共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Tricon V Le Pellec-Muller A Martin N Mesure S Azulay JP Vernazza-Martin S 《Neuroscience letters》2007,415(1):81-86
The present study focuses on the organization of kinematic synergy and its adaptation to an unstable support surface during upper trunk movements in aging adults. Seven healthy aging adults (49-66 years old) were instructed to bend the trunk forward (the head and the trunk together) by about 40 degrees and to stabilize their final position, in the standard condition (both feet on the ground), and on a seesaw swinging in the sagittal plane. Kinematic synergy was quantified by performing a principal components analysis on the hip, knee and ankle angle changes during the movement. The results indicate that trunk bending was represented by a single component (PC1) in both conditions, indicating a strong coupling between the angle changes during the movement. The results also show a reorganization of the contribution of PC1 to the three angles when the balance constraints are increased in the seesaw condition. It is concluded that kinematic synergy is preserved during trunk bending in aging adults, regardless of the support conditions. It can also be adapted when the balance constraints are increased by changing the ratio between the angles, indicating a modification of interjoint coordination without modifying the movement's trajectory. 相似文献
2.
Patel M Magnusson M Kristinsdottir E Fransson PA 《European journal of applied physiology》2009,105(2):167-173
The aim was to determine the contributions of foot mechanoreceptive sensation, vision and their interaction on postural stability
during quiet stance, balance perturbations and adaptive adjustments. Postural stability was measured as anteroposterior torque
variance in Young (n = 25, average age = 25.1 years) and Elderly subjects (n = 16, average age = 71.5 years) during repeated calf vibrations while standing with eyes open and closed. Sensation, recorded
using vibration perception and tactile sensitivity, was poorer in elderly than young subjects. Sensation was of low importance
for stability during quiet stance and the first 50 s of repeated vibrations, but was associated with stability during the
last three 50 s periods of balance perturbations, suggesting that the mechanoreceptive sensation affected how well postural
control could adapt to repeated balance perturbations. The findings suggest that clinicians should investigate whether patients
with balance problems and poor adaptation have mechanoreceptive sensation deficits. 相似文献
3.
Mahboobin A Loughlin PJ Redfern MS Sparto PJ 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2005,167(2):260-267
The aim of the current study was to further investigate a recently proposed “sensory re-weighting” hypothesis, by evoking
anterior–posterior (AP) body sway using visual stimuli during sway-referencing of the support surface. Twelve healthy adults
participated in this study. Subjects stood on the platform while looking at a visual scene that encompassed the full horizontal
field of view. A sequence of scene movements was presented to the subjects consisting of multiple visual push/pull perturbations;
in between the first two push/pull sequences, the scene either moved randomly or was stationary. The peak-squared velocity
of AP center-of-pressure (COP) was computed within a 6 s window following each push and pull. The peak-squared velocity was
lowest for the push/pull sequence immediately following the random moving scene. These results are consistent with the sensory
re-weighting hypothesis, wherein the sensory integration process reduced the contribution of visual sensory input during the
random moving scene interval. We also found evidence of habituation to moving scene perturbations with repeated exposure. 相似文献
4.
N. Chastan G.W.M. Westby S. Tézenas du Montcel M.C. Do R.K. Chong Y. Agid M.L. Welter 《Neuroscience letters》2010
Human gait requires the simultaneous generation of goal-directed continuous movement (locomotion) and the maintenance of balance (postural control). In adults, the centre of mass (CoM) oscillates in the vertical plane while walking. During the single support phase of gait initiation, its vertical (vCoM) velocity increases as the CoM falls and is actively reversed prior to foot-contact. In this study we investigated whether this active control, which is thought to reflect balance control during gait initiation, is controlled by visual and somatosensory inputs (Experiment 1) and whether it is modified by a change in motor demands, two steps versus one step (Experiment 2). In all healthy adults, the vCoM velocity was braked, or controlled, by contraction of the soleus muscle of the stance leg. The elimination of visual input alone had no effect on braking, although its amplitude decreased when somatosensory inputs were disrupted (−47%), and further decreased when both visual and somatosensory inputs were disrupted (−83%). When subjects performed only one step, with no trailing of the stance foot, the vCoM velocity braking also decreased (−42%). These results suggest that active braking of the CoM fall during the transition to double support, an indicator of balance control, is influenced by both multisensory integration and the demands of the current motor program. The neural structures involved in this mechanism remain to be elucidated. 相似文献
5.
