首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2260篇
  免费   198篇
  国内免费   8篇
耳鼻咽喉   16篇
儿科学   68篇
妇产科学   112篇
基础医学   229篇
口腔科学   68篇
临床医学   181篇
内科学   466篇
皮肤病学   26篇
神经病学   90篇
特种医学   40篇
外科学   621篇
综合类   61篇
一般理论   2篇
预防医学   242篇
眼科学   20篇
药学   125篇
中国医学   1篇
肿瘤学   98篇
  2022年   11篇
  2021年   31篇
  2020年   18篇
  2019年   28篇
  2018年   30篇
  2017年   32篇
  2016年   32篇
  2015年   42篇
  2014年   51篇
  2013年   74篇
  2012年   92篇
  2011年   105篇
  2010年   79篇
  2009年   65篇
  2008年   81篇
  2007年   117篇
  2006年   120篇
  2005年   86篇
  2004年   80篇
  2003年   93篇
  2002年   75篇
  2001年   73篇
  2000年   66篇
  1999年   78篇
  1998年   29篇
  1997年   26篇
  1996年   31篇
  1995年   27篇
  1994年   26篇
  1993年   24篇
  1992年   60篇
  1991年   68篇
  1990年   64篇
  1989年   51篇
  1988年   46篇
  1987年   42篇
  1986年   49篇
  1985年   53篇
  1984年   29篇
  1983年   26篇
  1981年   12篇
  1979年   26篇
  1978年   12篇
  1977年   11篇
  1976年   11篇
  1975年   14篇
  1974年   23篇
  1973年   16篇
  1972年   24篇
  1971年   12篇
排序方式: 共有2466条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
92.
ObjectivesAlthough pertussis vaccines have been widely used for many decades, a burden of illness persists. Resurgences in Ontario, Canada, have not been substantial in the past decade, but an outbreak of pertussis occurred in Toronto between 1 October 2005 and 31 March 2006. Previous Ontario studies found high vaccine effectiveness (VE) in the initial years post-immunization. In order to explore the impact of outbreaks and external factors on VE, we investigated pertussis VE during the period 2006–2008.MethodsWe assessed pertussis VE using a frequency-matched case-control study for the period 1 March 2006 to 31 December 2008. We used logistic regression to estimate VE by age, time since last vaccination, and vaccination status according to the Ontario recommended schedule. We compared analyses including and excluding cases from Toronto, and to two recent Ontario pertussis VE studies.ResultsWe included 1797 confirmed cases and 7188 matched controls. Most cases were under 4 years of age during the study period. Pertussis VE was 3.8% (95% CI: − 21.0, 24.0) in the period 15–364 days following the last pertussis vaccine dose, and increased with increasing time since vaccination. Pertussis VE in the first 15–364 days excluding Toronto increased to 57.1% (95% CI: 26.0, 75.1), but the trend of increasing VE with time since vaccination persisted. Although VE was higher in older (6–11 years) than younger (0–5 years) children, it was lower at 12–13 years than after 14 years.ConclusionVE was lower in comparison with other studies conducted in Ontario, particularly in younger children. Various factors occurring during the study period may have influenced the results, including clinical testing of asymptomatic contacts, laboratory testing and methods and reporting practice, and a sensitive case definition. Further studies are needed to optimize methods for measuring VE to inform pertussis vaccine policy.  相似文献   
93.
Primary tissue failure (structural valve deterioration) has been documented as the most prominent complication of porcine bioprostheses. The influence of age on primary tissue failure has received limited consideration. From 1975 to 1986, 1,301 Carpentier-Edwards standard porcine bioprostheses were implanted in 1,183 patients in 1,201 operations. Of the total number of prostheses, 97.7% were implanted prior to 1983. The mean follow-up was 5.6 years and was 97.5% complete. Primary tissue failure was identified in 96 patients (98 operations) at reoperation (95) or autopsy (3). One hundred four (104) prostheses were involved. Thirty-one failed after aortic valve replacement (AVR), 49 after mitral valve replacement (MVR), and 24 after multiple-valve replacement (18 patients). There were 47 male and 49 female patients. The mean age at implantation was 47 years (range, 8 to 72 years). The mean implantation time was 74.0 months. The freedom from primary tissue failure at 10 years is 77.0 +/- 2.9% overall; for AVR, 83.1 +/- 3.7%; for MVR, 72.1 +/- 4.9%; and for multiple-valve replacement, 65.5 +/- 7.8%. The freedom from deterioration for patients less than 20 years of age is significantly less than that for other age groups. The freedom from deterioration increased by decades; the greatest freedom was noted in patients 70 to 80 years old and 80 years old or older. The freedom from deterioration at 10 years for patients less than 30 years of age is 26.8 +/- 17.2%; 30 to 59 years, 77.4 +/- 3.0%; and 60 years and older, 83.1 +/- 4.2%.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   
94.
