首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 140 毫秒
1.
The present study investigated the effects of dexmedetomidine (an alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonist), atipamezole (an alpha-2 adrenoceptor antagonist) and tacrine (an inhibitor of acetylcholinesterase) on the performance of adult and aged rats in a delayed non-matching to position task assessing spatial short-term memory. Most of the aged rats were impaired in the pretraining phases and in the acquisition of the non-delayed version of the task. After a substantial training period of the delayed version of the task, both adult and aged rats reached their asymptotic level of performance. Both adult and aged rats showed a decline in the percent correct responses at the longest delays in this task, and a delay independent decrease in the percent correct responses across the delays (0–30 s) was found in the group of aged rats (25-month-old) as compared to the adults (10-month-old). Dexmedetomidine (0.3, 1.0 or 3.0 µg/kg), atipamezole (0.03, 0.3 or 3.0 mg/kg) and tacrine (1.0 or 3.0 mg/kg) did not increase the percent correct responses in adult or aged rats. The highest doses of dexmedetomidine and tacrine decreased behavioural activity of rats during this short-term memory testing. Atipamezole (0.03 mg/kg) increased behavioural activity of rats. The results suggest that acute, systemic administrations of alpha-2 drugs or an anticholinesterase do not improve short-term memory in rats.  相似文献   

2.
Noradrenergic alpha-2 agonists such as clonidine and guanfacine improve working memory performance in aged monkeys. Guanfacine also improves cognition in young monkeys, but there are conflicting reports of the effects of clonidine in young adult human and nonhuman primates. In the present study, high doses of clonidine (0.02-0.1 mg/kg) significantly improved performance of the delayed response task, a test of spatial working memory, in young adult monkeys. Lower doses (0.0001-0.01 mg/kg), similar to those used in human studies (0.001-0.003 mg/kg), had no effect on task performance. In contrast, monkeys experimentally depleted of catecholamines by chronic reserpine treatment have been improved by both dose ranges. These results provide further support for the hypothesis that alpha-2 agonists improve cognition via actions at post-synaptic alpha-2 receptors, and suggest that conflicting results with clonidine in previous studies of prefrontal cortical function may result from insufficient dosage.  相似文献   

3.
The present study investigates the effects of an alpha 2 antagonist, atipamezole (Ati), on the high-voltage spindle (HVS; Ati at 0.1, 1.0, and 3.0 mg/kg) activity, passive avoidance retention (PA; Ati at 3 mg/kg; injected before retention trial), and water maze (WM; Ati at 3 mg/kg; injected after daily training trials) acquisition of young and aged rats. PA retention trial performance defect of aged rats was partially alleviated by Ati at a 3-mg/kg dose. Ati at 3 mg/kg had no effect on the PA performance of young rats. Retention trial performance of nonshocked young or aged rats was not altered by a 3-mg/kg Ati dose. WM acquisition was not affected by posttraining Ati injections. Age-related increase of HVS was stabilized by Ati at 1 or 3 mg/kg. Ati at 1 and 3 mg/kg completely suppressed HVS of young rats. Ati at 0.1 mg/kg had no effect on HVS of young or aged rats. The results suggest that alpha 2-antagonist-administration-induced increase in noradrenergic activity may stabilize age-related HVS activity increase and PA performance defect.  相似文献   