Nagy E Feher-Kiss A Barnai M Domján-Preszner A Angyan L Horvath G 《European journal of applied physiology》2007,100(1):97-104
The changes in postural control in elderly people after an 8-week training course were characterized. Static postural stability
was measured during standing on a single force platform first with the eyes open and then with the eyes closed. Body sway
was analysed on a force plate in groups of elderly and of young subjects. Half of the elderly subjects then took part in the
training course. The posturographic measurements were repeated after the course. The sway in anteroposterior (AP) and mediolateral
(ML) directions was subjected to spectral analysis. The frequency spectrum of the platform oscillations was calculated by
fast Fourier transformation in the intervals 0.1–0.3, 0.3–1 and 1–3 Hz. It was found that the sway path was longer and the
frequency power was higher in the elderly group. The training caused a significant improvement in functional performance,
but a significantly longer sway path was observed after the training in the ML direction. The frequency analysis revealed
a significantly higher power after 8 weeks without visual control in the ML direction in the training group in the low and
the middle frequency bands. The results suggest that the participants’ balance confidence and the control of ML balance improved
in response to the training. The higher ML frequency power exhibited after the training may be indicative of a better balance
performance. Thus, the increase in the sway path in this age group did not mean a further impairment of the postural control. 相似文献
6.
P. Hammond G. S. V. Mouat A. T. Smith 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1988,72(1):1-20
Summary Motion after-effects were elicited from striate cortical cells in lightly-anaesthetized cats, by adapting with square-wave gratings or randomly textured fields drifting steadily and continuously in preferred or null directions. The time-course and recovery of responsiveness following adaptation were assessed with moving bars, gratings or textured fields. Results were compared with controls in which the adapting stimulus was replaced by a uniform field of identical mean luminance, and also assessed in relation to the strength and time course of adaptation. Within 30–60 s adaptation, firing declined to a steady-state. Induced after-effects were direction-specific, and manifest as a transitory depression in response to the direction of prior adaptation, recovering to control levels in 30–60 s. Maximal after effects were induced by gratings of optimal drift velocity and spatial frequency. With rare exceptions after-effects were restricted to driven activity; no consistent effects on resting discharge were observed. The onset of adaptation, and the recovery period, were more rapid in simple cells, although after effects of comparable strength were elicited from simple and from standard complex cells. Special complex cells, including many of the more profoundly texture-sensitive neurones in the cortex, were more resistant to adaptation. The results support the conclusion that psychophysically measured adaptation and induced motion after-effect phenomena reflect the known properties of cortical neurones. 相似文献
7.
Mark Shelhamer Caroline Tiliket Dale Roberts Phillip D. Kramer David S. Zee 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1994,100(2):316-327
We investigated the effect of short-term vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) adaptation in normal human subjects on the dynamic properties of the velocity-to-position ocular motor integrator that holds positions of gaze. Subjects sat in a sinusoidally rotating chair surrounded by an optokinetic nystagmus drum. The movement of the visual surround (drum) was manipulated relative to the chair to produce an increase (× 1.7 viewing), decrease (× 0.5, × 0 viewing), or reversal (× (-2.5) viewing) of VOR gain. Before and after 1 h of training, VOR gain and gaze-holding after eccentric saccades in darkness were measured. Depending on the training paradigm, eccentric saccades could be followed by centrifugal drift (after × 0.5 viewing), implying an unstable integrator, or by centripetal drift [after × 1.7 or × (-2.5) viewing], implying a leaky integrator. The changes in the neural integrator appear to be context specific, so that when the VOR was tested in non-training head orientations, both the adaptive change in VOR gain and the changes in the neural integrator were much smaller. The changes in VOR gain were on the order of 10% and the induced drift velocities were several degrees per secend at 20 deg eccentric positions in the orbit. We propose that (1) the changes in the dynamic properties of the neural integrator reflect an attempt to modify the phase (timing) relationships of the VOR and (2) the relative directions of retinal slip and eye velocity during head rotation determine whether the integrator becomes unstable (and introduces more phase lag) or leaky (and introduces less phase lag).Visiting scientist from: 1 INSERM-U94. 16, avenue du Doyen Lépine, F-69500 Bron, France 相似文献
8.
