首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Acute effects of d-amphetamine in a monkey operant behavioral test battery   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
The acute effects of d-amphetamine were assessed using a battery of complex food-reinforced operant tasks that included responding in delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS, n = 6), conditioned position responding (CPR, n = 7), progressive ratio (PR, n = 8), temporal response differentiation (TRD, n = 4), and incremental repeated acquisition (IRA, n = 9) tasks. Performance in these tasks is thought to depend upon specific brain functions such as short-term memory and attention (DMTS), color and position discrimination (CPR), motivation to work for food (PR), time perception (TRD), and learning (IRA). d-Amphetamine sulfate (0.01-1.0 mg/kg IV), given 15-min pression produced significant dose-dependent decreases in the number of reinforcers obtained in each task. Response accuracy was significantly decreased at doses of 0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg for TRD and at 1.0 mg/kg for CPR when compared to saline injections. Accuracy was not consistently affected in the DMTS or IRA tasks. Response rates decreased or response latencies increased significantly at doses of 0.3 and 1.0 mg/kg in the PR and DMTS tasks. A dose of 0.1 mg/kg for the IRA and TRD tasks, 0.3 mg/kg for DMTS and 1.0 mg/kg for the CPR tasks significantly decreased percent task completed. Thus, the relative sensitivities of these tasks for detecting d-amphetamine behavioral effects were IRA = TRD greater than PR = DMTS greater than CPR.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

2.
Multiple behavioral effects of diazepam in rhesus monkeys   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The acute effects of diazepam (Valium) were assessed using a battery of complex food-reinforced operant tasks that included responding in delayed matching to sample (DMTS, n = 5), conditioned position response (CPR, n = 7) progressive ratio (PR, n = 8), temporal response differentiation (TRD, n = 4), and incremental repeated acquisition (IRA, n = 9) tests. Diazepam (0.25-4.0 mg/kg IV) produced significant dose-dependent decreases in the number of reinforcers obtained in the TRD and IRA tasks only. TRD accuracy was significantly decreased at doses of 0.25, 1.0, 2.0, and 4.0 mg/kg when compared to vehicle injections. Significant decreases in IRA accuracy generally did not occur at doses below 1.0 mg/kg. DMTS accuracy was decreased at 0.5 mg/kg for some time delays but showed no clear dose-delay interaction. Performance in the CPR and PR tests showed no significant effects of diazepam exposure over the dose range tested. These results indicate that diazepam selectively disrupts performance of operant tasks in monkeys designed to model human correlates of time perception, learning ability and visual attention/short-term memory while not affecting tasks designed to model motivation and position/color discrimination.  相似文献   

3.
The effects of acute chlorpromazine treatment were assessed using a complex operant test battery (OTB) containing five tasks thought to depend upon processes associated with short-term memory and attention [delayed-matching-to-sample (DMTS)], color and position discrimination [conditioned position responding (CPR)], motivation [progressive ratio (PR)], time perception [temporal response differentiation (TRD)], and learning [incremental repeated acquisition (IRA)]. Adult male rhesus monkeys were tested 15 min after IV injection of saline or chlorpromazine (0.010, 0.030, 0.100, or 0.175 mg/kg). Behavioral endpoints measured included percent task completed, response rate or latency, and response accuracy. The order of task sensitivity to disruption by chlorpromazine was TRD = PR = IRA = DMTS = CPR in which sensitivity was defined as a significant alteration in any aspect of task performance. Chlorpromazine slowed response rates in all tasks except TRD but did decrease accuracy in that task. These effects were similar to those noted in previous studies of acute chlorpromazine treatment. Specific motoric effects suggested decreased task initiation at doses that left general motor ability intact. This finding is similar to that noted in parkinsonism caused by chronic chlorpromazine treatment.  相似文献   