OBJECTIVES: The purpose of this study was to determine the prevalence and severity of paravalvular regurgitation (PVR) in the Artificial Valve Endocarditis Reduction Trial (AVERT) cohort. BACKGROUND: The initial AVERT cohort consisted of 807 patients randomized to receive either a Silzone-coated prosthetic valve or a conventional prosthetic valve; early clinical reports showed higher rates of valve explant caused by PVR for Silzone-coated prosthetic valve. METHODS: Of the 678 eligible patients, 575 (85%) underwent postoperative transthoracic echocardiograms. The presence and severity of PVR were identified by color flow Doppler. Reviewers were blinded to the type of prosthetic valve and the demographic and clinical variables. RESULTS: Among those who underwent echocardiography (Silzone-coated prosthetic valve, n = 285 and conventional prosthetic valve, n = 290), 59% had prosthetic aortic valves, 32% prosthetic mitral valves, and 9% had both; demographic and clinical findings (i.e., prosthetic valve endocarditis, thromboembolism, bleeding, and all-cause death) were similar for the two groups. Echocardiographically determined PVR was present in 50 valves: Silzone-coated prosthetic valve, 29 of 285 (10%) and conventional prosthetic valve, 21 of 290 (7%, p = NS); the severity of PVR was similar in both groups. Kaplan-Meier analysis showed no significant differences in PVR at 24 months from valve implantation between the two groups (24-month event-free rate: 93% Silzone-coated prosthetic valve vs. 94% conventional prosthetic valve, p = NS). CONCLUSIONS: Excluding those patients who had initial prosthetic valve explant, the two-year echocardiographic follow-up of the AVERT cohort shows no statistically significant differences in the prevalence or severity of PVR in the Silzone-coated prosthetic valve compared with the conventional prosthetic valve. Further monitoring is warranted to determine whether these clinical outcomes remain similar on long-term follow-up.  相似文献   
95.
A large literature proposes that preferences for exaggerated sex typicality in human faces (masculinity/femininity) reflect a long evolutionary history of sexual and social selection. This proposal implies that dimorphism was important to judgments of attractiveness and personality in ancestral environments. It is difficult to evaluate, however, because most available data come from large-scale, industrialized, urban populations. Here, we report the results for 12 populations with very diverse levels of economic development. Surprisingly, preferences for exaggerated sex-specific traits are only found in the novel, highly developed environments. Similarly, perceptions that masculine males look aggressive increase strongly with development and, specifically, urbanization. These data challenge the hypothesis that facial dimorphism was an important ancestral signal of heritable mate value. One possibility is that highly developed environments provide novel opportunities to discern relationships between facial traits and behavior by exposing individuals to large numbers of unfamiliar faces, revealing patterns too subtle to detect with smaller samples.Inspired by evidence from nonhuman species indicating that exaggerated sex-typical traits (e.g., large antlers, peacock tails) are often attractive to mates or intimidating to rivals (1, 2), morphological sex typicality in humans (masculinity in men and femininity in women) has been the focus of considerable research into attractiveness judgments (3, 4). Facial attractiveness research has been revolutionized by this explanatory framework from the biological sciences, which proposes that attractive human faces honestly signaled mate value within ancestral environments.An influential proposal is that facial femininity is a signal of fertility in human female faces (49) because, within same-age women, it is associated with estrogens (10), which, in turn, are related to measures of reproductive health (11). Like ovarian function, facial femininity declines with age in adulthood (12, 13). The proposal that fertile women should be attractive to men is seemingly uncontroversial because males who discriminatively mate with fertile females should achieve a straightforward reproductive advantage over those males who do not, with all other factors being equal (6). Although direct associations between facial femininity and fertility have not been demonstrated, the consensus from Western preferences, and from the limited cross-cultural data available, is that femininity is attractive, as predicted by the fertility hypothesis (1417). In environments where fertility is high and variable, this relationship should be even more apparent.In male faces, masculinity has been variously proposed to signal heritable disease resistance (“good genes” or “immunocompetence”) (4, 15, 1822) and/or perceived as a cue of aggressiveness and, consequently, intrasexual competitiveness (22, 23). The “honesty” of face shape as an indicator of immunocompetence is proposed to be the result of an immunosuppressive effect of testosterone. Because testosterone influences the growth of sex-typical traits in many species (24, 25), masculine facial shape is proposed to be a costly, and thus honest, signal of male quality (22). The hypothesis that cues of heritable health should be attractive to females is