首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 10 毫秒
1.
2.
The difference limen for perceived stimulus orientation was measured for thin lines, and for sine wave gratings between 2.5 and 20.0 c/deg. All observers exhibited a marked meridional anisotropy on this task with both the thin line and grating test targets. For the sine wave gratings orientation discrimination was not found to depend on their spatial frequency. Contrast threshold measurements with the same set of stimuli confirmed earlier reports that the meridional anisotropy for contrast detection increases with test spatial frequency. The data are consistent with published hypotheses (Regan and Beverley, 1985) that detection and discrimination of spatial patterns may be processed differently by orientation selective elements of the visual system and that there are fewer of these mechanisms subserving oblique orientations than either vertical or horizontal orientations.  相似文献   

3.
Eye movements challenge the perception of a stable world by inducing retinal image displacement. During saccadic eye movements visual stability is accompanied by a remapping of visual receptive fields, a compression of visual space and perceptual suppression. Here we explore whether a similar suppression changes the perception of briefly presented low contrast targets during the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements. In a 2AFC design we investigated the contrast sensitivity for threshold-level stimuli during the initiation of smooth pursuit and during saccades. Pursuit was elicited by horizontal step-ramp and ramp stimuli. At any time from 200 ms before to 500 ms after pursuit stimulus onset, a blurred 0.3 deg wide horizontal line with low contrast just above detection threshold appeared for 10 ms either 2 deg above or below the pursuit trajectory. Observers had to pursue the moving stimulus and to indicate whether the target line appeared above or below the pursuit trajectory. In contrast to perceptual suppression effects during saccades, no pronounced suppression was found at pursuit onset for step-ramp motion. When pursuit was elicited by a ramp stimulus, pursuit initiation was accompanied by catch-up saccades, which caused saccadic suppression. Additionally, contrast sensitivity was attenuated at the time of pursuit or saccade stimulus onset. This attenuation might be due to an attentional deficit, because the stimulus required the focus of attention during the programming of the following eye movement.  相似文献   

4.
Contrast sensitivity gratings in glaucoma family screening.   总被引:4,自引:4,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
Contrast sensitivity was measured in the eyes of 200 consecutive patients attending the glaucoma family screening unit. One-third of the patients screened had an abnormal contrast sensitivity. Positive correlation (p less than 0.01) existed between this abnormal contrast sensitivity and cup : disc ratio, visual field loss, and age. No such correlation existed between contrast sensitivity and intraocular pressure.  相似文献   

5.
Variable contrast, large-field sine wave gratings are easily and inexpensively produced using a projection technique. Polaroid is used to manipulate the contrast and the system can be used with any light distribution. Other applications are suggested.  相似文献   

6.
Contrast sensitivity after wave front-guided LASIK   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
PURPOSE: To compare the effects on contrast sensitivity of wave front-guided (WFG) versus standard LASIK. DESIGN: Prospective, nonrandomized, comparative clinical study. PARTICIPANTS: Twenty-four eyes of 13 consecutive patients (mean age, 25.2+/-8.4 years; spherical equivalent, -0.5 to -4.25 diopters [D]) treated with WFG LASIK (WaveLight-Allegretto scanning-spot laser and wave front analyzer) and 22 eyes of 12 consecutive patients (mean age, 28.4+/-9.1 years; spherical equivalent, -0.75 to -4.5 D) treated with standard LASIK (WaveLight-Allegretto scanning-spot laser). METHODS: Best-corrected contrast sensitivity was measured before and 1 month after surgery in both the WFG LASIK group and the standard LASIK group. A sine-wave contrast sensitivity test (functional acuity contrast test) was used to measure contrast sensitivity at 5 spatial frequencies (1.5, 3, 6, 12, and 18 cycles/degree). We compared the LASIK-induced changes in contrast sensitivity in each groups at each spatial frequency. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURE: The effect on contrast sensitivity of WFG LASIK versus standard LASIK. RESULTS: Uncorrected visual acuity of 20/20 or better was achieved by 72% of eyes treated with WFG LASIK and by 70% of the eyes treated with standard LASIK. One month after LASIK, 88% of the contrast sensitivity measurements improved in the WFG LASIK group, whereas in the standard LASIK group, only 40% of the contrast sensitivity measurements improved. The contrast sensitivity improvement was significantly larger in the WFG LASIK group at all spatial frequencies (P<0.05). The WFG LASIK patients had a negative correlation between the changes in contrast sensitivity and the preoperative refractive error. CONCLUSIONS: The ability of WFG LASIK to correct optical aberrations results in significantly improved contrast sensitivity compared with standard LASIK 1 month after surgery.  相似文献   