4.
The effects of physostigmine were assessed in rhesus macaques using behavior in several complex tasks designed to model aspects of time estimation [temporal response differentiation (TRD)], short-term memory [delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS)], motivation [progressive ratio (PR)], learning [incremental repeated acquisition (IRA)], and color and position discrimination [conditioned position responding (CPR)]. The endpoints monitored included percent task completed, response rate, and accuracy. Physostigmine sulphate (0.001–0.056 mg/kg) significantly decreased the percentage of task completed and response rate in each task at 0.03 and 0.056 mg/kg. Accuracy in the TRD task was significantly decreased at 0.03 and 0.056 mg/kg, whereas accuracy in the CPR and IRA tasks was significantly decreased only at 0.056 mg/kg. DMTS accuracy was not significantly affected at any dose tested. A significant increase in accuracy was noted in learning task performance at the 0.01 mg/kg dose, although only for one-lever response sequences. Performance enhancements were not seen in any other task. These results indicate that in monkeys, low doses of physostigmine may facilitate acquisition or learning of simple one-lever spatial tasks while not significantly altering the acquisition of similar but more complex tasks. Impaired task performance at high doses may be more reflective of cholinomimetic side effects (tremor and hypothermia) that affect response rate than a central or “cognitive” impairment.  相似文献   

5.
The present experiment assessed nicotine's effects on complex cognitive processes using a variety of operant tasks in rats, including incremental repeated acquisition (IRA) to assess learning; conditioned position responding (CPR) to assess auditory, visual, and position discrimination; progressive ratio (PR) to assess motivation; temporal response differentiation (TRD) to assess timing; and differential reinforcement of low response rates (DRL) to assess timing and response inhibition. Acute nicotine administration (0.0, 0.3, 0.42, 0.56, 0.75, and 1.0 mg/kg, IP) increased IRA and CPR response rate without significantly altering accuracy. Nicotine had similar effects on response rate for PR. For TRD, nicotine had a U-shaped dose effect on accuracy, but failed to shift the mode of the TRD response distribution. For DRL, nicotine reduced accuracy and also shifted the mode of the DRL response initiation time distribution to the left. Nicotine produced an inverted U-shaped dose-effect curve for the overall number of "bursting" responses under both of these schedules. The results of this experiment suggest that nicotine can impair performance on some aspects of cognitive-behavioral performance, while simultaneously improving performance on others.  相似文献   

6.
Methylphenidate (MPH) is an amphetamine derivative widely prescribed for the treatment of attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder. Recent concern over its genotoxic potential in children [11] spurred a study on the effects of chronic MPH treatment in a nonhuman primate population and the studies reported here were conducted in conjunction with that study in the same animals. Here, the focus was on the ability of juvenile rhesus monkeys to learn how to perform a battery of operant behavioral tasks while being treated chronically with MPH. Performance of the National Center for Toxicological Research (NCTR) Operant Test Battery (OTB) was used to quantify the learning of tasks thought to model specific aspects of cognitive function including learning, motivation, color and position discrimination, and short-term memory. The OTB tasks designed to assess these specific behaviors included Incremental Repeated Acquisition (IRA), Progressive Ratio (PR), Conditioned Position Responding (CPR), and Delayed Matching-to-Sample (DMTS), respectively. Juvenile males (n = 10/group) pressed levers and press-plates for banana-flavored food pellets. Subjects were treated orally, twice a day, five days per week (M–F) for 66 weeks with escalating doses (0.15 mg/kg initially, increased to 2.5 mg/kg for the low dose group and to 12.5 mg/kg for the high dose group) and tested in OTB tasks 30 to 60 min after the morning dose. The findings indicate that MPH at doses up to 2.5 mg/kg twice per day were well tolerated (performance was no different than controls) but at doses of 12.5 mg/kg twice per day there was a significant decrement in OTB performance, characterized by decreases in both percent task completed and response rates for all tasks. These effects of MPH seem primarily due to decreases in motivation to perform for food, which is not surprising given the well known appetite suppressing effects of amphetamine-like stimulants. Thus, the current data do not strongly suggest cognitive impairments following chronic MPH administration.  相似文献   

