for themselves than in communities without a smoke-free bar law.28 Bars and nightclubs also attract young adults who are more likely to exhibit personality traits such as sensation seeking,29 increasing their risk30 independently of receptivity to tobacco advertising; tobacco promotional messages resonate with these personality traits.8,31 Tobacco marketing campaigns are tailored to specific segments of the population defined by psychographics (e.g., values, attitudes, shared interests, such as tastes in music and fashion, and friend groups) and demographic criteria, and they aim to create positive smoker images, identities, and social norms for smoking.1,8 Tobacco marketing campaigns also focus on young adult trendsetters to leverage peer influence to promote smoking.6,10In contrast to the tobacco companies’ efforts, most young adult health interventions take place in colleges or health centers rather than social environments.32–39 Bars and nightclub venues represent an opportunity to reach those at highest risk for long-term smoking morbidity and mortality.40 We evaluated the effectiveness of an intervention to decrease cigarette smoking by countering tobacco industry marketing strategies targeting young adults attending bars and nightclubs in the San Diego, California, “hipster” scene. Because tobacco and alcohol use are strongly linked,41,42 we also examined the effects of the intervention on alcohol use and among binge drinkers. We found a significant decrease in smoking in the community where the intervention took place, including significant decreases among nondaily smokers and binge drinkers, as well as a significant decrease in binge drinking.  相似文献   
82.
In its graphic warning label regulations on cigarette packages, the Food and Drug Administration severely discounts the benefits of reduced smoking because of the lost “pleasure” smokers experience when they stop smoking; this is quantified as lost “consumer surplus.” Consumer surplus is grounded in rational choice theory. However, empirical evidence from psychological cognitive science and behavioral economics demonstrates that the assumptions of rational choice are inconsistent with complex multidimensional decisions, particularly smoking. Rational choice does not account for the roles of emotions, misperceptions, optimistic bias, regret, and cognitive inefficiency that are germane to smoking, particularly because most smokers begin smoking in their youth. Continued application of a consumer surplus discount will undermine sensible policies to reduce tobacco use and other policies to promote public health.The 2009 Family Smoking Prevention and Tobacco Control Act (HR 1256, 2009) required the United States Food and Drug Administration (FDA) to issue a regulation requiring cigarette companies to place large graphic warnings on all cigarette packages. As part of the process of issuing this regulation, the FDA conducted a cost–benefit analysis of the graphic warning label regulation.1 In its analysis, the FDA estimated the benefits of graphic warning labels, including reduced tobacco-induced illness and premature death, then cut the estimated benefits of these warning labels in half to account for the cost of lost “pleasure” smokers incurred as a result of quitting (and lost pleasure would-be smokers would never experience) because of the new warning labels. The FDA quantified the cost of this lost pleasure using the economic concept of “consumer surplus,” which is the difference between what a utility maximizing individual would be willing to pay and the actual price.2–6Because of the extent that smokers are willing to pay more for cigarettes than their monetary cost, this willingness to pay more is an indication that smokers obtain a surplus benefit of smoking beyond the cost of the cigarettes. The FDA justified applying a large discount to the estimated health benefits of the warning labels, stating,
The concept of consumer surplus is a basic tool of welfare economics… . In an analysis of benefits based on willingness-to-pay, we cannot reject this tool and still fulfill our obligation to conduct a full and an objective economic analysis.1(p36714)
Consumer surplus based on willingness to pay is a well-established concept in classical economics and is grounded in rational choice theory, a normative model of human decision-making.7 Rational choice theory represents human decision-making at its most logical, when decisions are the result of careful cost–benefit analysis, with people choosing the option that maximizes the utility of the choice after subtracting perceived costs.8–10When applied to smoking, this theory posits that smokers (and potential smokers) smoke because they computed that the current and future benefits of the pleasures of smoking outweigh the present value of future financial, social, and medical costs of smoking.11–13 These benefits may include both the physiologic responses and emotional or social advantages (either real or imagined) that smoking provides.By contrast, a large body of empirical evidence from cognitive behavioral sciences demonstrates that smokers (and would-be smokers) smoke because they are addicted and overestimate their ability to quit in the future.14 Rational choice theory (and the adjustments that have been proposed to deal with addictive behaviors) assumes stable preferences, foresight, knowledge, and adequate cognitive abilities to make the decision to start or continue smoking. Conversely, empirical evidence demonstrates that these assumptions are seriously violated by smoking behavior that almost always begins during adolescence15(p179) and continues in adulthood through addictive consumption. In addition, there is no empirical literature that suggests adults who start smoking engage in deliberate decision-making processes in which they evaluate risks against benefits. The empirical literature suggests the opposite; even adults, who presumably are better equipped to consider the risks and benefits of smoking, do not anticipate regret or understand addiction.16–18Applying a significant loss in (real or potential) consumer surplus when measuring the value of antismoking initiatives has important implications for policy, including reducing the benefits of proposed health regulations. This reduction in the estimated benefits of the policy results in weakened regulations that are harder to defend when challenged in court.19,20 In using consumer surplus, a measure grounded in rational choice theory, to estimate a theoretical “cost” of not smoking,1(p36772),4 the FDA is ignoring the strong empirical evidence against the validity of applying rational choice to smoking decisions, leading the FDA to seriously overestimate the costs of reducing smoking, and in turn, underestimate the net benefits.  相似文献   
83.