7.
Smooth pursuit of natural objects requires flexible allocation of attention to inspect features. However, it has been reported that attention is focused at the fovea during pursuit. We ask here if foveal attention is obligatory during pursuit, or if it can be disengaged. Observers tracked a stimulus composed of a central dot surrounded by four others and identified one of the dots when it dimmed. Extinguishing the center dot before the dimming improved task performance, suggesting that attention was released from it. To determine if the center dot automatically usurped attention, we provided the pursuit system with an alternative sensory signal by adding peripheral motion that moved with the stimulus. This also improved identification performance, evidence that a central target does not necessarily require attention during pursuit. Identification performance at the central dot also improved, suggesting that the spatial extent of the background did not attract attention to the periphery; instead, peripheral motion freed pursuit attention from the central dot, affording better identification performance. The results show that attention can be flexibly allocated during pursuit and imply that attention resources for pursuit of small and large objects come from different sources.  相似文献   

8.
Contrast sensitivity for both vertically and obliquely oriented gratings increased with increases in stimulus area to an asymptotic value. Sensitivity grew more slowly for oblique gratings and reached an asymptotic value at a larger area than for vertical gratings. For equal areas, oblique gratings always yielded poorer sensitivity. The results suggest a larger spatial summation area for obliquely oriented gratings.  相似文献   

9.
PURPOSE: To evaluate relationships among contrast sensitivity function, ocular higher-order aberration, and myopic correction in eyes undergoing overnight orthokeratology for myopia. METHODS: A prospective study was conducted in 46 eyes of 23 patients undergoing orthokeratology. Inclusion criteria were spherical equivalent refraction between -1.00 and -4.00 diopters (D), refractive astigmatism up to 1.00 D, and best-corrected visual acuity of 20/20 or better. Ocular higher-order aberrations and contrast sensitivity function were determined before and 3 months after initiation of the procedure. We measured three indices of contrast sensitivity function: contrast sensitivity, low-contrast visual acuity, and letter contrast sensitivity with the CSV-1000 charts (Vector Vision Co., Greenville, OH). Area under the log contrast sensitivity function (AULCSF) was calculated from the contrast sensitivity data. RESULTS: Orthokeratology significantly improved logMAR uncorrected visual acuity (P < 0.0001; paired t-test) but significantly increased ocular higher-order aberrations (P < 0.0001) and decreased contrast sensitivity function, including AULCSF (P < 0.0001), low-contrast visual acuity (P = 0.0025), and letter contrast sensitivity (P < 0.0001; Wilcoxon signed-rank test). The induced changes in AULCSF, low-contrast visual acuity, and letter contrast sensitivity by orthokeratology showed significant correlation with changes in third-order (Pearson r = -0.430, P = 0.0026; r = 0.423, P = 0.0031; and Spearman r(s) = -0.351, P = 0.0186, respectively), fourth-order (r = -0.418, P = 0.0035; r = 0.425, P = 0.0029; and r(s) = -0.566, P = 0.0001, respectively), and total higher-order (r = -0.460, P = 0.0011; r = 0.471, P = 0.0008; and r(s) = -0.434, P = 0.0036, respectively) aberrations. The induced changes in contrast sensitivity function and higher-order aberrations significantly correlated with the amount of myopic correction (P < 0.01). CONCLUSIONS: Orthokeratology significantly increases ocular higher-order aberrations and compromises contrast sensitivity function, depending on the amount of myopic correction.  相似文献   