7.
A battery of behavioral tasks, designed to monitor complex "cognitive" functions in nonhuman primates, has been in use for several years at the National Center for Toxicological Research. Subjects performing in this Operant Test Battery (OTB) work for food reinforcers and correct performance in each task contained in the OTB is thought to depend upon a relatively specific brain function. The specific tasks and the functions that they are thought to model are: delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS), short-term memory and attention; incremental repeated acquisition (IRA), learning; temporal response differentiation (TRD), time perception; progressive ratio (PR), motivation; and conditioned position responding (CPR), color and position discrimination. Data from various studies indicate that, in general: 1) OTB responding between subjects follows a normal or near-normal distribution; 2) performance in one task does not correlate very highly with performance in any of the other tasks; 3) females acquire correct OTB responding more rapidly than do males; and 4) the profile of disruption of task performance by acute drug administration varies depending upon drug class.  相似文献   

8.
Delayed match-to-sample (DMTS) procedures are among the most commonly used attention and memory tasks in behavioral pharmacology and have been utilized in a variety of species. Although macaque species such as the rhesus and cynomolgus macaque are often used for such studies, availability and disease transmission raise concerns over their use. The present study investigated whether the African green monkey might function as a suitable alternative by evaluating operant performance on a DMTS task and comparing this species' response to some commonly used drugs (0.025-0.075 mg/kg physostigmine, 0.0033-0.03 mg/kg scopolamine, 0.014-0.44 mg/kg atropine, 0.125-1.0 mg/kg midazolam, and 0.125-2.0 mg/kg diazepam) to the responses previously reported in macaques. Results demonstrated that African green monkeys are capable of learning and performing a DMTS task, and dose-effect functions for behavioral pharmacology were quite similar to those reported for rhesus macaques and other nonhuman primate species. Thus, the African green monkey may function as a suitable alternative to macaque species in behavioral pharmacology research.  相似文献   

9.
The purpose of the present experiment was to assess the effects of chronic MK-801 (an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist) and/or phenytoin (a sodium channel blocker) treatment on behavioral acquisition and performance in rats. Learning, audio/visual discrimination and motivation were modeled using incremental repeated acquisition (IRA), audio/visual discrimination (AVD) and progressive ratio (PR) tasks, respectively. MK-801 and/or phenytoin were administered daily, 7 days/week by orogastric gavage beginning just after weaning on postnatal day (PND) 23 and continuing until PND 306. Monday through Friday behavioral assessments began on PND 27 and continued until PND 430. Throughout treatment, subjects in the high dose MK-801 (1.0 mg/kg/day) and the high dose drug combination (1.0 mg/kg/day MK-801+150 mg/kg/day phenytoin) groups exhibited decreased body weight gains compared to control subjects. For these two affected groups, response rates were also decreased in all tasks. Task acquisition, as evidenced by an increase in response accuracy, was decreased for both these groups in the AVD task, but only for the high dose MK-801 group in the IRA task. The data suggest that chronic MK-801 treatment adversely affects the acquisition of IRA and AVD task performance and that the inclusion of phenytoin in the MK-801 dosing regimen blocks some of the adverse effects of chronic MK-801 treatment on IRA task acquisition. These findings are in marked contrast with those observed in nonhuman primates and suggest important species differences associated with chronic exposure to compounds that block NMDA receptors and/or sodium channels.  相似文献   

10.
Acute bolus doses of morphine induce a state of acute opioid dependence as measured by naloxone-precipitated withdrawal. Repeated morphine and precipitated withdrawal experience further enhances naloxone-induced withdrawal severity, partly because of direct neuroadaptation to repeated morphine, and partly because of conditioned associations of context and withdrawal experience. To determine whether a discrete tone/light conditioned stimulus could elicit conditioned withdrawal responses in acute dependence, rats trained on a fixed-ratio-15 operant schedule for food reward received morphine (5.6 mg/kg) 4x at daily or weekly intervals, with each morphine injection followed at 4 h by naloxone (1.0 mg/kg) and an operant session. The conditioned stimulus was presented to a Paired group after each naloxone injection. Separate control groups experienced the conditioned stimulus either at a different time of the day or on a different day of the week than naloxone (Unpaired), received naloxone without any conditioned stimulus exposure [Paired-no conditioned stimulus (Paired-NO CS)] or received vehicle instead of naloxone before conditioned stimulus presentation (NaI-Naive). On the test day, all rats received vehicle before conditioned stimulus exposure. The conditioned stimulus alone reliably suppressed responding in Paired groups relative to control conditions with either daily or weekly intervals between conditioning sessions. The administration of morphine 4 h before conditioned stimulus exposure on the test day was not necessary to observe conditioned withdrawal. Thus, conditioned withdrawal is reliably established to discrete cues associated with naloxone-precipitated withdrawal from acute, infrequent (weekly) opioid exposure.  相似文献   