Hatzitaki V Konstadakos S 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2007,182(4):525-535
The effects of aging on the acquisition of a novel visuo-postural coordination task were addressed at two levels: (a) changes
in the intersegmental coordination (local dynamics) (b) changes in the coupling of postural sway to the visual driving stimulus
(global dynamics). Twelve elderly (age: 71.2 ± 6.4 years; height: 169.3 ± 3.8 cm; mass: 72.4 ± 6.1 kg) and 12 young women
(age: 27.1 ± 4.9 years; height: 178.3 ± 2.9 cm; mass: 56.7 ± 4.1 kg) practiced a visually guided Weight-Shifting (WS) task
while standing on a dual force platform. The participants were asked to keep the vertical force applied by each limb within
a ±30% force boundary that was visually specified by a target sine-wave signal. Practice consisted of three blocks of five
trials performed in 1-day, followed by a block of five trials performed 24 h later. Ground reaction forces and segment (shank,
pelvis, and upper trunk) angular kinematics were synchronously sampled through an A/D acquisition board and further analyzed
employing spectral and coherence analysis. Elderly women had longer WS cycles, lower response gain, and higher within-trial
variability, suggesting a weaker coupling between the visual stimulus and the response force. Spectral analysis of the ground
reaction forces confirmed that regardless of age, visuo-postural coupling improved with practice. However, the recruitment
of local degrees of freedom was different between the two age groups. With practice, young performers increased peak coherence
between the pelvis and the upper trunk and reduced peak power of segment oscillations in the pitch direction. On the other
hand, elderly women decreased active upper trunk rotation while shifting control to the lower limb. It is suggested that different
functional coordination solutions are possible for attaining the same overall task goal. These solutions are determined by
age-related constraints in the physiological systems supporting postural control. 相似文献
9.
Laurette Hay Chantal Bard Michelle Fleury Normand Teasdale 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1996,108(1):129-139
The ability of young and elderly adults to keep a stable upright posture while facing changes in the availability of visual and/or propriomuscular information was investigated. The two sensory sources of information were alternatively available and withdrawn, jointly and separately, during 10-s alternating sequences. Vision was modified by means of liquid-crystal goggles, and proprioception was altered by means of tendon vibration of both antagonistic ankle muscles. Elderly adults were less stable than young adults when vision was withdrawn. Both groups were greatly affected when proprio-muscular inputs were altered by vibration. Under constant visual conditions and following a propriomuscular perturbation (i.e., vibration), elderly adults were unable to take advantage of the reinsertion of propriomuscular inputs. They showed a transient, decreased stability and were unable to fully recover during a 10-s period, whereas young adults were able to rapidly integrate the information to stabilize their posture. When both propriomuscular and visual inputs were withdrawn and concurrently reinserted, the elderly adults did not show a transitory increase in the velocity of the center of foot pressure. The present results extend findings on the inability of elderly adults to reconfigure rapidly the postural set following reinsertion of sensory inputs. The results also suggest that elderly adults have difficulties in taking advantage of sensory redundancy in postural control. 相似文献
10.
11.