4.
Acute and chronic nicotine effects on working memory in aged rats   总被引:10,自引:4,他引:6  
Acute and chronic nicotine administration has been repeatedly been found in our laboratory to improve working memory performance of normal adult rats in the radial-arm maze. The current study was conducted to determine if acute or chronic nicotine administration would improve working memory performance in aged rats. Sixteen young adult (3–7 months) and 32 aged (24–28 months) male Sprague-Dawley rats were trained on an eight-arm radial maze. A significant age-related choice deficit was seen during the 21 sessions of training. After training, half of the rats in each age group were implanted with nicotine-containing osmotic minipumps and the other half implanted with vehicle-containing pumps. Consistent with previous work, the young adult rats given chronic nicotine (approximately 5 mg/kg per day as measured as nicotine base) showed a significant improvement in working memory performance. In contrast, the aged rats did not show a significant effect of this dose of chronic nicotine. After a 2 week withdrawal period the remaining rats underwent a series of acute drug challenges with nicotinic and muscarinic agonists and antagonists as well as the dopaminergic antagonist haloperidol. Mecamylamine and haloperidol impaired the memory performance of the young adult rats, whereas the aged rats showed no effect. In contrast, scopolamine impaired performance of both young adult and aged rats in a similar manner. Both pilocarpine and nicotine improved the memory performance of the aged rats, but did not improve the young adult rats, possibly due to a ceiling effect on performance. During the cholinergic agonist drug phase, the aged rats which had previously been given chronic nicotine infusions showed better performance than those which had not. The resistance of the aged rats to chronic nicotine-induced working memory improvements and acute mecamylamine-induced working memory deficits may have resulted from the decline in nicotinic receptors seen with aging. Chronic co-administration of the nicotinic antagonist mecamylamine in a previous study was found to abolish the chronic nicotine-induced working memory improvement. The aged rats were resistant to haloperidol-induced deficits which may have resulted from the decrease in dopaminergic receptors seen with aging. Interestingly, acute cholinergic agonists including nicotine did improve working memory performance in the aged rats and previous chronic nicotine infusion was beneficial during the period of acute cholinergic agonist challenge. This suggests that nicotinic treatment may be of use for treating age associated memory impairments but that special dosing regimens may be required.This work was carried out in partial fulfillment of a BSc (Hons) in Biochemistry at the University of Bath (UK) by D.T.  相似文献   

5.
A series of experiments examined the effects of 5-HT1A ligands alone and in combination with the muscarinic antagonist scopolamine on short term working memory in the rat. The behavioural paradigm was a discrete trial, operant delayed matching to position task, with delays of 0, 5, 15 and 30 s. The 5-HT1A ligands tested were the full agonist, 8-OH DPAT (0, 0.1, 0.3 and 1 mg/kg), the partial agonist, ipsapirone (0, 1, 3 and 10 mg/kg), and the purported antagonist, NAN 190 (0, 1, 2, and 4 mg/kg). 1-PP (0, 0.1, 0.3, 1 mg/kg), the major metabolite of ipsapirone, was also tested. The lowest dose of 8-OH DPAT significantly improved matching accuracy at the longest delay, whereas the highest dose impaired matching accuracy and increased the latency to respond. Ipsapirone also significantly improved the accuracy of performance at a dose of 3 mg/kg, but the doses of 1 and 10 mg/kg did not significantly affect performance. NAN-190, at the highest dose tested (4 mg/kg), impaired matching accuracy, whereas the two lower doses did not significantly affect performance. The highest dose also increased the latency to respond. 1-PP had no effect on performance. Scopolamine HBr (0.14 mg/kg) caused a delay dependent impairment in matching accuracy, and had no effect on missed trials or the latency to respond. Low doses of 8-OH DPAT (0.1 and 0.3 mg/kg) significantly attenuated the scopolamine induced accuracy impairment, whereas 1 mg/kg 8-OH DPAT potentiated the impairment. Ipsapirone (3 mg/kg) also significantly improved the performance of scopolamine impaired rats. NAN-190 increased the latency to respond and reduced the number of nose pokes made during the delays in scopolamine-treated rats, and tended to potentiate the scopolamine-induced accuracy impairment. 1-PP did not affect the performance of scopolamine treated rats. Taken together, these results suggest that modulation of 5-HT1A receptors influences short term spatial working memory in the rat.  相似文献   

6.
The aim of the present study was to evaluate the effects of chronic and acute treatment by the Gingko biloba extract, EGb 761 (IPSEN, France) on olfactory short-term memory in rats, using a spontaneous recognition procedure. The effects of a daily EGb 761 treatment (30 or 60 mg/kg) over a period of 30 days (Experiment 1) were evaluated in young male rats. Those of a single injection of EGb 761 were assessed either in young male rats at 60 or 120 mg/kg (Experiment 2) or in aged female rats at 60 mg/kg (Experiment 3). Results showed that, at the highest dose (60 mg/kg), chronic EGb 761 treatment enhanced the recognition performances, allowing recognition at delays at which control animals did not show any recognition. Acute treatment enhanced recognition at both doses tested. The results of the third experiment showed that EGb 761 had an overall enhancement effect on the performances of aged rats. In summary, our results provide evidence for a short-term memory enhancement effect of EGb 761 in both young and aged rats.  相似文献   