11.
The effects of chronic exposure to ketamine or remacemide on the acquisition and performance of food-reinforced operant behaviors was assessed in female Sprague-Dawley rats. Ketamine is an anesthetic N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist, whereas remacemide is an active central nervous system compound with both NMDA receptor antagonist and sodium channel blocking properties. Learning, audio/visual discrimination and motivation were modeled using incremental repeated acquisition (IRA), audio/visual discrimination (AVD) and progressive ratio (PR) tasks, respectively. Ketamine (10 or 100 mg/kg/day), remacemide (100 or 150 mg/kg/day) or water was administered daily (7 days/week) via orogastric gavage beginning on postnatal day (PND) 23 and continuing until PND 257. Monday through Friday behavioral assessments began on PND 27 and continued until PND 383. Chronic treatment with the high dose of ketamine decreased response rate in all tasks suggesting decreased motivation or motoric capabilities. Chronic treatment with ketamine or remacemide had no effect on the acquisition of IRA task performance at any dose tested. While chronic treatment with either high-dose ketamine or low-dose remacemide only delayed the acquisition of AVD task performance for a brief period midway through treatment, chronic treatment with high-dose remacemide delayed the acquisition of AVD task performance until late in treatment. The findings for ketamine are quite different from those of MK-801 (the prototypic NMDA receptor antagonist) in a previous rat study in which MK-801 severely disrupted the acquisition of both IRA and AVD task performances. These observations suggest important differences in the mechanism of action between ketamine and MK-801. For example, ketamine has a much lower binding affinity than MK-801 for the NMDA receptor, the dopamine transporter and the dopamine D2 receptor. In addition, the findings for remacemide observed in rats are in marked contrast with those seen in monkeys where chronic remacemide had profound disruptive effects on the acquisition of both IRA and AVD task performances and suggest important species differences.  相似文献   

12.
The purpose of this study was to determine whether progressive ratio (PR) schedules might provide additional information, as compared with fixed ratio or fixed interval schedules, on pharmacologic features of discriminative stimuli (e.g. stimulus intensity). Five pigeons discriminated between 5.6 mg/kg morphine and saline under an arithmetic PR5 schedule of food presentation. The final ratio before pigeons either stopped responding for 5 min or switched responding from the selected to the non-selected key was designated as the last completed ratio (LCR). Pigeons responded 6.8% on the drug key following saline and 96.4% on the drug key following 5.6 mg/kg morphine. The average LCR value for saline was not significantly different from the average LCR value for morphine. A larger dose of morphine (10.0 mg/kg) increased the LCR value and significantly decreased rates of responding. Smaller doses of morphine (0.32 and 1.0 mg/kg) occasioned primarily saline-appropriate responding and decreased LCR values. Buprenorphine substituted for morphine and significantly increased LCR values, whereas nalbuphine produced only partial (20-80%) morphine-key responding and significantly decreased LCR. Cocaine did not substitute for morphine or modify LCR compared with saline control. Together, these results suggest that the stimulus effects of micro-opioids vary on a dimension (e.g. intensity) that can be quantified using PR schedules.  相似文献   