Neuromuscular adaptation during prolonged strength training, detraining and re-strength-training in middle-aged and elderly people 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Häkkinen K Alen M Kallinen M Newton RU Kraemer WJ 《European journal of applied physiology》2000,83(1):51-62
Effects of a 24-week strength training performed twice weekly (24 ST) (combined with explosive exercises) followed by either
a 3-week detraining (3 DT) and a 21-week re-strength-training (21 RST) (experiment A) or by a 24-week detraining (24 DT) (experiment
B) on neural activation of the agonist and antagonist leg extensors, muscle cross-sectional area (CSA) of the quadriceps femoris,
maximal isometric and one repetition maximum (1-RM) strength and jumping (J) and walking (W) performances were examined. A
group of middle-aged (M, 37–44 years, n=12) and elderly (E, 62–77, n=10) and another group of M (35–45, n=7) and E (63–78, n=7) served as subjects. In experiment A, the 1-RM increased substantially during 24 ST in M (27%, P < 0.001) and E (29%, P < 0.001) and in experiment B in M (29%, P < 0.001) and E (23%, P < 0.01). During 21 RST the 1-RM was increased by 5% at week 48 (P < 0.01) in M and 3% at week 41 in E (n.s., but P < 0.05 at week 34). In experiment A the integrated electromyogram (IEMG) of the vastus muscles in the 1-RM increased during
24 ST in both M (P < 0.05) and E (P < 0.001) and during 21 RST in M for the right (P < 0.05) and in E for both legs (P < 0.05). The biceps femoris co-activation during the 1-RM leg extension decreased during the first 8-week training in M (from
29 ± 5% to 25 ± 3%, n.s.) and especially in E (from 41 ± 11% to 32 ± 9%, P < 0.05). The CSA increased by 7% in M (P < 0.05) and by 7% in E (P < 0.001), and by 7% (n.s.) in M and by 3% in E (n.s.) during 24 ST periods. Increases of 18% (P < 0.001) and 12% (P < 0.05) in M and 22% (P < 0.001) and 26% (P < 0.05) in E occurred in J. W speed increased (P < 0.05) in both age groups. The only decrease during 3 DT was in maximal isometric force in M by 6% (P < 0.05) and by 4% (n.s.) in E. During 24 DT the CSA decreased in both age groups (P < 0.01), the 1-RM decreased by 6% (P < 0.05) in M and by 4% (P < 0.05) in E and isometric force by 12% (P < 0.001) in M and by 9% (P < 0.05) in E, respectively, while J and W remained unaltered. The strength gains were accompanied by increased maximal voluntary
neural activation of the agonists in both age groups with reduced antagonist co-activation in the elderly during the initial
training phases. Neural adaptation seemed to play a greater role than muscle hypertrophy. Short-term detraining led to only
minor changes, while prolonged detraining resulted in muscle atrophy and decreased voluntary strength, but explosive jumping
and walking actions in both age groups appeared to remain elevated for quite a long time by compensatory types of physical
activities when performed on a regular basis.
Accepted: 2 May 2000 相似文献
12.
Desmurget M Pélisson D Grethe JS Alexander GE Urquizar C Prablanc C Grafton ST 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2000,132(2):243-259
It is known that the saccadic system shows adaptive changes when the command sent to the extraocular muscles is inappropriate. Despite an abundance of supportive psychophysical investigations, the neurophysiological substrate of this process is still debated. The present study addresses this issue using H2(15)O positron emission tomography (PET). We contrasted three conditions in which healthy human subjects were required to perform saccadic eye movements toward peripheral visual targets. Two conditions involved a modification of the target location during the course of the initial saccade, when there is suppression of visual perception. In the RAND condition, intra-saccadic target displacement was random from trial-to-trial, precluding any systematic modification of the primary saccade amplitude. In the ADAPT condition, intra-saccadic target displacement was uniform, causing adaptive modification of the primary saccade amplitude. In the third condition (stationary, STAT), the target remained at the same location during the entire trial. Difference images reflecting regional cerebral-blood-flow changes attributable to the process of saccadic adaptation (ADAPT minus RAND; ADAPT minus STAT) showed a selective activation in the oculomotor cerebellar vermis (OCV; lobules VI and VII). This finding is consistent with neurophysiological studies in monkeys. Additional analyses indicated that the cerebellar activation was not related to kinematic factors, and that the absence of significant activation within the frontal eye fields (FEF) or the superior colliculus (SC) did not represent a false negative inference. Besides the contribution of the OCV to saccadic adaptation, we also observed, in the RAND condition, that the saccade amplitude was significantly larger when the previous trial involved a forward jump than when the previous trial involved a backward jump. This observation indicates that saccade accuracy is constantly monitored on a trial-to-trial basis. Behavioral measurements and PET observations (RAND minus STAT) suggest that this single-trial control of saccade amplitude may be functionally distinct from the process of saccadic adaptation. 相似文献
13.