10.
This study examined the extent to which letter optotypes and grating stimuli provide equivalent measures of contrast sensitivity under conditions designed to favor the magnocellular (MC) and parvocellular (PC) pathways. The contrast sensitivity functions (CSFs) of three visually normal observers were measured for Sloan letters and Gabor patches, using steady- and pulsed-pedestal paradigms to bias processing toward MC and PC pathways, respectively. CSFs for Gabor patches were low-pass for the steady-pedestal paradigm and band-pass for the pulsed-pedestal paradigm, in agreement with previous reports. However, CSFs for letters were low-pass for both testing paradigms. CSFs for letters restricted in frequency content by spatial filtering were equivalent to those for Gabor patches for both testing paradigms. Results indicate that conventional letter optotypes can provide a misleading measure of contrast sensitivity, especially under conditions emphasizing the PC pathway. The use of spatially band-pass filtered letters can provide a more appropriate evaluation of spatial contrast sensitivity while maintaining some of the potential advantages of letters.  相似文献   

11.
The experiment measured contrast sensitivity of three human observers to sinusoidal gratings presented in 10 msec exposures. The gratings were presented to the steadily fixating eye and during 6° horizontal saccades. Experimental conditions of viewing in a Ganzfeld reduced possible effects of contour masking. The use of horizontal gratings minimized retinal smear.Results showed a significant suppression of sensitivity (more than 0.6 log unit of contrast) to low spatial frequency gratings presented during saccades. The magnitude of saccadic suppression decreased as spatial frequency of the gratings increased.We conclude that optical and neural effects combine in normal viewing to produce saccadic suppression. Minimizing the optically originating effects of contour masking and retinal image smear failed to eliminate the considerable impairment of vision that occurs during a saccadic eye movement.  相似文献   

12.
During smooth pursuit eye movements, stimuli other than the pursuit target move across the retina, and this might affect their detectability. We measured detection thresholds for vertically oriented Gabor stimuli with different temporal frequencies (1, 4, 8, 12, 16, 20, and 24 Hz) of the sinusoids. Observers kept fixation on a small target spot that was either stationary or moved horizontally at a speed of 8 deg/s. The sinusoid of the Gabor stimuli moved either in the same or in the opposite direction as the pursuit target. Observers had to indicate whether the Gabor stimuli were displayed 4 degrees above or below the target spot. Results show that contrast sensitivity was mainly determined by retinal-image motion but was slightly reduced during smooth pursuit eye movements. Moreover, sensitivity for motion opposite to pursuit direction was reduced in comparison to motion in pursuit direction. The loss in sensitivity for peripheral targets during pursuit can be interpreted in terms of space-based attention to the pursuit target. The loss of sensitivity for motion opposite to pursuit direction can be interpreted as feature-based attention to the pursuit direction.  相似文献   

13.
14.
We measured contrast sensitivity to drifting sine wave gratings in central (20 degrees) and peripheral (from +/- 10 degrees to +/- 30 degrees) retinal areas. Low spatial frequencies were used (0.1-3 c/deg). The spatiotemporal sensitivity for spatial frequencies above 0.8 c/deg was found to be better in the central than in the peripheral retina. However, for spatial frequencies below 0.8 c/deg, the spatiotemporal contrast sensitivities of central and peripheral retinal areas were found to be the same. These results were obtained without scaling the peripheral gratings by the reciprocal of the cortical magnification factor.  相似文献   

15.
Our eyes are never still, but tend to "freeze" in response to stimulus onset. This effect is termed “oculomotor inhibition” (OMI); its magnitude and time course depend on the stimulus parameters, attention, and expectation. We previously showed that the time course and duration of microsaccade and spontaneous eye-blink inhibition provide an involuntary measure of low-level visual properties such as contrast sensitivity during fixation. We investigated whether this stimulus-dependent inhibition also occurs during smooth pursuit, for both the catch-up saccades and the pursuit itself. Observers followed a target with continuous back-and-forth horizontal motion while a Gabor patch was briefly flashed centrally with varied spatial frequency and contrast. Catch-up saccades of the size of microsaccades had a similar pattern of inhibition as microsaccades during fixation, with stronger inhibition onset and faster inhibition release for more salient stimuli. Moreover, a similar stimulus dependency of inhibition was shown for pursuit latencies and peak velocity. Additionally, microsaccade latencies at inhibition release, peak pursuit velocities, and latencies at minimum pursuit velocity were correlated with contrast sensitivity. We demonstrated the generality of OMI to smooth pursuit for both microsaccades and the pursuit itself and its close relation to the low-level processes that define saliency, such as contrast sensitivity.  相似文献   

