首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Wistar rat pups were trained in an olfactory associative conditioning task on postnatal Day 6, 12, or 20. The training consisted of 20 pairings of a novel odor (peppermint) with footshock (1.5 mA, 1 s) with an intertrial interval of 3 min. Additional pups were trained in either unpaired or naive control conditions. On the day following training, pups were either tested for their behavioral response to the conditioned odor in a two-odor choice test, or injected with 14C-2-deoxyglucose and exposed to the odor for examination of olfactory bulb neural responses to the odor. The results demonstrate that, although pups at all ages learned to avoid the odor, only pups trained during the first postnatal week had a modified olfactory-bulb glomerular-layer response to the odor. These results suggest that although olfactory memory is correlated with modification of olfactory bulb glomerular layer function in newborns, these changes are not required for normal memory in older pups. © 1995 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.  相似文献   

2.
Following olfactory classical conditioning, infant rats exhibit a preference for the conditioned odor and exhibit enhanced uptake of focal 14C 2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) within the olfactory bulb. The present experiments assessed the role of respiration on the expression of the enhanced 2-DG uptake response. Pups were conditioned from postnatal day (PN) 1-18 with an olfactory stimulus paired with a reinforcing tactile stimulus which mimics maternal contact (Odor-Stroke). Control pups received odor only or tactile stimulation only. On PN 19, pups received 1 of 3 tests: 1) a two-odor choice test, 2) an odor/2-DG test with normal respiration allowed, or 3) an odor/2-DG test with respiration experimentally controlled. The results indicated that: 1) Odor-Stroke pups learned the conditioned odor preference, 2) Odor-Stroke, normally respiring pups exhibited enhanced olfactory bulb 2-DG uptake when compared to control pups. No difference in respiration rate was detected between groups in normally respiring pups. 3) Odor Stroke pups whose breathing was experimentally controlled exhibited enhanced olfactory bulb 2-DG uptake when compared to control pups with an identical number of respirations. Together, these results demonstrate that modified respiration during testing is not required for the expression of a modified olfactory bulb response to learned attractive odors. Therefore, the data suggest that the olfactory system itself is modified by early learning.  相似文献   

3.
1. Neonatal rat pups were classically conditioned to an odor stimulus from postnatal day 1 (PN1) to PN18. Tactile stimulation (stroking) was used as the unconditioned stimulus. On PN19, mitral/tufted cell single-unit responses to the conditioned odor were examined in both conditioned and control pups. Recordings were made from mitral/tufted cells in two regions of the olfactory bulb: 1) an area typically associated with focal [14C]2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) uptake in response to the conditioned odor and 2) an area distant from focal 2-DG uptake to the conditioned odor. Animals were anesthetized with urethane and were naturally respiring during the single-unit recording procedure. 2. Changes in mitral/tufted cell firing rate in response to odors in both bulbar regions and all training groups were classified as either excitatory, suppressive, or no response. This response classification was used to compare response patterns to the conditioned odor between bulbar regions and training groups. 3. Classical conditioning selectively modified the response patterns of mitral/tufted cells to the conditioned odor when those cells were associated with regions of focal 2-DG uptake for that odor. Mitral/tufted cells demonstrated significantly more suppressive and fewer excitatory responses to the conditioned odor than cells in control pups. Response patterns to a novel odor were not similarly modified. 4. Response patterns of mitral/tufted cells distant from the focal region of 2-DG uptake to the conditioned odor were not modified by conditioning compared with control pups. 5. The difference in response pattern between cells in the 2-DG focus and cells distant to the 2-DG focus was apparent within 500 ms of the stimulus onset. Given the respiratory rate of these pups (2 Hz), these data suggest that the modified response pattern occurred on the first inhalation of the learned odor. 6. These data demonstrate that both spatial and temporal patterns of olfactory bulb output neuron activity are used in the coding of olfactory information in the bulb. Furthermore, these spatial/temporal response patterns can be modified by early learning.  相似文献   