7.
Young (5 month) and aged (23 month) male rats were tested in delayed nonmatching to position task using a T-maze, and their ability of working memory retention was assessed over interrun intervals ranging between 5 and 300 sec. There were no significant age differences in pretest performance at 0 sec interval, but significant memory loss was observed in aged rats when tested with the interrun intervals. Physostigmine (0.1 and 0.2 mg/kg) improved this age-related decline in working memory in a dose-dependent manner, whereas the treatment slightly but not significantly improved the performance of young rats. These results suggest that the central cholinergic system in aged rats was functionally deteriorated and that stimulation of the system could enhance working memory retention in aged rats.  相似文献   

8.
 In aged monkeys with naturally occurring catecholamine depletion, α-2 adrenergic agonists such as guanfacine have repeatedly been shown to improve dorsolateral prefrontal cortical function, as assessed by the spatial delayed response task. Both low (0.0001–0.001 mg/kg) and high (0.5 mg/kg) but not intermediate (0.01–0.05 mg/kg) doses of guanfacine improve spatial working memory performance in aged animals. However, it is not known whether guanfacine would similarly improve performance in young animals. In the present study, the effects of guanfacine on delayed response performance were characterized in seven young adult rhesus monkeys. Low doses of guanfacine (0.0001–0.01 mg/kg) had no effect on task performance, while high doses of guanfacine (0.1–0.7 mg/kg) significantly improved task performance. The highest doses produced mild sedation that was independent of drug effects on delayed response. The most effective dose of guanfacine was challenged with the α-2 antagonist idazoxan (0.1 mg/kg). This dose of idazoxan had no effect on task performance when given alone. Consistent with an α-2 mechanism, idazoxan significantly decreased delayed response performance in guanfacine-treated animals. These results support the hypothesis that delayed response performance in young intact animals can be improved through actions at α-2 adrenergic receptors. Received: 25 January 1997 / Final version: 15 October 1997  相似文献   

9.
The present study had two goals (1) to examine the effects of treatments with nicotinic agonist (nicotine) and antagonist (mecamylamine) on working memory in normal adult rats (14 months of age), and (2) to determine if treating aged (36 to 42 months of age), memory-impaired rats with nicotine could improve their memory function. Memory testing was carried out using a delayed non-matching to position paradigm in a T-maze. Rats were trained to run down one arm of the maze (e.g., right) on an information run and then, after a variable memory delay period of 10, 90, or 180 sec, run down the other arm (e.g., left). In normal adult rats after water injection, memory accuracy was inversely related to memory delay (> 90, 75, and 67% accuracy at the 10, 90, and 180 sec delays, respectively). Administration of nicotine (0.1 or 0.4 mg/kg), or mecamylamine (2.5 or 5 mg/kg), or combinations of these drugs had no significant effects on memory in these rats. However, mecamylamine alone or in combination with nicotine reduced running speed in the T-maze in normal adults, indicating that the drugs did produce some behavioral effects. In aged rats, memory accuracy after water injection was much lower than that of younger adults, averaging 64, 57, and 50% at 10, 90, and 180 sec delays. Interestingly, nicotine injections of 0.1 or 0.4 mg/kg resulted in highly significantly improved accuracy in the memory task. The deficit in memory accuracy in aged rats, evident even at the shortest delay period (10 sec), suggests that the rats may have had impairments in attention or visual discriminatory ability. If this is so, then the beneficial effects of nicotine observed in this study may be more related to its effects on attention rather than memory directly. © 1994 Wiley-Liss, Inc.  相似文献   