Benoît G. Bardy William H. Warren Jr. Bruce A. Kay 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1996,111(2):271-282
Three experiments tested the hypothesis that postural sway during locomotion is visually regulated by motion parallax as well as optical expansion. Oscillating displays of three-dimensional scenes were presented to participants walking on a treadmill, while postural sway was recorded. Displays simulated: (a) a cloud, in which parallax and expansion are congruent, (b) a hallway, (c) the side walls of the hallway, (d) a ground surface, (e) a wall, (f) the wall with a central hole, (g) a hall farther from the observer, and (h) a wall farther from the observer. In contrast to previous results with a hallway, responses with the cloud were isotropic and directionally specific. The other displays demonstrated that motion parallax was more effective than simple horizontal flow in eliciting lateral sway. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that adaptive control of sway during walking is based on congruent expansion and parallax in natural environments. 相似文献
14.
Dr. G. Melvill Jones A. Gonshor 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1982,45(1-2):45-58
Summary This study examines long-term (up to 27 days) effects of maintained vision reversal on (i) smooth visual tracking with head still, (ii) oculomotor response to actively generated head oscillation and (iii) spontaneous saccades. Dove prism goggles produced horizontal, but not vertical (sagittal plane), vision reversal. Eye movements were recorded by EOG; head movements by an electro-magnetic search coil.Both visual tracking and saccade dynamics remained unchanged throughout. In contrast, both the ocular response to active head osculations (goggles off and subject looking at a stationary target) and associated retinal image blur showed substantial and retained adaptive changes, akin to those previously found in the vestibulo-ocular reflex as tested in darkness at 0.17 Hz.However, several additional unexpected results emerged. First, in the fully adapted state smooth eye movements tended to be of reversed phase in the range 0.5–1.0 Hz (in spite of normal vision during tests), but of normal phase from about 2 Hz and above (in spite of negligible visual tracking in this upper range). Second, after permanent removal of the inverting goggles, this peculiar frequency response of the fully adapted state quickly (36 h) reverted to a dynamically simpler condition manifest as retained (2–3 weeks) attenuation of gain (eye vel./ head vel.) which, as in control conditions, was monotonically related to frequency. From these two findings it is inferred that the fully adapted state may have comprised two separate components: (i) A simple element of monotonic and long-lasting gain attenuation and (ii) a complex, frequency labile, element which could be quickly rejected. Dynamic characteristics of the putative complex element were estimated by vectorial subtraction of the simple one from that of the fully adapted condition. The outcome suggests that the inferred complex condition might represent a predictive element.Two further findings are reported: (i) Substantially different vectors of the adapted response were obtained with normal and reversed vision at 3.0 Hz head oscillation, indicating a novel visual influence acting above the cut-off frequency for visual tracking. (ii) During head oscillation in the vertical sagittal plane (in which vision was not reversed) there was never any image blur, indicating high geometric specificity in the adaptive process.Supported by Canadian Medical Research Council Operating Grant No. MT5630 相似文献
15.