4.
The olfactory memory acquired during the early postnatal period is known to be maintained for a long period, however, its neural mechanism remains to be clarified. In the present study, we examined the effect of olfactory conditioning during the early postnatal period on neurogenesis in the olfactory bulb of rats. Using the bromodeoxyuridine-pulse chase method, we found that the olfactory conditioning, which was a paired presentation of citral odor (conditioned stimulus) and foot shock (unconditioned stimulus) in rat pups on postnatal day 11, stimulated the proliferation of neural stem/progenitor cells in the anterior subventricular zone (aSVZ), but not in the olfactory bulb, at 24 h after the conditioning. However, the number of newborn cells in the olfactory bulb was increased at 2 weeks, but not 8 weeks, after such conditioning. Neither the exposure of a citral odor alone nor foot shock alone affected the proliferation of neural stem/progenitor cells in the aSVZ at 24 h after and the number of newborn cells in the olfactory bulb at 2 weeks after. The majority of newborn cells in the olfactory bulb of either the conditioned rats or the unconditioned rats expressed the neural marker NeuN, thus indicating that the olfactory conditioning stimulated neurogenesis in the olfactory bulb. These results suggest that olfactory conditioning during the early postnatal period temporally stimulates neurogenesis in the olfactory bulb of rats.  相似文献   

5.
One of the circuits modified by early olfactory learning is in the olfactory bulb. Specifically, response patterns of mitral-tufted cells are modified by associative conditioning during the early postnatal period. In addition, previous work has demonstrated that mitral-tufted cell single units respond to both olfactory conditioned stimuli and rewarding stimulation of the medial forebrain bundle-lateral hypothalamus (MFB-LH). The present study suggests that norepinephrine beta-receptor activation is required for early olfactory learning using MFB-LH stimulation as reward. Propranolol injected before odor-MFB-LH pairings blocks the acquisition of conditioned behavioral responses and their neural correlates to the conditioned odor. Furthermore, propranolol blocks a specific class of the mitral-tufted cell responses to MFB-LH reward stimulation. The relationship of this response to reward and early learning is discussed.  相似文献   

6.
These experiments examined the sufficiency of pairing an odor with either intrabulbar activation of noradrenergic beta-receptors or pharmacological stimulation of the locus coeruleus to support learned odor preferences in Postnatal Day 6-7 rat pups. The results showed that pups exposed to odor paired with beta-receptor activation limited to the olfactory bulb (isoproterenol, 50 microM) displayed a conditioned approach response on subsequent exposure to that odor. Furthermore, putative stimulation of the locus coeruleus (2 microM idazoxan or 2 mM acetylcholine) paired with odor produced a subsequent preference for that odor. The effects of locus coeruleus stimulation could be blocked by a pretraining injection of the beta-receptor antagonist propranolol (20 mg/kg). Together these results suggest that convergence of odor input with norepinephrine release from the locus coeruleus terminals within the olfactory bulb is sufficient to support olfactory learning.  相似文献   

7.
Male rats that copulate to ejaculation with female rats bearing an odor show a learned preference to ejaculate selectively with females that bear the odor. This conditioned ejaculatory preference reflects an association between the odor and the reward state induced by ejaculation. Although little is known about the neuronal mechanisms that mediate this form of learning, convergence of genitosensory and olfactory inputs occurs in both hypothalamic and cortical regions, notably within primary olfactory (piriform) cortex, which may be involved in the encoding or storage of the association. The present study contrasted the ability of genital investigations, mounts, intromissions, ejaculations, and a sexually conditioned olfactory stimulus, to enhance evoked synaptic field potentials in the piriform cortex. Rats in the Paired group underwent conditioning trials in which they copulated with sexually receptive females bearing an almond odor. Rats in the Unpaired control group copulated with receptive females bearing no odor. Responses in the piriform cortex evoked by electrical stimulation of the olfactory bulb were recorded in male rats as they engaged in different aspects of sexual behavior, and were also recorded after conditioning, during exposure to cotton swabs bearing the almond odor. The monosynaptic component of responses was increased during intromission and ejaculation, and the late component of responses was increased during anogenital sniffing and mounting (with or without intromission). However, no differences in the amplitudes of evoked responses were found between the Paired and Unpaired groups, and no differences in synaptic responses were found during presentation of the odor after conditioning. These data indicate that short-term alterations in synaptic responsiveness occur in piriform cortex as a function of sexual stimulation in the male rat, but that responses are not significantly altered by a conditioned odor.  相似文献   