13.
Single injections with morphine can induce a state of acute opioid dependence in humans and animals, typically measured as precipitated withdrawal when an antagonist such as naloxone is administered 4-24 h after morphine. Repeated treatment with morphine at 24-h intervals can result in a progressive shift in potency of naloxone to produce such acute withdrawal signs, including suppression of operant responding for food reward. The current study characterized fully both morphine and naloxone dose-effect functions in an effort to establish the relative contributions of repeated morphine vs. repeated naloxone (Nal) experience to these potency shifts. Rats trained on an FR15 schedule for food received four vehicle or morphine injections (0.56-5.6 mg/kg sc), spaced 24 h apart. Four hours after each morphine pretreatment (Repeat Nal), or 4 h after the fourth and final morphine pretreatment only (Single Nal), a cumulative dose-effect function for naloxone-induced suppression of responding was determined. Vehicle-pretreated (Morphine Naive) rats showed little change in the naloxone dose effect function even after four cumulative dose-effect determinations. By contrast, a progressive increase in naloxone potency was observed following successive pretreatments with morphine under Repeat Nal conditions, and the magnitude of naloxone potency shift was morphine dose dependent. At a morphine dose of 5.6 mg/kg, repeated naloxone experience in the presence of morphine was not an absolute requirement to produce an increase in naloxone potency across days, but repeated naloxone could potentiate the magnitude of the observed shift, indicating both experience-independent and experience-dependent processes at work. At lower doses of morphine (1.0 and 3.3 mg/kg) no shift in naloxone potency was observed across days of morphine treatment in the absence of repeated naloxone experience (Single Nal conditions), indicating an increasing contribution of naloxone experience-dependent processes as dose of morphine was decreased. It is argued that these experience-dependent processes in the progressive shift of naloxone potency observed in the current study may reflect an important role of conditioning in the early development of opioid dependence.  相似文献   

14.
Long-Evans hooded rats were initially trained to lever press, in standard, operant conditioning chambers, according to a fixed-ratio 1 (FR1) reinforcement schedule using 0.06ml deliveries of 8% w/v ethanol as the reinforcer, during daily Monday-Friday, 1h experimental sessions. Next, experimental sessions were reduced to 0.5h, the FR value was increased to 5, and the rats were trained to discriminate 2.0mg/kg s.c. phencyclidine (PCP) from saline vehicle using standard, drug discrimination training procedures, with 8% ethanol as the reinforcer. Following training, dose-response tests with PCP (0.1-4.0mg/kg), ketamine (0.1-18mg/kg), dexoxadrol (1.0-5.6mg/kg) and morphine (1.0-9.0mg/kg) were conducted. More PCP-lever presses were emitted than saline-lever presses at several doses of PCP, ketamine, and dexoxadrol, indicating generalization from the 2.0mg/kg PCP stimulus. When morphine was tested, more saline-lever than PCP-lever presses were made, and percent PCP-lever pressing never exceeded an average of 12% at any dose tested. This study demonstrates that one drug of abuse, PCP, can serve as a discriminative stimulus when another drug of abuse, ethanol, serves as the reinforcing stimulus, and is the first explicit laboratory demonstration of drug discriminative stimulus control during drug self-administration.  相似文献   

15.
The development of tolerance to behavioral effects of morphine was investigated in rats that responded on a two-lever, multiple-trial, multiple differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate fixed-ratio (mult DRL FR) schedule of food presentation. Stable performances were maintained when sessions were conducted just twice per week. The effects of cumulative doses of morphine (1.0–8.0 mg/kg) or chlordiazepoxide (CDP; 4.0–32.0 mg/kg) were evaluated once per week; saline injections were given in the intervening sessions. The effects of saline and morphine on nociception were also evaluated in hot-plate tests conducted on the same subjects 15 min after selected operant sessions. Initially, morphine produced dose-related decreases in response rates and reinforcement rates in the DRL and FR components as well as significant increases in hot-plate response latencies. Following weekly administration of morphine (1.0–8.0 mg/kg) for 10 weeks, there was little or no tolerance to its effects on operant behavior. In contrast, complete tolerance developed to the antinociceptive effects of morphine. These results suggest that tolerance to various behavioral effects of morphine may be dissociated, and that the loss of reinforcement may be insufficient by itself to produce tolerance to effects of morphine on operant behavior. Additionally, whereas CDP initially produced only dose-related decreases in DRL and FR response rates, following weekly morphine the smaller doses of CDP (4.0–16.0 mg/kg) produced increases in response rates. Finally, the effects of cumulative doses of morphine did not differ significantly from the effects of noncumulative doses of the drug.  相似文献   