Context learning in postnatal day (PD) 16–18 rats has been taken by Revillo, Cotella, Paglini, and Arias (2015, Physiology & Behavior, 148 , 6–21) to challenge the view that the ontogeny of contextual learning is related to the development of the hippocampal system (Rudy, 1993, Behavioral Neuroscience, 107 (5), 887–891; Schiffino, Murawski, Rosen, & Stanton, 2011 Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 95 (2), 190–198). Whether context learning is “incidental” or “reinforcement-driven” may determine the ontogeny and neural systems involved (Rudy, 2009, Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), 16 , 573–585). However, we have shown differential ontogeny of two different forms of incidental context learning, the context pre-exposure facilitation effect (CPFE; Jablonski, Schiffino, & Stanton, 2012, Developmental Psychobiology, 54 (7), 714–722), which emerges between PD 17 and 21; and object-in-context recognition (OiC, Ramsaran, Westbrook, & Stanton, 2016, Developmental Psychobiology, 58 (7), 883–895; Ramsaran, Sanders, & Stanton, 2016, Behavioural Brain Research, 298 , 37–47), which is present on PD17. We investigated whether this task-dissociation reflects an encoding or a retention deficit, by varying the sample-to-testing intervals for both tasks. Experiment 1A found that PD17 rats were able to perform the OiC task after short (5 min) but not long (24 hr) sample-to-test intervals. Experiments 1B and 1C found that PD17 rats trained on the CPFE are able to acquire and express context-shock associations after short but not long retention intervals. These findings suggest that pre-weanling rats encode contexts but show poor consolidation or retrieval after longer retention intervals.  相似文献   
84.
To fully understand the role of diet diversity on allergy outcomes and to set standards for conducting research in this field, the European Academy of Allergy and Clinical Immunology Task Force on Diet and Immunomodulation has systematically explored the association between diet diversity and allergy outcomes. In addition, a detailed narrative review of information on diet quality and diet patterns as they pertain to allergic outcomes is presented. Overall, we recommend that infants of any risk category for allergic disease should have a diverse diet, given no evidence of harm and some potential association of benefit in the prevention of particular allergic outcomes. In order to harmonize methods for future data collection and reporting, the task force members propose relevant definitions and important factors for consideration, when measuring diet diversity in the context of allergy. Consensus was achieved on practice points through the Delphi method. It is hoped that the definitions and considerations described herein will also enable better comparison of future studies and improve mechanistic studies and pathway analysis to understand how diet diversity modulates allergic outcomes.  相似文献   
85.
BackgroundCOVID-19 restrictions and fear dramatically changed the use of medical care. Understanding the magnitude of cancelled and postponed appointments and associated factors can help identify approaches to mitigate unmet need.ObjectiveTo determine the proportion of medical visits cancelled or postponed and for whom. We hypothesized that adults with serious medical conditions and those with higher anxiety, depressive symptoms, and avoidance-oriented coping would have more cancellations/postponements.DesignFour nationally representative cross-sectional surveys conducted online in May, July, October, and December 2020.Participants59,747 US adults who completed 15-min online surveys. 69% cooperation rate.MeasuresPhysical and mental health visits and cancer screening cancelled or postponed over prior 2 months. Plan to cancel or postpone visits over the next 2 months. Relationship with demographics, medical conditions, local COVID-19 death rate, anxiety, depressive symptoms, coping, intolerance of uncertainty, and perceived COVID-19 risk.Key ResultsOf the 58% (N = 34,868) with a medical appointment during the 2 months before the survey, 64% had an appointment cancelled or postponed in May, decreasing to 37% in December. Of the 41% of respondents with scheduled cancer screening, 20% cancelled/postponed, which was stable May to December. People with more medical conditions were more likely to cancel or postpone medical visits (OR 1.19 per condition, 95% CI 1.16, 1.22) and cancer screening (OR 1.20, 95% CI 1.15, 1.24). Race, ethnicity, and income had weak associations with cancelled/postponed visits, local death rate was unrelated, but anxiety and depressive symptoms were strongly related to cancellations, and this grew between May and December.ConclusionsCancelled medical care and cancer screening were more common among persons with medical conditions, anxiety and depression, even after accounting for COVID-19 deaths. Outreach and support to ensure that patients are not avoiding needed care due to anxiety, depression and inaccurate perceptions of risk will be important.Supplementary InformationThe online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s11606-021-07254-x.KEY WORDS: COVID-19, missed medical appointments, cancer screening  相似文献   
86.
87.
