首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Objectives: We studied cognitive function in high‐risk relatives belonging to a single extended family showing linkage of bipolar disorder to a locus on chromosome 4. High‐risk relatives were defined as those that carried the risk haplotype of polymorphic markers, identified in a previous linkage study. This family provided a rare opportunity to characterize a neuropsychological endophenotype in a homogeneous sample of relatives with a common genetic risk factor. Methods: Fifteen family members carrying the risk haplotype (eight diagnosed with bipolar disorder or depression and seven with no psychiatric diagnosis), unrelated patients with bipolar disorder (n = 36) and major depressive disorder (n = 40), and healthy control subjects (n = 33) were administered the California Verbal Learning Test, Verbal Fluency Test, Hayling Sentence Completion Test, and Brixton Spatial Anticipation Test to assess verbal memory, verbal fluency, and executive function. Results: Compared with healthy controls, family members carrying the risk haplotype were impaired in indices of memory and executive function. There were no significant differences between unaffected and affected haplotype‐carrying family members in any cognitive measure. Pronounced deficits in the encoding stage of verbal memory and category verbal fluency were evident in individuals with the risk haplotype. Conclusions: Verbal learning and semantic verbal fluency impairments may represent a cognitive endophenotype for both bipolar disorder and major depression in relatives of bipolar disorder patients, as impairment was also present in high‐risk relatives who had not developed any affective disorder symptoms. These findings suggest that impairment in semantic organization may be linked to the genetic aetiology of bipolar disorder.  相似文献   

2.
Kashyap H, Kumar JK, Kandavel T, Reddy YCJ. Neuropsychological correlates of insight in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Objective: There are limited data on neuropsychological correlates of poor insight in obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). We hypothesize that poor insight may be associated with greater impairment in tasks of conflict resolution/response inhibition and possibly impairment in a task of verbal learning and memory. Method: Insight and neuropsychological functions were assessed in 150 subjects with DSM‐IV OCD. The neuropsychological data of 177 healthy control subjects were used for comparison. Results: Insight score correlated significantly with the Stroop Interference Test for conflict resolution/response inhibition (P = 0.002), and showed trends for significance with the Controlled Oral Word Association (COWA) average for verbal fluency (P = 0.021) and delayed recall on the Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) for verbal memory (P = 0.015). On regression analysis, the AVLT delayed recall, the COWA average, the Matrix score, the Yale‐Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale total score, and current antipsychotic use emerged as significant predictors of poorer insight. Conclusion: Poor insight is associated with greater impairments in conflict resolution/response inhibition, verbal memory, and fluency. Individuals with poorer insight may have difficulty in appropriately processing conflicting information, updating their memory with corrective information, and then accessing this corrective information to modify their irrational beliefs.  相似文献   

3.
OBJECTIVE: Mania has received little attention from a contemporary neuropsychological perspective despite its clear resemblance to the disinhibition syndrome sometimes seen after frontal brain injury, particularly injury to the inferior aspect of the prefrontal cortex. The purpose of this investigation was to describe the neuropsychological profile of severe acute mania by using a range of tasks selected primarily for the detection of localized neural disruption within the prefrontal cortex. METHOD: Fifteen acutely manic inpatients were compared with 30 nonpsychiatric subjects on tasks from the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (Tower of London, spatial working memory, intradimensional-extradimensional attentional shift, and rapid visual information processing tasks) and on the Iowa Gambling Task, Stroop Color and Word Test, a verbal fluency task, and the California Verbal Learning Test. RESULTS: Discriminant function analysis identified deficits in sustained attention (on the rapid visual information processing task) and verbal learning (on the California Verbal Learning Test) as the best indicators of manic performance, rather than deficits on any of the tests of executive functioning. The model correctly classified 91% of subjects overall and 87% of manic subjects. Manic patients did not resemble patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage in their performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. CONCLUSIONS: Acute mania is characterized by core deficits in verbal memory and sustained attention against a background of milder impairments in functions that are traditional measures of prefrontal cortex integrity (attentional set shifting, planning, working memory). The data do not implicate ventral prefrontal cortex disruption as a locus of pathology in acute mania. Verbal memory and sustained attention deficits may relate differentially to the state and trait characteristics of bipolar disorder.  相似文献   