10.
The present study assessed the effects of the indirect acetylcholinesterase inhibitor metrifonate on learning and memory functions in young (3-month-old) and aged (25-month-old) rats. In the shuttle box, metrifonate at a dose of 12.5mg/kg, p.o., 30min before each of the daily acquisition sessions, improved the acquisition of the active avoidance response, whereas a dose of 25mg/kg did not. Metrifonate, 12.5mg/kg, p.o., administered before each of the daily acquisition sessions, also facilitated the acquisition of the Morris water escape task in both young and aged rats: metrifonate-treated rats swam a shorter distance to reach the escape platform than did the vehicle-treated rats. The 3-month-old rats treated with metrifonate did not show the increase in swimming speed over training observed in vehicle-treated animals; no effects of metrifonate were found on the swimming speed of aged rats. In a probe trial carried out immediately after the fifth daily acquisition session, metrifonate treatment did not affect the bias of the aged rats for the quadrant in which the platform had been positioned during acquisition. It is concluded that metrifonate improves performance during the acquisition phase of two aversively motivated learning and memory tasks at the dose of 12.5mg/kg, p.o.  相似文献   

11.
The effects of SR 57746A during acquisition and on stabilized performance of a delayed alternation task in a Skinner Box (Model E 10.10, Coulbourn Instruments, Lehigh Valley, PA) were investigated in aged rats (22–26 months old). Test sessions consisted of repeated trials during which animals were first presented with a single lever (left or right) followed several seconds later by two levers. A press on the lever opposite to that presented previously (delayed alternation) was rewarded. The number of correct responses and the reaction times to the one- and two-lever presentations were recorded. SR 57746A (0.3 and 3 mg/kg p.o.) was first evaluated during the acquisition of the delayed alternation, given before each of 10 daily acquisition sessions to different groups of aged animals. The imposed delay was 5 sec. Afterward, training continued without drug until asymptotic performance was reached. Rats were then given repeated drug test sessions during which different intervals (5, 10, and 20 sec) were presented in a random order. During this phase, both young and aged animals received all the different treatments (SR 57746A at 0.3, 1, and 3 mg/kg and vehicle p.o.). Aged animals showed clear deficits during both acquisition and stabilized performance phases. SR 57746A, particularly at the highest dose tested (3 mg/kg p.o.), clearly increased response accuracy of aged animals both during acquisition of the delayed alternation and during stabilized performance. In the latter phase, the facilitation of response accuracy was observed only in conditions (10 sec retention delay) where aged animals showed a deficit compared with young animals. SR 57746A did not affect response accuracy during stabilized performance in young animals at any delay. The drug-induced increase in response accuracy in aged animals occurred at a dose of SR 57746A (3 mg/kg p.o.) that lengthened reaction times, particularly simple reaction times, during both acquisition and stabilized performance and also lengthened reaction times in young animals during the stabilized performance phase. These results suggest that SR 57746A specifically attenuates age-related short-term memory impairment in rats. © 1995 Wiley-Liss, Inc.  相似文献   

12.
Both normal aging and age-related disease, such as Alzheimer's disease, have diverse effects on forebrain-dependent cognitive tasks as well as the underlying neurobiological substrates. The purpose of the current study was to investigate if age-related alterations in the function of the cholinergic system are associated with memory impairments in auditory-cued and contextual fear conditioning. Young (2-3 months) and aged (19-20 months) C57BL/6 mice were administered scopolamine (0.1, 0.3, 0.5, or 1.0 mg/kg), a muscarinic cholinergic receptor antagonist, mecamylamine (1.0 and 2.0 mg/kg), a nicotinic cholinergic receptor antagonist, both scopolamine and mecamylamine (0.1 and 1.0 mg/kg, respectively), or saline prior to training. Training consisted of two white-noise CS (85 dB, 30 s)-footshock US (0.57 mA, 2 s) presentations. Testing occurred 48 h post-training. Scopolamine administration impaired contextual and cued fear conditioning in young and aged mice, although the aged mice were less sensitive to disruption by scopolamine. Mecamylamine did not disrupt conditioned fear in the young or aged mice. Scopolamine and mecamylamine co-administration, at doses sub-threshold for disrupting fear conditioning with separate administration, disrupted contextual and auditory-cued fear conditioning in the young mice, indicating that in the young mice the muscarinic and nicotinic cholinergic processes interact in the formation and maintenance of long-term memories for conditioned fear. Co-administration of both antagonists did not disrupt fear conditioning in the aged mice, indicating that age-related alterations in the cholinergic receptor subtypes may occur.  相似文献   