Kelly JW Loomis JM Beall AC 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2005,161(3):285-292
Two experiments investigated the role of optic flow in controlling posture. Both experiments measured postural sway in two virtual environments with different 3-D structure but the same optic flow. Observers attempted to maintain balance on one foot while viewing an object that appeared either rigid with respect to the environment or that appeared to move concomitantly with head movements. The apparent object motion concomitant with head motion was achieved by changing the perceived, but not physical, depth of the object. For both objects, the optic flow information was the same and only depth information was varied. Observers showed a decrease in stability (as measured by head sway) when viewing the object that appeared to move, suggesting that perceived relative motion, not optic flow, signals self-motion to the postural control system. 相似文献
16.
Ursula Margareta Küng C. G. C. Horlings F. Honegger J. E. J. Duysens J. H. J. Allum 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2009,194(4):631-645
Does the central nervous system (CNS) independently control roll and pitch movements of the human body during balance corrections? To help provide an answer to this question, we perturbed the balance of 16 young healthy subjects using multi-directional rotations of the support surface. All rotations had pitch and roll components, for which either the roll (DR) or the pitch (DP) component were delayed by 150 ms or not at all (ND). The outcome measures were the biomechanical responses of the body and surface EMG activity of several muscles. Across all perturbation directions, DR caused equally delayed shifts (150 ms) in peak lateral centre of mass (COM) velocity. Across directions, DP did not cause equally delayed shifts in anterior–posterior COM velocity. After 300 ms however, the vector direction of COM velocity was similar to the ND directions. Trunk, arm and knee joint rotations followed this roll compared to pitch pattern, but were different from ND rotation synergies after 300 ms, suggesting an intersegmental compensation for the delay effects. Balance correcting responses of muscles demonstrated both roll and pitch directed components regardless of axial alignment. We categorised muscles into three groups: pitch oriented, roll oriented and mixed based on their responses to DR and DP. Lower leg muscles were pitch oriented, trunk muscles were roll oriented, and knee and arm muscles were mixed. The results of this study suggest that roll, but not pitch components, of balance correcting movement strategies and muscle synergies are separately programmed by the CNS. Reliance on differentially activated arm and knee muscles to correct roll perturbations reveals a dependence of the pitch response on that of roll, possibly due to biomechanical constraints, and accounts for the failure of DP to be transmitted equally in time across all limbs segments. Thus it appears the CNS preferentially programs the roll response of the body and then adjusts the pitch response accordingly. 相似文献
17.
Hudson TE Lackner JR DiZio P 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2005,165(3):283-293
We investigated whether pointing movements made with the torso would adapt to movement-contingent augmentation or attenuation of their spatial amplitude. The pointing task required subjects standing on a platform in the dark to orient the mid-sagittal plane of their torso to the remembered locations of just extinguished platform-fixed visual targets without moving their feet. Subjects alternated pointing at two chest-high targets, 60° apart, (1) in a baseline period with the stance platform stationary, (2) during exposure to concomitant contra or ipsiversive platform rotations that grew incrementally to 50% of the velocity of torso rotation, and (3) after return in one step to stationary platform conditions. The velocity and amplitude of torso movements relative to space decreased 25–50% during exposure to contraversive platform rotations and increased 20–50% during ipsiversive rotations. Torso rotation kinematics relative to the platform (as well as the platform-fixed targets and feet) remained virtually constant throughout the incremental exposure period. Subjects were unaware of the altered motion of their body in space imposed by the platform and did not perceive their motor adjustments. Upon return to stationary conditions, torso rotation movements were smaller and slower following adaptation to contraversive rotations and larger and faster after ipsiversive platform rotations. These results indicate a rapid sensory-motor recalibration to the altered relationship between spatial (inertial) torso motion and intended torso motion relative to the feet, and rapid re-adaptation to normal conditions. The adaptive system producing such robust torso regulation provides a critical basis for control of arm, head, and eye movements. 相似文献
18.
Jeka JJ Oie KS Kiemel T 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》2008,191(4):453-463
Human movement control is inherently stochastic, requiring continuous estimation of self-motion based upon noisy sensory inputs.