4.
We hypothesized that a modified version of the Frontal Behavioral Inventory (FBI-mod), along with a few cognitive tests, would be clinically useful in distinguishing between clinically defined Alzheimer's disease (AD) and subtypes of frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD): frontotemporal dementia (dysexecutive type), progressive nonfluent aphasia, and semantic dementia. We studied 80 patients who were diagnosed with AD (n = 30) or FTLD (n = 50), on the basis of a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, imaging, neurological examination, and history. We found significant between-group differences on the FBI-mod, two subtests of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (verbal learning and delayed recall), and the Trail Making Test Part B (one measure of 'executive functioning'). AD was characterized by relatively severe impairment in verbal learning, delayed recall, and executive functioning, with relatively normal scores on the FBI-mod. Frontotemporal dementia was characterized by relatively severe impairment on the FBI-mod and executive functioning in the absence of severe impairment in verbal learning and recall. Progressive nonfluent aphasia was characterized by severe impairment in executive functioning with relatively normal scores on verbal learning and recall and FBI-mod. Finally, semantic dementia was characterized by relatively severe deficits in delayed recall, but relatively normal performance on new learning, executive functioning, and on FBI-mod. Discriminant function analysis confirmed that the FBI-mod, in conjunction with the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, and the Trail Making Test Part B categorized the majority of patients as subtypes of FTLD or AD in the same way as a full neuropsychological battery, neurological examination, complete history, and imaging. These tests may be useful for efficient clinical diagnosis, although progressive nonfluent aphasia and semantic dementia are likely to be best distinguished by language tests not included in standard neuropsychological test batteries.  相似文献   

5.
The course of myasthenia gravis (MG) is complicated by increased reports of cognitive defects in both human and animal models, which suggests potential central nervous system (CNS) damage. We conducted a systematic review of the relationships between MG and cognitive function. This systematic review followed Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. Major databases were searched to examine the neuropsychological studies of adults with MG. Weighted effect sizes were pooled by cognitive domain. Eight studies representing 300 subjects were included. Eight cognitive domain categories were identified: (i) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), (ii) language, (iii) processing speed, (iv) verbal learning and memory, (v) visual learning and memory, (vi) attention span, (vii) response fluency, and (viii) motor performance. Nine (cognitive domain categories, MMSE, language, processing speed, verbal learning and memory (except for delayed recall memory), and motor performance) of 16 cognitive tasks revealed significant moderate effect sizes. Verbal logical-delayed memory, finger tapping with the preferred hand, and the Symbol Digit Modalities Test showed a greater magnitude relationship to cognitive function than did other specific cognitive domains. Verbal learning and memory seems to be the most significant affected according to cognitive domain categories. For MG, the ability of attention, response fluency, visual learning, and memory seems to be reserved. The MG patients seem to perform significantly worse than the non-MG controls in a range of cognitive domains. Our findings should be interpreted with caution because of the clinical and methodological heterogeneity of included studies.  相似文献   

6.
BACKGROUND: There is evidence that individuals with bipolar disorder exhibit neuropsychological impairments not only during mood episodes but also when they are euthymic. One of the most consistently reported cognitive problems in euthymic individuals with bipolar disorder is an impairment in verbal episodic memory. Verbal learning and memory depend on individuals' ability to organize verbal information appropriately during learning. The purpose of the present study was (i) to determine whether episodic memory impairment in euthymic individuals with bipolar disorder is mediated by impairments in organization of verbal information during learning and (ii) to compare the characteristics of memory impairment in bipolar disorder with that previously found in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). METHODS: Study participants were 30 individuals with DSM-IV bipolar I disorder (BP-I), 30 individuals with DSM-IV OCD and 30 normal control participants matched for age, gender and education. Participants completed the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), a well-established measure of verbal learning and memory that enables assessment of verbal organization strategies during learning. RESULTS: Compared with control subjects, both BP-I and OCD participants showed impaired performance in long-delayed free recall and verbal organization strategies during learning. BP-I participants showed greater long-delay free recall difficulties but not greater verbal organization difficulties during learning than OCD participants. For OCD participants, the long-delay recall impairment was mediated by difficulties using verbal organizational strategies during learning. In contrast, the group difference in long-delayed free recall between BP-I and control participants remained significant even when semantic clustering was introduced as a mediator. This indicated that BP-I participants' long-delayed free recall difficulties were mediated to a lesser extent by difficulties using verbal organizational strategies than for OCD participants. CONCLUSIONS: Verbal episodic memory problems in individuals with bipolar I disorder and OCD are mediated to different degrees by difficulties using semantic clustering encoding strategies compared with control participants.  相似文献   