13.
Social short-term memory in rodents is based on the recognition of a juvenile by an adult conspecific when the juvenile is presented on two successive occasions. Cannabimimetics are claimed to induce memory deficits in both humans and animals. In the brain, they mainly bind to CB1 receptors for which anandamide is a purported endogenous ligand. SR 141716, a specific antagonist of CB1 receptors, dose-dependently reverses biochemical and pharmacological effects of cannabimimetics. More particularly, it antagonizes the inhibition of hippocampal long-term potentiation induced by WIN 55,212-2 and anandamide, and it increases arousal when given alone. The present experiments study the ability of SR 141716 (from 0.03 to 3 mg/kg SC) to facilitate short-term olfactory memory in the social recognition test in rodents. SR 141716 improved social recognition in a long intertrial paradigm with a threshold dose of 0.1 mg/kg SC. At 1 mg/kg, it antagonized the memory disturbance elicited by retroactive inhibition. Scopolamine (0.06 mg/kg IP) partially reversed its memory-enhancing effect. Moreover, SR 141716 reduced memory deficit in aged rats (0.03–0.1 mg/kg) and mice (0.3–1 mg/kg). As SR 141716 is not known to exhibit any pharmacological activity which is not mediated by CB1 receptors, the results strongly support the concept that blockade of CB1 receptors plays an important role in consolidation of short-term memory in rodents and suggest there may be a role for an endogenous cannabinoid agonist tone (anandaminergic) in forgetting.  相似文献   

14.
Scopolamine dose-dependently inhibits passive avoidance latency and decreases spontaneous alternation in the Y-maze, suggesting effects on long-term and short-term memory, respectively. Chlorisondamine (10 mg/kg), a compound which produces a long-lasting central nicotinic receptor blockade, did not affect short-term and long-term memory performance. In normal rats, nicotine at the doses of 0.3, 1.0, and 3.0 mg/kg administered once had a facilitating effect on short-term memory; a higher dose (3.0 mg/kg) did not show a more pronounced effect than a lower one (0.3 mg/kg). Nicotine, by activating the nicotinic acetylcholine receptors, attenuated the impairment of short-term memory induced by muscarinic or dopamine D2 receptor blockade. On long-term memory, a single dose of nicotine (0.3, 1.0, 3.0 mg/kg) did not affect memory performance, but improved it after chronic (10 consecutive days, 0.3 mg/kg) administration. The antiamnesic effect of nicotine administered once was observed in scopolamine-, scopolamine+chlorisondamine- or sulpiride-treated rats. These results suggest that the antiamnesic effect of nicotine can result from an action at nicotinic receptors subtypes not blocked by chlorisondamine or at nonnicotinic receptors.  相似文献   

15.
The present study examined the role of the 5-HT1B receptor in learning and memory. The ability of the 5-HT1B receptor agonist anpirtoline and the selective 5-HT1B receptor antagonist NAS-181 to affect spatial learning in the water maze (WM) and aversive learning in the passive avoidance (PA) task were examined in the rat. Anpirtoline (0.1-1.0 mg/kg, s.c.) caused a dose-dependent impairment of learning and memory in both the WM and PA tasks. NAS-181 (1.0-10 mg/kg, s.c.) failed to alter performance of the WM task, but produced a dose-dependent (0.1-20 mg/kg) facilitation of PA retention. Furthermore, treatment with NAS-181 (10 mg/kg) fully blocked the impairment of the WM and PA performance caused by anpirtoline (1.0 mg/kg). In contrast, NAS-181 (3.0-10 mg/kg) did not attenuate the spatial learning deficit and the impairment of PA retention caused by scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg in WM task, 0.3 mg/kg in PA task, s.c.), a nonselective muscarinic antagonist. Moreover, a subthreshold dose of scopolamine (0.1 mg/kg) blocked the facilitation of PA retention induced by NAS-181 (1.0-10 mg/kg). In addition, the behavioral disturbances (eg thigmotaxic swimming and platform deflections) induced by anpirtoline and scopolamine were analyzed in the WM task and correlated with WM performance. These results indicate that: (1) 5-HT1B receptor stimulation and blockade result in opposite effects in two types of cognitive tasks in the rat, and that (2) the 5-HT1B antagonist NAS-181 can facilitate some aspects of cognitive function, most likely via an increase of cholinergic transmission. These results suggest that 5-HT1B receptor antagonists may have a potential in the treatment of cognitive deficits resulting from loss of cholinergic transmission.  相似文献   