widely accepted (26), although the evidence for a link between heritable health and masculinity in humans is tentative at best (22).Support for a link between masculinity and aggression is largely indirect, and it consists of an association between testosterone and both aggressive behavior (27, 28) and face shape (25), in addition to the fact that honest signaling of dominance is commonly observed in nonhuman species (3). Masculine faces are perceived as aggressive in those groups (i.e., urban, Western) where the relationship has been tested (29). Because masculinity may signal both (desirable) immunity and (potentially costly) aggression in humans, some authors have proposed that preferences for masculinity reflect women trading-off benefits of traits putatively associated with health against those traits associated with prosocial behaviors, such as parental investment (23, 30, 31).Consistent with both of these proposals, data indicate that preferences for masculinity are stronger in circumstances where indirect benefits (heritable quality) can be realized without accompanying direct costs (aggression and low paternal investment). Such circumstances include judging attractiveness in the context of a short-term (vs. a long-term) relationship (32) and in the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle when conception following intercourse is most likely (33). Masculinity is also reported to be more strongly preferred in environments with relatively high pathogen burdens (19, 30) and in environments with higher local homicide rates (23), which has been interpreted as a response to variation in the benefits of heritable disease resistance (19) and in the net benefits conferred by aggressive males under varying levels of male–male competition (23).All of this supporting evidence comes with a very important caveat; although there has been some cross-cultural work in this area (34), the majority of studies have been conducted in Western, often student, populations characterized by high levels of development and urbanization [Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic; so-called WEIRD participants (35)]. Research on preferences in other groups is scant and methodologically inconsistent, using Internet-based designs or a limited cross-cultural component (7, 1518). Because there are differences between Western/non-Western and industrial/small-scale societies in many behaviors, including aspects of visual perception and mate choice (35), this over-representation greatly limits generalizability. Perhaps most importantly, large-scale (post)industrial societies present inhabitants with large numbers of unfamiliar faces and provide venues for the efficient exchange of (visual) social information (e.g., posters, television, Internet); these factors may be instrumental in the acquisition and reinforcement of preferences (3639). It is possible therefore that rather than being a legacy of ancestral selection pressures, preferences for dimorphism emerge in large urban groups as a byproduct of the information-processing strategies used to process large amounts of social information or in response to arbitrary cultural norms.Development also introduces an increased presence of highly differentiated social roles that arise from a greater division of labor, along with opportunities to acquire prestige without strength or aggression. Because partner preferences have been proposed to develop in response to sex-typical social roles (40, 41), it is possible that increasingly differentiated roles could influence masculinity preferences if desirable social roles not present in less developed groups are associated with facial appearance.We assessed preferences for, and trait attributions made to, faces varying in dimorphism in a cross-cultural sample of 12 groups, including non-Western, nonstudent, and small-scale societies (n = 962; Tables S1 and S2). We tested the predictions, derived from the immunocompetence handicapping hypothesis, that (i) preferences for dimorphism will be stronger in less developed groups and (ii) masculine faces would be perceived as aggressive in all populations, with perceptions in low-development groups at least as strong as in groups with high development. We estimated social development with the Human Development Index (HDI), which is a composite indicator compiled by the United Nations Development Program. To investigate which aspects of development were associated with variation in perception of our facial stimuli, we took the World Health Organization measures of years lost to disease and United Nations (UN) measures of homicide rates as proxy measures of disease burden and male intrasexual competition, respectively (both log-transformed), and UN measures of levels of urbanization. Using these national statistics almost certainly underestimates disease burden in the small-scale societies in our sample, which is a conservative estimate with regard to our hypotheses.