16.
Threshold contrast was measured for noise gratings of various spatial frequency bandwidths and central frequencies. The results indicate that the contrast reaches threshold whenever the spatial power of the stimulus, filtered according to the contrast sensitivity function, exceeds a fixed level, irrespective of the width of the spatial frequency range over which the power is distributed. No “critical band effect” is found. For suprathreshold noise gratings of various bandwidths we measured thresholds for the detection of sinusoidal (amplitude) modulation of the contrast. The results indicate that the visual system uses the spatial power always with about the same efficiency, irrespective of the bandwidth over which it is distributed. The thresholds depend on the spatial frequency of the modulation: the visual system appears to be unable to use information from more than one cycle of the modulation frequency.  相似文献   

17.
Contrast matching was performed with isoluminant red-green and s-cone gratings at spatial frequencies ranging from 0.5 to 8 c/deg. Contrast threshold curves were low-pass in shape, in agreement with previous findings. Contrast matching functions resembled threshold curves at low contrast levels, but became flat and independent of spatial frequency at high contrasts. Thus, isoluminant chromatic gratings exhibited contrast constancy at suprathreshold contrast levels in a similar manner as has been demonstrated for achromatic gratings.  相似文献   

18.
Li DF  Freeman AW  Alais D 《Vision research》2005,45(10):1255-1263
Binocular rivalry, which is induced by presenting the two eyes with incompatible stimuli, results in periods where one eye's stimulus is seen and the other stimulus is suppressed. We measured the depth of suppression in two ways, with very different results. First, two similar forms were briefly presented to one eye: the difference in shapes required to discriminate the forms was substantially greater during suppression than during dominance. Second, the two forms were made sufficiently different in shape to be easily distinguishable at high contrast, and contrast was lowered to find the threshold for discrimination of the forms. Contrast sensitivity did not differ between the suppression and dominance states. These results were replicated with a motion discrimination task: suppression markedly worsened the ability to distinguish increases from decreases in speed but did not elevate the minimum contrast required for the same task. We interpret the results in terms of steep contrast-response functions in visual cortex beyond the primary area.  相似文献   

19.
Contrast sensitivity has been assessed in 24 diabetic patients in order to test the hypothesis that contrast sensitivity is impaired in the early stages of diabetes mellitus. All patients had 20/20 vision. Some evidence of visual dysfunction was observed in 33% of the diabetics with no retinopathy and 83% of the 6 patients with retinopathy when compared to 48 age-matched controls. Contrast sensitivity was mainly reduced in the mid-range spatial frequencies and correlated with the degree of retinopathy. The accurate assessment of visual dysfunction in diabetes is very important, as new drugs (i.e. aldose reductase inhibitors) are currently under evaluation.  相似文献   

20.
Marc Green 《Vision research》1983,23(3):281-289
Observers performed simple detection and left right discrimination of drifting sinusoidal gratings. Ratio of detection to discrimination sensitivities was measured under variations in several experimental parameters. In the first experiment, it was found that combinations of spatial and temporal frequency which resulted in the same velocity produced similar detection discrimination ratios. At an exposure duration of 800 msec, the relationship between the ratio and velocity described a power function with the intercept at 0.6 sec?1. Decreasing duration shifted the curve to higher velocities. I examined the effect of grating orientation in a second experiment. Visual sensitivity was poorer for oblique than for vertical gratings with detection and discrimination exhibiting similar size anisotropies. In a third experiment, observers viewed gratings presented to different retinal loci, Visual performance in both detection and discrimination fell with greater eccentricity. However, motion discrimination fell more steeply resulting in an increase in the ratio. The results demonstrate that form and motion analyzing mechanisms cannot be distinguished by their response to changes of spatial frequency, orientation or retinal locus.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号