8.
F Okutani  F Yagi  H Kaba 《Neuroscience》1999,93(4):1297-1300
Olfactory learning in young rats correlates with neural plasticity in the olfactory bulb, and involves noradrenergic modulation of reciprocal dendrodendritic synapses between mitral cells and GABAergic granule cells. The purpose of this study was to examine, in vivo, the consequences of manipulating bulbar GABA transmission during training. In the first experiment, postnatal day 11 rat pups were trained in an olfactory associative learning task with citral odor and foot shock as the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli, respectively. The pups received continuous infusion of saline or the GABA(A) receptor agonist muscimol into the olfactory bulbs throughout a 30-min training session. The pups were then tested on postnatal day 12 for a preference for or an aversion to citral odor. Saline-infused control pups developed an aversion to citral odor. The GABA(A) receptor agonist muscimol impaired this aversive learning in a dose-dependent manner. In the second experiment, pups were exposed to the odor for 30 min while receiving continuous intrabulbar infusion of a low or high dose of the GABA(A) receptor antagonist bicuculline, without any other reinforcer. Depending on whether a low (0.2 nmol/bulb) or high (1.0 nmol/bulb) dose of bicuculline was infused, the pups showed a preference or an aversion for citral odor after infusion of low and high doses, respectively. These results indicate that disinhibition of mitral cells in the olfactory bulb is critical for olfactory learning in young rats, and suggest that the degree of disinhibition is an important determinant in acquiring either preference or aversion for the conditioned odor.  相似文献   

9.
During development, conditioned responses usually occur first to olfactory, then to auditory, and finally to visual cues. The authors of the present study report that fear potentiation of startle to an olfactory conditioned stimulus emerges relatively late in development (i.e., at 23 days of age; Experiments 1 and 2). The failure to observe conditioned odor potentiation of startle (OPS) in younger rats was not due to a failure to either acquire or remember the odor-shock association (Experiment 3). Surprisingly, the authors also found that rats trained at 16 but tested at 23 days of age failed to exhibit the OPS effect even though they did exhibit pronounced odor avoidance (Experiment 4). The results are discussed in terms of (a) sensory-specific sequential emergence of learned fear, (b) the neural circuit involved in fear potentiation of startle, and (c) the concept that conditioned responding is appropriate to the animal's age at the time of training rather than its age at testing.  相似文献   

10.
In vitro odor-aversion conditioning in a terrestrial mollusk   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
We developed an in vitro odor-aversion conditioning system in the terrestrial mollusk, Limax, and found a behavioral correlate of network oscillation in the olfactory CNS. We first examined the odor-induced behavior of Limax, after odor-aversion conditioning in vivo. Shortening of mantle muscles was specifically observed in response to aversively conditioned odors. We previously identified that parietal nerves, which project to the mantle muscle in Limax, regulate shortening of the mantle muscle. We therefore isolated whole brains containing noses (sensory organs) and parietal nerves (motor output), and applied an odor-aversion conditioning paradigm to these in vitro preparations. Before the in vitro conditioning, application of attractive odors to the noses did not elicit any discharge in the parietal nerves. However, after odor-aversion conditioning, discharges in the parietal nerves were observed in response to the natively attractive but aversively conditioned odors. We also found that network oscillation frequency in the procerebrum (PC), the olfactory CNS of Limax, increased specifically in response to the aversively conditioned odors that elicited avoidance behavior. In naive (nonconditioned) preparations, increases in the PC oscillation frequency were observed specifically in response to innately aversive odors. These results indicate that the isolated brains have an ability of odor learning. They also suggest that changes in PC network oscillation are associated with aversively conditioned and innately aversive odors, both of which elicit avoidance behavior. This in vitro conditioning system would be an effective approach for exploring the neural mechanism to determine the aversion to odors.  相似文献   