We describe the largest-to-date single-center implementation of tele-epilepsy. Beginning in 2017, all patients at a single tertiary care academic epilepsy center were offered the option to complete outpatient follow-up visits via video-conferencing using personal devices. A retrospective review of all patients who self-selected virtual visits over nearly 3 years showed 2140 patients completed 3698 tele-epilepsy visits, with 41% completing more than one visit during the study period. Based on the distance from the center to the home address, 26.7% of patients were local (≤50 miles), 30.5% were near regional (51-150 miles), 20.1% were far regional (151-270 miles), and 22.7% were remote (>270 miles), from 43 different states. An estimated 928 696 miles of travel was prevented, with a median travel distance saved of 124.5 miles (interquartile range = 45.0-253.0). The mean visit time was 15.7 (±10.4) minutes. More than 90% of patients gave the visit and provider experience the maximum rating, with a nearly 60% response rate on the post-visit survey. Virtual outpatient follow-up care provides a convenient way to connect with epilepsy specialists and reduce the burden of care by cutting travel time. Our experience demonstrates that outpatient tele-epilepsy is feasible, sustainable, and scalable.  相似文献   
88.
89.
Partial reduction of [Mg2+]o from 2 to 1 mM markedly enhanced neuronal responses evoked by Schaffer collateral-commissural fiber stimulation in the CA1-region of rat hippocampal slices. The amplitude of extracellular population potentials recorded in the CA1-pyramidal cell layer and maximum dV/dt of extracellular population EPSP's recorded in the CA1-pyramidal apical dendritic layer were both increased. However, unlike findings from slices where Mg2+ was completely removed from the bathing medium, there was no spontaneous or evoked epileptiform activity, and the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist 2-amino-5-phosphonovalerate (2-APV) did not antagonize the enhancement of evoked responses. These results indicate that, in addition to the participation of NMDA receptors in the epileptiform activity observed when Mg2+ is completely removed from the bathing medium, there is also an NMDA receptor-independent excitatory action of partial reduction of [Mg2+]o in hippocampal slices.  相似文献   
90.
目的:一些理论质疑富血小板血浆对骨前体细胞成骨分化的作用,本实验拟验证富血小板血浆对体外培养的人骨髓间充质干细胞成骨分化的抑制效应。方法:实验于2005-05/11在南方医科大学组织工程试验室(省级)完成。①实验方法:抽取6名健康志愿者髂前上棘骨髓5mL进行体外细胞培养扩增,静脉血10mL以二次离心法制得富血小板血浆。诱导骨髓间充质干细胞时富血小板血浆与骨髓间充质干细胞均来自同一个体。②碱性磷酸酶染色:取第4代骨髓间充质干细胞,分为两组:富血小板血浆组加入富血小板血浆使终浓度为100g/L,单纯血清培养组仅加入等量胎牛血清。培养后第7天进行碱性磷酸酶染色,阳性细胞为胞质中呈现黑色颗粒或块状沉淀。③矿化结节染色:取第4代骨髓间充质干细胞,分组同上。培养后第19天以0.1%茜素红-TrisHcl(pH8.3)37℃下放置30min,矿盐沉积染色阳性为红色。④Cbfa1基因表达:取第4代骨髓间充质干细胞,分组同上。培养后第3,7,12,16天RT-PCR法检测骨髓间充质干细胞Cbfa1基因的表达。⑤形态学观察:实验过程中使用相差显微镜观察各组细胞生长情况及形态学变化。结果:①骨髓间充质干细胞碱性磷酸酶染色结果:培养后第7天,富血小板血浆组碱性磷酸酶阳性细胞数量较单纯血清培养组明显减少,且阳性细胞内灰黑色颗粒也明显减少,为弱阳性。②骨髓间充质干细胞矿化结节染色结果:培养后第19天,单纯血清培养组可见细胞表面有较多的矿盐沉积,但未形成明显的矿化结节。富血小板血浆组细胞表面只有稀少的矿盐沉积。③骨髓间充质干细胞cbfa1mRNA的表达:培养后第3,7,12,16天,随着培养时间的延长单纯血清培养组与富血小板血浆组cbfa1基因表达量均逐渐增高,同一时间点两组间cbfa1基因的表达基本相似。④骨髓间充质干细胞形态学变化:富血小板血浆组骨髓间充质干细胞增殖旺盛,细胞达到单层汇合的时间较单纯血清培养组明显缩短。单纯血清培养组细胞在完全汇合后开始出现聚合现象(14~16d),但趋向性不明显,未完全形成团簇;富血小板血浆组细胞在完全汇合后未出现聚合现象,细胞密集生长。培养初期两组细胞以梭形为主,多角形细胞较少,培养至14~16d单纯血清培养组多角形细胞较富血小板血浆组增多。结论:富血小板血浆可抑制人骨髓间充质干细胞碱性磷酸酶的分泌与矿盐沉积,对人骨髓间充质干细胞成骨分化的直接效应是抑制其分化。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号