The nervous system must determine which sensory signals are relevant on a time scale that enables successful behavior. In
human stance control, failure to effectively adapt to changing sensory contexts could lead to injurious falls. Nonlinear changes
in postural sway amplitude in response to changes in sensory environmental motion have indicated a dynamic changing of the
weighting of the nervous system’s multiple sensory inputs so that estimates are based upon the most relevant and accurate
information available. However, the time scale of these changes is virtually unknown. Results here show systematic changes
in postural gain when visual scene motion amplitude is increased or decreased abruptly, consistent with sensory re-weighting.
However, this re-weighting displayed a temporal asymmetry. When visual motion increased, gain decreased within 5 s to a value
near its asymptotic value. In contrast, when visual motion decreased, it took an additional 5 s for gain to increase by a
similar absolute amount. Suddenly increasing visual motion amplitude threatens balance if gain remains high, and rapid down-weighting
of the sensory signal is required to avoid falling. By contrast, slow up-weighting suggests a conservative CNS strategy. It
may not be functional to rapidly up-weight with transient changes in the sensory environment. Only sustained changes necessitate
the slower up-weighting process. Such results add to our understanding of adaptive processing, identifying a temporal asymmetry
in sensory re-weighting dynamics that could be a general property of adaptive estimation in the nervous system. 相似文献
19.
J. J. Collins C. J. De Luca A. Burrows L. A. Lipsitz 《Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation cérébrale》1995,104(3):480-492
In an earlier posturographic investigation (Collins and De Luca 1993) it was proposed that open-loop and closed-loop control mechanisms are involved in the regulation of undisturbed, upright stance. In this study, stabilogram-diffusion analysis was used to examine how the natural aging process affects the operational characteristics of these control mechanisms. Stabilogram-diffusion analysis leads to the extraction of repeatable center-of-pressure (COP) parameters that can be directly related to the steady-state behavior and functional interaction of the neuromuscular mechanisms underlying the maintenance of erect posture. Twenty-five healthy young males (aged 19–30 years) and twenty-five elderly males (aged 71–80 years) who were free of major gait and postural disorders were included in the study. An instrumented force platform was used to measure the time-varying displacements of the COP under each subject's feet during quiet standing. The COP trajectories were analyzed as one-dimensional and two-dimensional random walks, according to stabilogram-diffusion analysis. Using this technique, it was demonstrated cross-sectionally that healthy aging is associated with significant changes in the quasi-static dynamics of the postural control system. (It was also shown that more traditional posturographic analyses, i.e., summary statistics, were not sensitive enough to detect these age-related differences.) It was found that the steady-state behavior of the open-loop postural control mechanisms in the elderly is more positively correlated and therefore perhaps more unstable, i.e., the output of the overall system has a greater tendency to move or drift away from a relative equilibrium point over the short term. In contrast with this result, it was also found that the steady-state behavior of the closed-loop postural control mechanisms in the elderly is more negatively correlated and therefore perhaps more stable, i.e., over the longer term, there is an increased probability that movements away from a relative equilibrium point will be offset by corrective adjustments back towards the equilibrium position. In addition, it was demonstrated that the elderly utilize open-loop control schemes for longer time intervals and correspondingly larger COP displacements during periods of undisturbed stance. This result suggests that in the elderly there is a greater delay, on average, before closed-loop feedback mechanisms are called into play. Finally, it was shown that there is an increased heterogeneity of postural control abilities in healthy older adults. 相似文献
20.
The regulation of vestibular afferent information during monocular vision while standing 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0
The purpose of this study was to investigate the contribution of the vestibular system to postural control during monocular vision using binaural-bipolar galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS). Four visual (both eyes, dominant eye, non-dominant eye, and no vision) conditions were tested during GVS in five healthy subjects while focusing on a target placed in front of them. GVS evoked similar upper body postural sway during both monocular and no vision conditions that were significantly greater to those during binocular vision. Changes in ground reaction forces to the anode side followed that same trend, although data for vision with the dominant eye were not significantly different from that for binocular vision. These data suggest an increase in the weighting of vestibular afferent information during monocular vision for standing postural control. 相似文献