7.
A range of neuropsychological deficits have been identified in individuals with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS) and have been related to disruptions in function of the frontal cortex of the brain. We hypothesized that impairments in the use of strategic, frontally-mediated processes that facilitate learning and memory would be associated with deficits in the long-term episodic memory of verbal material (i.e., word lists). We evaluated 28 adults with OSAS and 24 controls (ranging from 28 to 60 years of age) using the California Verbal Learning Test. General executive abilities were assessed using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Letter fluency, and Category fluency. Individuals with OSAS exhibited poorer recall across learning trials, less efficient use of semantic clustering, and poorer use of semantic cues. Retention of previously encoded information and recognition, however, were intact. With the exception of letter fluency, deficits were not observed in general executive control. Results are discussed within the context of disruptions in the interactions between long-term memory and executive abilities that are subserved by frontal and distal brain regions.  相似文献   

8.
A range of neuropsychological deficits have been identified in individuals with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS) and have been related to disruptions in function of the frontal cortex of the brain. We hypothesized that impairments in the use of strategic, frontally-mediated processes that facilitate learning and memory would be associated with deficits in the long-term episodic memory of verbal material (i.e., word lists). We evaluated 28 adults with OSAS and 24 controls (ranging from 28 to 60 years of age) using the California Verbal Learning Test. General executive abilities were assessed using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Letter fluency, and Category fluency. Individuals with OSAS exhibited poorer recall across learning trials, less efficient use of semantic clustering, and poorer use of semantic cues. Retention of previously encoded information and recognition, however, were intact. With the exception of letter fluency, deficits were not observed in general executive control. Results are discussed within the context of disruptions in the interactions between long-term memory and executive abilities that are subserved by frontal and distal brain regions.  相似文献   

9.
OBJECTIVE: To determine and correlate alterations in neuropsychological function and cerebral blood flow in bipolar patients. METHOD: Assessments included the Positive and Negative Symptom Scale, Global Assessment Functioning, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Stroop test, Trail Making Test (TMT), California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) and phonetic verbal fluency/controlled oral word association tests. Single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) was carried out with the administration of 99mTc-HMPAO. Forty-three outpatients out of 85 fulfilling RDC diagnostic criteria for bipolar disorder and six healthy subjects were included in the study. SPECT and neuropsychological assessments were performed in 30 patients in manic (n = 7), hypomanic (n = 8), depressed (n = 12) or euthymic (n = 3) states. All assessments were carried out before starting treatment. RESULT: Several corrected correlations between neuropsychological function and cerebral blood flow (CBF) were identified: executive function (WCST) and striatal, frontal, temporal, cerebellum, parietal and cingulate CBF; memory (WMS, WAIS-Digits) and striatal, frontal, temporal and parietal CBF; attentional tasks (Stroop) and striatal, temporo-medial and parietal CBF; verbal learning (CVLT) and frontal, posterior temporal, cingulate and occipital CBF; psychomotor disturbances (TMT) and anterior temporal CBF; poorer intelligence performance scores (WAIS-Vocabulary) and cerebellum and parietal CBF. CONCLUSIONS: This study confirms the presence of functional disturbances in fronto-subcortical structures, the cerebellum and limbic system in bipolar patients.  相似文献   

10.
抑郁症和早期阿尔茨海默病的记忆和执行功能   总被引:5,自引:3,他引:2  
目的 研究抑郁症和早期阿尔茨海默病(AD)的神经心理学特征,试图运用神经心理学评估对两者进行鉴别。方法 对32例单相抑郁症、38例早期AD和34例对照进行WHO-UCLA词语学习、词语流畅、复杂图形和逻辑记忆的评估。结果 早期AD组神经心理学测验得分最低,抑郁症组次之,对照组最高,3组之间有显著性差异(P<0.01);抑郁症组仅表现为词语学习和逻辑记忆的自由回忆以及语义流畅的损害(P<0.05),而早期AD组表现为全面的认知功能损害(P<0.01);逐步判别分析提示,复杂图形延迟自由回忆、词语学习长时延迟自由回忆和语义流畅是区分抑郁症组和早期AD组的重要指标。结论 抑郁症和早期AD认知功能损害的特征不同,长时延迟自由回忆、再认和语义流畅能够区分早期AD和抑郁症。  相似文献   