11.
Aversive olfactory memory is formed in the mushroom bodies in Drosophila melanogaster. Memory retrieval requires mushroom body output, but the manner in which a memory trace in the mushroom body drives conditioned avoidance of a learned odor remains unknown. To identify neurons that are involved in olfactory memory retrieval, we performed an anatomical and functional screen of defined sets of mushroom body output neurons. We found that MB-V2 neurons were essential for retrieval of both short- and long-lasting memory, but not for memory formation or memory consolidation. MB-V2 neurons are cholinergic efferent neurons that project from the mushroom body vertical lobes to the middle superiormedial protocerebrum and the lateral horn. Notably, the odor response of MB-V2 neurons was modified after conditioning. As the lateral horn has been implicated in innate responses to repellent odorants, we propose that MB-V2 neurons recruit the olfactory pathway involved in innate odor avoidance during memory retrieval.  相似文献   

12.
For a better understanding of the encoding of odor quality in the olfactory system, it is critical to determine how electrophysiological responses to odorants are reflected in the behavioral responses to these odorants. In this article, we use a simple behavioral paradigm to show that the behavioral responses to similar odorants can be predicted from the electrophysiological responses of neurons in the olfactory bulb. Carbon chain length in aliphatic aldehydes has been used as a model for graded similarity among odorants. Recent electrophysiological experiments have shown that mitral cells in the rabbit olfactory bulb respond with similar response patterns to aliphatic aldehydes of similar chain length. On average, mitral cells responded with increased spiking activity to stimulation with two to three different aldehydes of neighboring chain length. We here show that the perception of these odorants can be predicted from the electrophysiological responses: rats that are conditioned to a given aldehyde generalize to aldehydes with one to two carbon differences in chain length from the conditioned aldehyde. When asked to discriminate between aldehydes of different chain lengths, rats learned to discriminate between any two odorants, but the rate of acquisition depended on the degree of similarity between the two odorants.  相似文献   

13.
Okutani F  Zhang JJ  Yagi F  Kaba H 《Neuroscience》2002,112(4):901-906
On postnatal day 12, young rats show an aversion to an odor to which they had been exposed along with presentations of foot shock on postnatal day 11. The acquisition of this aversive learning involves and requires disinhibition of the mitral/tufted cells induced by centrifugal noradrenergic activation during somatosensory stimulation. This olfactory learning is established only for the odor to which the rat has been exposed during conditioning. Infusion of the GABA(A) receptor antagonist bicuculline at a high dose (2.0 nmol/each olfactory bulb) into the olfactory bulb in the presence of an odor is capable of developing olfactory aversive responses without somatosensory stimulation in young rats. The purpose of this study is to characterize the properties of bicuculline-induced aversive responses. In contrast to the odor specificity of aversive learning produced by odor-shock conditioning, bicuculline-induced aversive responses lack odor specificity. Namely, bicuculline infusion in the presence of a citral odor results, in a dose-dependent manner, in subsequent aversive responses to strange odors (benzaldehyde and vanillin) that have never been presented. Moreover, bicuculline infusion alone is sufficient to produce dose-dependent aversive responses to strange odors (citral, benzaldehyde and geraniol).From these results we suggest that disinhibition of mitral/tufted cells from granule cells by bicuculline infusion makes young rats aversive to strange odors non-specifically, as if the rats had learned the odor aversion as a result of odor exposure paired with foot shock. Different mechanisms of disinhibition of the mitral/tufted cells may underlie both the pharmacological manipulation and noradrenergic activation by somatosensory stimulation.  相似文献   