16.
Nicotinic systems have been shown in numerous studies to be important for spatial working memory. Nicotinic systems are certainly not acting alone in the basis of memory function, but act in concert with a variety of other neural systems. Important for these interactions is nicotinic induced release of a variety of neurotransmitters involved in memory function including serotonin (5-HT). We have found that the 5-HT2 receptor antagonist, ketanserin, effectively attenuated nicotine-induced attentional improvement. The current study explored the interaction between nicotinic and serotonergic systems in the performance of a spatial working memory task in the radial-arm maze. Female Sprague-Dawley rats were trained on the win-shift working memory task on the 8-arm radial maze. After 18 sessions of acquisition training the rats were given acute doses of nicotine (0.2 and 0.4 mg/kg), ketanserin (0.5, 1 and 2 mg/kg) or combinations of the two. The vehicle served as the control. As seen in previous studies, nicotine caused a significant improvement in working memory performance as indexed by the number of correct arm entries before the first error (entries to repeat). Ketanserin at the doses tested did not cause a significant effect on choice accuracy, but it did significantly attenuate the improvement caused by the 0.2 mg/kg nicotine dose. The higher 0.4 mg/kg nicotine dose was nearly sufficient to overcome the ketanserin effect. This study shows that, as with attentional function, nicotine-induced working memory improvement is attenuated by the 5-HT2 antagonist ketanserin. Given that many antipsychotic drugs have substantial 5-HT2 antagonist effects, these atypical antipsychotic drugs may reduce the cognitive improvements caused by nicotinic treatments.  相似文献   

17.
Several experiments were conducted to study the effects of the noncompetitive N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist, MK-801, on learning and memory in the rat. Rats displayed impaired performance on several sensorimotor tests and appeared grossly intoxicated when treated IP with 0.2 mg/kg MK-801, but not when treated with lower doses (0.05 or 0.1 mg/kg). Postacquisition performance on two spatial learning tasks involving working memory protocols (reinforced alternation and radial arm maze) was impaired by MK-801 at intoxicating doses (0.2 mg/kg) but not at lower doses (0.05 or 0.1 mg/kg). Using a position habit reversal task, we found that rats could learn to reverse a position habit while under the influence of a nonintoxicating dose of MK-801 (0.1 mg/kg), but when tested on the following day performed as if they did not recall what they had learned. Thus, acute administration of a nonintoxicating dose of MK-801 disrupts the retention of new information learned under the influence of the drug but does not interfere with the performance of tasks that are well learned before the drug is administered. Whether the performance deficits on the spatial learning tasks observed only following intoxicating doses of MK-801 reflect an effect on memory is not clear.  相似文献   