Table 1.

Summary information for the groups tested
GroupLocal regionCountrySubsistence moden malen femalen female after exclusions
Canadian studentsAlberta provinceCanadaMarket economy236018
UK studentsBristol cityUnited KingdomMarket economy80238134
Shanghai studentsShanghai municipalityChinaMarket economy413838
Hangzhou citizensZhejiang provinceChinaMarket economy435248
Cree CanadiansAlberta provinceCanadaMarket economy262813
TuvansTyva RepublicRussiaPastoralism, wages303018
Kadazan-DusunSabah regionMalaysiaPastoralism, agriculture252618
Fijian villagersCakaudrove provinceFijiForaging, agriculture, wages9105
ShuarMorona Santiago provinceEcuadorHorticulture, hunting, foraging, recent small-scale agropastoralism303119
MiskituRegión Autónoma del Atlántico SurNicaraguaHorticulture, fishing, hunting131715
TchimbaKunene regionNamibiaPastoralism352720
AkaSouthwest Central African RepublicCentral African RepublicForaging252511
Open in a separate windowParticipants were asked to choose the most attractive face from five sets (representing five different ethnicities, representing considerable phenotypic variation in human faces) of three opposite-sex photographs, with one 60% masculinized [i.e., with the shape differences between male and female faces caricatured by 60% (4)], one 60% feminized, and one unaltered face in each set (Fig. 1). Participants assessed attractiveness for long-term and short-term relationships. Participants were also asked to choose the most aggressive-looking face, and responses were scored in the same way. Custom randomization tests were used to test for nonrandomness of choice (e.g., Fig. S1), and ordinal generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) were used to test for associations between choices and predictor variables.Open in a separate windowFig. 1.Examples of stimuli used. A European female composite (Upper) and an East Asian male composite (Lower) are shown. Masculinized stimuli (Left) and feminized stimuli (Right) are shown.Although the previous literature suggests that familiarity effects of ethnicity can subtly affect dimorphism preferences, this influence is small and inconsistent across cultures and is unlikely to bias results as a result of exposure to ethnic variation in facial appearance (4, 15).  相似文献   
96.
Childhood abuse and lifetime psychopathology in a community sample.   总被引:27,自引:0,他引:27  
OBJECTIVE: The authors assessed lifetime psychopathology in a general population sample and compared the rates of five psychiatric disorder categories between those who reported a childhood history of either physical or sexual abuse and those who did not. METHOD: A modified version of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview and a self-completed questionnaire on child abuse were administered to a probability sample (N=7,016) of Ontario residents 15 to 64 years of age. RESULTS: Those reporting a history of childhood physical abuse had significantly higher lifetime rates of anxiety disorders, alcohol abuse/dependence, and antisocial behavior and were more likely to have one or more disorders than were those without such a history. Women, but not men, with a history of physical abuse had significantly higher lifetime rates of major depression and illicit drug abuse/dependence than did women with no such history. A history of childhood sexual abuse was also associated with higher rates of all disorders considered in women. In men, the prevalence of disorders tended to be higher among those who reported exposure to sexual abuse, but only the associations with alcohol abuse/dependence and the category of one or more disorders reached statistical significance. The relationship between a childhood history of physical abuse and lifetime psychopathology varied significantly by gender for all categories except for anxiety disorders. Although not statistically significant, a similar relationship was seen between childhood history of sexual abuse and lifetime psychopathology. CONCLUSIONS: A history of abuse in childhood increases the likelihood of lifetime psychopathology; this association appears stronger for women than men.  相似文献   
97.
98.