14.
Early olfactory preference learning in rat pups occurs when novel odors are paired with reinforcing tactile stimulation that activate the noradrenergic locus coeruleus. Pairing of odor and a noradrenergic agonist in the olfactory bulb is both necessary and sufficient for odor preference learning. This suggests the memory change occurs in the olfactory bulb. Previous electrophysiological experiments demonstrated that odor preference training induces an increase in the field excitatory postsynaptic potential to olfactory nerve input and an alteration, after training, in glomerular [14C]2- deoxyglucose uptake and in single-unit responses of principal cells. We investigate here whether, 24 h after olfactory preference training, there is an alteration in intrinsic optical signals at the glomerular level. Six-day-old rat pups were trained, as previously, for a peppermint odor preference. Trained pups and control littermates were subjected to imaging of odor-induced intrinsic optical signals 1 day after the training session. Trained pups exhibited significantly larger responses to the peppermint compared with untrained littermates previously exposed to the same odor. The response of trained pups to a control odor (amyl acetate) was, however, not significantly different from that of untrained littermates. These observations demonstrate that odor preference memory can be read-out by optical imaging techniques.  相似文献   

15.
16.
Unilateral olfactory deprivation in the rat induces changes in the catecholaminergic system of the olfactory bulb. Nevertheless, evidence suggests that unilateral deprivation does not fully prevent stimulation of the deprived bulb. The present report analyses the response of the catecholaminergic system of the olfactory bulb in fully deprived rats obtained by bilateral naris occlusion. The complete deprivation produces more rapid and dramatic changes in both the intrinsic and extrinsic catecholaminergic systems of the olfactory bulb. Intrinsic responses involve a rapid decrease in dopamine-containing cells to about 25% of controls, correlated with a decreased Fos expression in juxtaglomerular cells of all olfactory glomeruli, with the only exception of those of the atypical glomeruli which maintain unaltered expression of both markers. In parallel with these events, there is a progressive increase in the density of extrinsic noradrenergic axons arising from neurons in the locus coeruleus, which shows, in parallel, a progressive increase in Fos expression. This model demonstrates plastic changes in the catecholaminergic system of the olfactory bulb forming a valid morphological substrate for lowering thresholds in the processing of olfactory information. In addition to this generalized response, there is another one, directed to a specific subset of olfactory glomeruli (atypical glomeruli) involved in the processing of odor pheromone-like cues related to behavioral responses, that could be responsible for keeping active this reduced and selected group of glomeruli carrying crucial olfactory information. These results indicate the existence of adaptive changes in the catecholaminergic system of the olfactory bulb as a response to the lack of afferent peripheral stimulation. These changes involve dopamine- and noradrenaline-immunoreactive elements, in a strategy presumably directed at maintaining to the highest possible level the ability to detect olfactory signals.  相似文献   

17.
Experience shapes both central olfactory system function and odor perception. In piriform cortex, odor experience appears critical for synthetic processing of odor mixtures, which contributes to perceptual learning and perceptual acuity, as well as contributing to memory for events and/or rewards associated with odors. Here, we examined the effect of odor fear conditioning on piriform cortical single-unit responses to the learned aversive odor, as well as its effects on similar (overlapping mixtures) in freely moving rats. We found that odor-evoked fear responses were training paradigm dependent. Simple association of a condition stimulus positive (CS+) odor with foot shock (unconditioned stimulus) led to generalized fear (cue-evoked freezing) to similar odors. However, after differential conditioning, which included trials where a CS- odor (a mixture overlapping with the CS+) was not paired with shock, freezing responses were CS+ odor specific and less generalized. Pseudoconditioning led to no odor-evoked freezing. These differential levels of stimulus control over freezing were associated with different training-induced changes in single-unit odor responses in anterior piriform cortex (aPCX). Both simple and differential conditioning induced a significant decrease in aPCX single-unit spontaneous activity compared with pretraining levels while pseudoconditioning did not. Simple conditioning enhanced mean receptive field size (breadth of tuning) of the aPCX units, while differential conditioning reduced mean receptive field size. These results suggest that generalized fear is associated with an impairment of olfactory cortical discrimination. Furthermore, changes in sensory processing are dependent on the nature of training and can predict the stimulus-controlled behavioral outcome of the training.  相似文献   