18.
The present study investigated whether alpha-1 adrenergic and glutamatergic N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-mediated mechanisms interact in memory processes, by examining the effects of individual and combined systemic administration of ST 587, a putative alpha-1 agonist, and D-cycloserine (DCS), a partial agonist at the glycine-B binding site of the NMDA receptor, on the performance of rats in non-delayed and delayed (4-6 h) foraging behaviour in the radial arm maze task, using the delayed non-matching to sample (DNMTS) version. The results indicated that DCS (5.0 mg/kg) decreased working memory errors, i.e. the number of re-entries into the previously visited arms during the sampling phase. In addition, both ST 587 (100 microg/kg) and DCS (10 mg/kg), when administered alone 30 min before a sampling phase, improved retention of this task as reflected by the increased number of correct choices before the first error during the retention phase. The combined administration of ST 587 and DCS, however, did not lead to better retention in the DNMTS task compared with the administration of each of the drugs alone. Combinations of sub-threshold doses of ST 587 (50 or 75 microg/kg) and DCS (5.0 or 7.5 mg/kg) also did not improve retention in this task. DCS (5.0 or 7.5 mg/kg) increased activity as indicated by the increased number of arm entries in a given time during the sampling phase. These findings suggest that the systemic administration of a positive modulator of the NMDA receptor facilitates hippocampal-dependent memory functions, but that these effects are not enhanced by combined administration with an alpha-1 agonist, even though the alpha-1 agonist is effective when given alone. The results support the idea that NMDA receptors modulate both mnemonic and non-mnemonic functions in the brain.  相似文献   

19.
In the present studies we investigated the actions of ondansetron, a prototypic 5-hydroxytryptamine3 (5-HT3) receptor antagonist, on performance in a complex spatial navigation/memory task in rats. Specifically, we compared the activity of ondansetron to that of the cholinesterase inhibitor physostigmine in attenuating two distinct cognitive deficits in the Morris water maze. In the first model, rats treated with the muscarinic receptor antagonist atropine (30 mg/kg) had significantly longer latencies to find the submerged platform across two days of testing. Physostigmine (0.03, 0.1 and 0.3 mg/kg) and ondansetron (0.03–1 mg/kg) significantly reduced the latencies to find the submerged platform in atropine-treated animals, suggesting an increase in cognitive performance. There was little evidence of a dose-response relationship for either compound, and a loss of efficacy for ondansetron was seen at 3 mg/kg. In the second model, pre-screened, aged (23 months), cognition-impaired and nonimpaired rats were tested. Ondansetron (0.1 mg/kg), but not physostigmine (0.1 mg/kg), decreased the latencies to find the submerged platform in the aged-impaired rats, while neither compound improved performance of aged-nonimpaired rats. These data suggest that ondansetron may have cognition enhancing properties in animal models of aging and cholinergic hypofunction.  相似文献   

20.
This experiment investigated the hypothesis that nicotine-induced regional release of noradrenaline contributes to the improvements in radial maze performance following nicotine treatment in rats with lesions to the forebrain cholinergic projection system (FCPS), by examining whether pretreatment with the noradrenergic beta-receptor antagonist propranolol abolished the facilitative effects of nicotine. After S-AMPA (8.0mM) lesions to the nuclei of origin of the FCPS in the nucleus basalis and medial septal areas, rats displayed long-lasting impairment in long-term reference and short-term working memory in both spatial (place) and associative (cue) radial maze tasks. Performance of control and lesioned rats was assessed after administration of nicotine (0.1mg/kg), propranolol (either 0.5 or 5.0mg/kg) and both treatments. Nicotine reduced working memory error rates in lesioned animals, but did not affect the performance of controls. Propranolol dose-relatedly increased error rates in both control and lesioned animals. Adverse effects were more marked in controls, all four types of error being increased under the high dose of propranolol, whereas in lesioned rats significant increases in error rates above baseline were confined to working memory. The low dose of propranolol, in conjunction with nicotine, abolished the improvement in working memory seen with nicotine alone in lesioned rats. However, under joint treatment with the high dose, the substantial increases in working memory error rates seen in lesioned rats after propranolol alone were reduced to baseline level. In controls, reduction in errors to baseline was seen only in the cue task; place task errors remained significantly elevated. These results suggest that both cholinergic depletion and noradrenergic blockade exert disruptive effects on cognition, but that these effects are largely independent, since an additive or interactive mechanism would be predicted to produce greater disruption, following noradrenergic blockade, in lesioned rather than in control animals. Although facilitative effects of nicotine were abolished with the low dose of propranolol, the results further suggest that these effects are independent of release of noradrenaline, since nicotine continued to reduce errors in control and lesioned animals following blockade of beta receptors with the high dose of propranolol.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号