Objective

To explore the patterns of living arrangements, ethnicity and loneliness amongst older adults (aged 65+ years) living at home.

Methods

National interRAI‐HC (International Residential Assessment Instrument‐Home Care) assessments conducted between 1 September 2012 and 31 January 2016 were analysed. Analysis focused on the associations between loneliness and both ethnic groups and living arrangements.

Results

There were 71 859 eligible participants, with average age 82.7 years, comprising Māori (5%), Pasifika (3%), Asian (2%) and European/Other (89%) ethnic identification. Most stated that they were not lonely (79%), but those living alone were more likely to be lonely (29%) than those living with others (14%) (P < 0.05). Amongst those living alone, significant differences in the likelihood of being lonely emerged between ethnic groups (P < 0.05).

Conclusions

Ethnic identification and living arrangements were significantly associated with the likelihood of loneliness for those having an interRAI‐HC assessment. Efforts to reduce the negative impacts of loneliness need a nuanced approach.  相似文献   
99.
Two cardioacceleratory peptides from the corpora cardiaca of Periplaneta americana have been purified by gel filtration and reversed-phase liquid chromatography, Based on analysis of the intact factors and their chymotryptic fragments, we have assigned the primary structure of these octapeptides as pGlu-Val-Asn-Phe-Ser-Pro-Asn-Trp-NH2, designated periplanetin CC-1, and pGlu-Leu-Thr-Phe-Thr-Pro-Asn-Trp-NH2, designated periplanetin CC-2. They represent new members of a family of invertebrate peptides that includes locust adipokinetic hormone and crustacean red-pigment concentrating hormone. Both peptides show adipokinetic activity in grasshoppers and hyperglycemic activity in cockroaches. One of these peptides (CC-2) has provocative sequence homology with the NH2-terminal portion of glucagon.  相似文献   
100.
The ventilatory response to submaximal exercise, defined as the slope of minute ventilation over carbon dioxide production (VE/VCO2), was determined in 12 normal subjects, ten patients with pulmonary hypertension before and after heart-lung transplantation, and eight patients following heart transplantation. Patients with pulmonary hypertension show an augmented ventilatory response compared to normal subjects (pulmonary hypertension [mean, 57.7 +/- 6.8 (SE) ml/ml VCO2; normal subjects, 22.3 +/- 1.4 ml/ml VCO2; p less than 0.001]). Following heart-lung transplantation, VE/VCO2 slope fell to 24.7 +/- 1.6 ml/ml VCO2, a value which is not significantly different than the value in normal subjects. Patients after heart transplantation show a mean slope value of 25.3 +/- 1.3 ml/ml VCO2, which is not significantly different than the normal value or the value found after heart-lung transplantation. The augmented ventilatory response to exercise did not correlate with the usual chemical modulators of ventilation (arterial pH, arterial carbon dioxide tension, or arterial oxygen tension). These results suggest the following: the existence of a neural system in patients with pulmonary hypertension which results in an augmentation of ventilatory drive in response to exercise; the augmented ventilatory response reflects excessive neural activity of pulmonary afferents during exercise; narrow regulation of the ventilatory response to exercise in normal subjects which is preserved in the denervated lung, indicating that pulmonary afferents are not critical to ventilatory control during exercise in the normal subject; and the possible use of measurements of the ventilatory response to exercise as a noninvasive screening test for pulmonary hypertension.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号