18.
At 5 days of age, rat pups were treated with a combination of desmethylimipramine (DMI) and 6-hydroxydopamine (6-OHDA) to selectively deplete brain dopamine (DA) or with vehicle (saline) control solutions. Two days later, all animals received conditioning to a novel odor by pairing the odor with intraoral milk. When the odor was anise, treated pups spent less time near the conditioned stimulus than did controls, but there were no 6-OHDA effects when the stimulus was a lemon odor (Experiment I). The difference in performance between the treated and control animals was not attributable to alterations in activating effects of the reinforcer (Experiment I), changes in olfactory sensitivity or olfactory preference (Experiment II), or sensitization to the stimulus (Experiment III). In Experiment IV, animals received d-amphetamine sulfate (0.5 mg/kg) prior to conditioning, testing, or both conditioning and testing. Amphetamine treatment before conditioning produced an improvement in performance in animals previously treated with 6-OHDA/DMI, but it impaired performance in controls, regardless of the time of injection. The results indicate a role of brain DA in learning in young rats.  相似文献   

19.
To support nipple attachment and huddling, rat pups must learn to approach and prefer maternal odor. Similar to other altricial species, rat pups have a sensitive period for learning this odor preference, which ends around postnatal day (PN) 10 and coincides with the emergence of walking. One characteristic of this sensitive period is that an odor paired with moderate shock elicits an odor preference. After PN10, this behavioral training produces an odor aversion, although pain threshold remains unchanged. Recently, we demonstrated that the endogenous opioid system might be a key element in the acquisition of the shock-induced odor preference during the sensitive period since antagonism of this system disrupts odor preference learning. In older pups, acquisition of a shock-induced odor aversion was unaffected by opioid system manipulation. The purpose of these experiments was to further elucidate the role of opioids in infant olfactory learning through assessment of memory consolidation and expression during and after the sensitive period. In Experiment 1, we demonstrate that naltrexone (NTX), a nonspecific opioid antagonist, given immediately following odor-shock conditioning during the sensitive period, blocks odor preference formation and yields an odor aversion. However, the same treatment does not disrupt consolidation of an odor aversion in older pups. In Experiment 2, we demonstrate that during the sensitive period, NTX disrupts expression of the shock-induced odor preference, but not the learned odor aversion in older pups. Results using this model of attachment suggest that opioids have an important role in the acquisition, consolidation, and expression of early olfactory preferences. Furthermore, since prenatal drug exposure is known to alter the endogenous opioid system, these results highlight the capacity of prenatal opiate exposure to disrupt early infant learning and attachment.  相似文献   

20.
Developmental change in the access to olfactory memories   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Memory for a learned odor preference can be functionally confined to one side of the brain in 6-day-old rat pups by preferentially stimulating a single naris and corresponding olfactory bulb during training. We report here that this form of unilateral learning is present only during the first postnatal week; older pups show bilateral recall of unilateral olfactory experience. The maturation of bilateral learning probably depends on the postnatal growth and development of olfactory commissural fibers, because infantlike unilateral learning and memory is reinstated when these commissural fibers are sectioned before training in older pups. Section of commissural fibers after training also resulted in unilateral preferences. This latter finding indicates that the learned odor preference of older pups tested with the untrained naris open depends on access to unilaterally stored memories on the contralateral side, access provided by the newly developed commissural projections.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号