11.
Several studies have shown subtle neuropsychological deficits in healthy relatives of schizophrenic patients. However, older relatives and parents have been less frequently assessed than younger adult relatives and siblings. Furthermore, some areas of neuropsychological functioning such as memory and learning have been little studied. Thirty-seven 22-70-year-old non-psychotic parents and siblings of schizophrenic patients were compared to 37 healthy control subjects on a battery of neuropsychological tests (Trail Making, parts A and B, verbal fluency, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and four subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised: logical memory, design reproduction, verbal paired associates and digit span). Relatives did not differ from control subjects on Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance and on visual memory, but were significantly impaired on verbal fluency; more subtle deficits were found on Trail Making, part B, digit span and paired associates. A higher proportion of relatives than control subjects showed impairment on verbal fluency and verbal memory. These neuropsychological weaknesseswere present as much in siblings as in parents of schizophrenic patients, and age did not cancel differences between relatives and control subjects. Thus, these subtle deficits seem to be potential phenotypic markers of schizophrenia.  相似文献   

12.
While neuropsychological tests have been identified for the early prediction of Alzheimer's disease, this has not been established for prediction of all-cause dementia. This would be helpful for clinicians concerned about the risk of progression to dementia in patients who may present with a variety of medical and neurological conditions. We wanted to determine whether neuropsychological tests could accurately predict incident dementia within 10 and five years of diagnosis in a community-based sample. The Canadian Study of Health and Aging was conducted in three waves over a 10-year period (1991-2002). We studied 1472 non-demented participants who completed neuropsychological testing in 1991 and received a diagnostic assessment for dementia in 2001 (n = 284). We also studied 1231 non-demented participants who completed neuropsychological testing in 1996 and received a diagnostic assessment in 2001 (n = 634). Diagnosticians were blinded to performance on the predictive tests. Age, education, and sex were included as covariates in all regression analysis. Ten-year prediction: 2 tests, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) short delayed verbal recall and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test Revised (WAIS-R) Digit Symbol, were significant predictors of dementia (sensitivity = 78%, specificity = 72%, positive likelihood ratio = 2.81). Five-year prediction: 4 tests, Wechsler Memory Scale Information, RAVLT short delayed verbal recall, animal fluency, and WAIS-R Digit Symbol, significantly predicted incident dementia (sensitivity = 75%, specificity = 74%, positive likelihood ratio = 2.90). Regression models were supported with bootstrapping estimates. Neuropsychological tests can accurately predict progression to all-cause dementia within 10 years of diagnosis in a large community-based sample of non-demented participants.  相似文献   

13.
This study aimed to investigate the association between atrophy in the prefrontal cortex with executive function and verbal fluency in elderly male and female patients poststroke. Thirty elderly female patients with non-aphasic ischemic stroke aged ≥ 60 years and 30 age-matched non-aphasic male patients with ischemic stroke were recruited. Automatic magnetic resonance imaging segmentation was used to assess the volume of the whole prefrontal cortex, along with its subdivisions: anterior cingulate cortex, orbitofrontal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The Semantic Verbal Fluency Test was administered at 3 and 15 months poststroke. At 3 months poststroke, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volume was significantly correlated with Verbal Fluency Test score in female patients only (partial coefficient = 0.453, P = 0.045), after controlling for age, education, diabetes, neurological deficit, white matter lesions volume, as well as the location and volume of infarcts. At 15 months poststroke, there remained a significant association between the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volume and Verbal Fluency Test (partial coefficient = 0.661, P = 0.001) and between the left prefrontal cortex volume and Verbal Fluency Test (partial coefficient = 0.573, P = 0.004) in female patients after the same adjustments. These findings indicate that atrophy of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex contributes to the impairment of verbal fluency in elderly female patients with stroke. Sex differences may be present in the neuropsychological mechanisms of verbal fluency impairment in patients with stroke.  相似文献   