16.
The centrally acting nicotinic‐cholinergic antagonist mecamylamine (mec) is well documented to produce amnestic effects in animals and humans. However, in certain circumstances the compound has enhanced performance of some memory‐related tasks in animals and further investigation of this paradoxical effect is warranted. The present study was designed to determine under what conditions mec would enhance memory‐task performance in rats and aged nonhuman primates. Mec (various doses) or saline was administered IP to rats tested in the Morris Water Maze (MWM), to rats trained to perform a delayed stimulus discrimination task (DSDT), and IM to aged rhesus monkeys (average age 24.6 years) trained to perform a delayed matching to sample task (DMTS). In rats, mec 1.0 mg/kg improved location of the hidden platform on day 1 of the MWM, but inhibited learning in subsequent trials, while several μg/kg doses improved DSDT accuracy. Further, some μg/kg doses of mec also improved accuracy in aged monkeys in DMTS at both 10 min and 24 h after administration. Mec had no effect on swim speeds in the MWM, response latencies in the DSDT, or on choice or response latencies in the DMTS task. Collectively, the results indicate that some doses of mec can mimic certain memory‐enhancing effects produced by nicotinic‐acetylcholine receptor agonists. It is not clear whether mec is acting as a partial agonist in this regard, or whether low‐level nicotinic antagonism produces a cellular response that is in some way analogous to nicotine‐induced receptor desensitization. Drug Dev. Res. 47:127–136, 1999. © 1999 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.  相似文献   

17.
Deficits in temporal processing are implicated in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) for which the most common rodent model is the Spontaneously Hypertensive Rat (SHR). To assess strain differences in temporal processing, males and females of the SHR, Wistar-Kyoto (WKY), and Sprague-Dawley (SD) strains were compared on two timing tasks: one requiring maintenance of a lever press for 10-14 s (TRD, temporal response differentiation) and the other requiring withholding of a lever press for 10-14 s (DRL, differential reinforcement of low rates). Performance of the progressive ratio (PR) task more directly assessed food-motivated behavior. Strains did not differ in task acquisition; however, steady state TRD and DRL performance of the SHR and WKY strains was less accurate which was related to increased burst (non-timing related) responses in those strains relative to the SD. PR performance demonstrated that the SHR and WKY strains exhibited higher response rates and breakpoints than the SD. Subsequently, methylphenidate (1, 3.25, 4.50, 7.50, and 12.0 mg/kg) and d-amphetamine (0.1, 0.25, 0.65, 1.0, and 2.0 mg/kg) were administered intraperitoneally pre-testing. Both drugs disrupted TRD and DRL performances by increasing burst response frequency; however, the strains were not differentially sensitive to either drug. Strain differences were generally maintained throughout the drug and extinction portions of the study. These results indicate increased similarity between the SHR and WKY strains relative to the SD in performance of timing and motivation tasks. Further, the current results do not support continued use of the SHR as a model for ADHD.  相似文献   

18.
Rats were implanted with stimulating electrodes aimed at the medial forebrain bundle-lateral hypothalamus (MFB-LH) and were trained to lever-press for brain self-stimulation on a fixed interval: 60 s schedule of reinforcement. The effects of graded doses of naloxone (0.1–30 mg/kg), morphine (0.3–5.6 mg/kg), naloxone plus morphine,d-amphetamine (0.03–1.0 mg/kg), naloxone plusd-amphetamine, phencyclidine (0.3–5.6 mg/kg), and naloxone plus phencyclidine were tested. Naloxone produced a significant decrease in rates at 30 mg/kg. Naloxone (0.1–1.0 mg/kg) plus morphine blocked the dose-dependent decrease produced by morphine alone. In contrast, naloxone (1.0–10 mg/kg) plusd-amphetamine attenuated the graded increase in response rates produced byd-amphetamine. Naloxone (1.0–10 mg/kg) plus phencyclidine did not reliably change the increase in response rates produced by phencyclidine alone. The use of the fixed interval schedule of brain self-stimulation to study these drug interactions is novel, and further demonstrates that the highly reinforcing aspects of brain stimulation, known to be influenced by dopamine, may also be modulated by the endogenous opiate system.  相似文献   

19.
Pretreatment with a single moderate dose of morphine (e.g. 5.6-10 mg/kg) 4-24 hr prior to challenge with an opioid antagonist such as naloxone results in reliable expression of behaviors that resemble aversive or emotional consequences of withdrawal from chronic opioid exposure, including suppression of operant responding, elevations in brain reward thresholds, and conditioned place aversion. Repeated daily or weekly treatment with these same morphine doses results in a progressive increase in naloxone potency to elicit these withdrawal signs. The current study sought to determine whether increased anxiety-like behavior during withdrawal from chronic opioid dependence is also seen after acute morphine exposure, and progresses with repeated intermittent treatment. Male Wistar rats were handled and injected with either vehicle or morphine for 4 consecutive days. Three injection regimens were employed: Morphine Naive (4 vehicle injections), Acute Morphine (3 vehicle injections, 4th injection 5.6 or 10 mg/kg morphine), or Repeat Morphine (all 4 injections with 5.6 or 10 mg/kg morphine). Acute pretreatment with 5.6 mg/kg or 10 mg/kg morphine resulted in time-dependent increases in exploration of the open arms of the plus maze in naloxone-naive rats when tested at 2, 4 or 8 hr after the final pretreatment injection, with the effects at the higher dose appearing later (4 hr) than after the lower dose (2 hr). This pattern of results, in combination with a separate study which confirmed a significant anxiolytic-like effect of a low dose of morphine (0.56 mg/kg) administered 15 min prior to test, suggested that low residual morphine levels remaining in plasma at 2-4 hr after 5.6 and 10 mg/kg morphine may be sufficient to elicit anxiolytic-like effects. Repeat treatment with either dose of morphine resulted in a further increase in the magnitude and duration of this anxiolytic-like effect. These effects had dissipated by 8 hr post-morphine, and therefore precipitation of withdrawal by one of several doses of naloxone (0.10-3.3 mg/kg) was assessed in separate cohorts of rats 8 hr after the final pretreatment under Morphine Naïve, Acute Morphine, or Repeat Morphine conditions. Naloxone resulted in a significant dose-dependent expression of anxiety-like behavior with no effects on general activity after Acute Morphine pretreatment at either 5.6 or 10 mg/kg morphine. A further significant shift in naloxone potency was observed after Repeat Morphine pretreatment at the 10 mg/kg but not the 5.6 mg/kg dose. Thus, anxiety-like behavior is a prominent feature of the negative emotional consequences of naloxone-precipitated withdrawal from acute opioid dependence.  相似文献   

20.
Sodium cyanide (NaCN) is a commonly and widely used industrial and laboratory chemical that is highly toxic. Its availability and rapid harmful/lethal effects combine to make cyanide a potential foodborne/waterborne intentional‐poisoning hazard. Effective antidotes to cyanide poisoning are currently approved only for intravenous administration. Therefore, an effective cyanide antidote that can be administered intramuscularly in pre‐hospital and/or mass‐casualty settings is needed. Dimethyl trisulfide (DMTS) is a naturally occurring substance used as a flavour enhancer in foods. DMTS has shown antidotal efficacy in cyanide poisoning and is thought to act as both a sulphur donor and partial methaemoglobin inducer. In this study, an intramuscular injection of DMTS (6.25‐200 mg/kg) was given to rats 1 minute after an oral dose of NaCN (98.2 mg/kg; twice the median lethal dose) to test the antidotal efficacy and safety of DMTS treatment. Toxic signs and survival were examined along with behavioural function (up to 30 hour after ingestion) using a previously established operant behavioural model. A large range of DMTS doses (6.25‐100 mg/kg) increased survival after oral cyanide poisoning, and the lower DMTS doses (6.25‐25 mg/kg) also proved to be behaviourally and physiologically safe. Larger DMTS doses (50‐200 mg/kg) produced side effects (ie, inflammation and limping) that were more severe and protracted than those observed at lower DMTS doses. The 25 mg/kg DMTS proved to be the most efficacious (increasing survival from 20% to 75%) and also produced minimal side effects (eg, inflammation) that resolved within 24‐72 hour. Thus, DMTS shows promise as an intramuscularly administered cyanide antidote useful for prompt pre‐hospital or mass‐casualty emergency medical treatment.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号