14.
BACKGROUND: Patients with schizophrenia have impairments in several domains of cognition, including working memory/executive function, verbal memory, language, oculomotor scanning/psychomotor speed, and general intelligence. Impairments have also been found in unaffected siblings, suggesting they could be heritable. To assess the suitability of cognitive dysfunction for use in genetic studies, we estimated relative risk (lambda) in a large cohort of siblings. METHODS: One hundred forty-seven patients with schizophrenia, 193 of their siblings, and 47 control subjects were studied using a neuropsychological test battery, which included intelligence quotient (IQ), Wide Range Achievement Test, Wisconsin Card Sort, Wechsler Memory Scale (revised), California Verbal List Test, Trails A and B, and Letter and Category Fluency. Relative risk was estimated using a cutoff score of 1 SD below the control mean. RESULTS: As expected, patients performed markedly worse than control subjects on all tests except the Wide Range Achievement Test. Siblings had impaired performance on the Wisconsin Card Sort and Trails B, with trends for reduction (p = .01-.05) on the California Verbal List Test and Letter Fluency. Relative risk to siblings was elevated on the Trails B (lambda = 4.0) and California Verbal List Test (lambda = 2.8). Trends (p = .01-.05) for increased lambda were also seen for Wisconsin Card Sort, Letter Fluency, Wechsler Memory Scale and decline in IQ (lambda = 1.74-2.4). Correlations between tests of different cognitive functions were weak, indicating they measure relatively independent processes. CONCLUSION: Unselected siblings of patients with schizophrenia have impairments in several cognitive domains. Relative risk scores were in the moderate range, suggesting a significant genetic component. Impairments on one test only weakly predicted impairments on other tests. Thus, cognitive phenotypes identify distinct, familial traits associated with schizophrenia. Using this dimensional approach to subdividing schizophrenia may reduce the clinical and genetic heterogeneity of schizophrenia and improve the power of genetic studies.  相似文献   

15.
BACKGROUND: Hypomanic temperament and rigid personality are putative risk factors for affective episodes and even bipolar disorder. Individuals with bipolar disorder exhibit neuropsychological impairments, especially memory difficulties, not only during mood episodes but also when they are euthymic. Such cognitive impairments may also constitute a risk factor for bipolar disorder. The purpose of the present study was to investigate the presence of memory difficulties in individuals with hypomanic and rigid personality traits. METHODS: Study participants were 6000 German students recruited from high schools, colleges, and vocational schools in Germany. The students completed the Hypomanic Personality Scale and the Rigidity subscale of the Munich Personality Test. Four groups of students were selected from this sample based on their scores in the Hypomanic Personality Scale and Rigidity subscale: individuals with (1) hypomanic temperament, (2) rigid temperament, or (3) hypomanic-rigid temperament and (4) control participants. These students (n = 153) completed the Rey-Auditory Verbal Learning Test, a well-established measure of verbal learning and memory, as well an IQ test (Leistungsprüfsystem). RESULTS: Multiple regression analyses indicated that sex and IQ, but not temperament, predicted learning of the Auditory Verbal Learning Test word list, the number of words recalled at short-delayed recall, and recognition. LIMITATIONS: The risk for affective disorders was only defined by psychometric measures, and we did not control for family history of bipolar disorders. CONCLUSIONS: Hypomanic temperament and Rigid personality were not associated with verbal learning and memory. Cognitive impairment may be associated with repeated mood episodes rather than constituting a risk factor for bipolar disorder.  相似文献   

16.
Introduction: Cognitive set shifting requires flexible application of lower level processes. The Delis–Kaplan Executive Functioning System (DKEFS) Color–Word Interference Test (CWIT) is commonly used to clinically assess cognitive set shifting. An atypical pattern of performance has been observed on the CWIT; a subset of individuals perform faster, with equal or fewer errors, on the more difficult inhibition/switching than the inhibition trial. This study seeks to explore the cognitive underpinnings of this atypical pattern. It is hypothesized that atypical patterns on CWIT will be associated with better performance on underlying cognitive measures of attention, working memory, and learning when compared to typical CWIT patterns. Method: Records from 239 clinical referrals (age: M = 68.09 years, SD = 10.62; education: M = 14.87 years, SD = 2.73) seen for a neuropsychological evaluation as part of diagnostic work up in an outpatient dementia and movement disorders clinic were sampled. The standard battery of tests included measures of attention, learning, fluency, executive functioning, and working memory. Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted to compare the cognitive performance of those with typical versus atypical CWIT patterns. Results: An atypical pattern of performance was confirmed in 23% of our sample. Analyses revealed a significant group difference in acquisition of information on both nonverbal (Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–Revised, BVMT–R total recall), F(1, 213) = 16.61, p < .001, and verbal (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised, HVLT–R total recall) learning tasks, F(1, 181) = 6.43, p < .01, and semantic fluency (Animal Naming), F(1, 232) = 7.57, p = .006, with the atypical group performing better on each task. Effect sizes were larger for nonverbal (Cohen’s d = 0.66) than verbal learning (Cohen’s d = 0.47) and semantic fluency (Cohen’s d = 0.43). Conclusions: Individuals demonstrating an atypical pattern of performance on the CWIT inhibition/switching trial also demonstrated relative strengths in semantic fluency and learning.  相似文献   

17.
Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) in children is accompanied by quantifiable deficits on attentional, learning, and memory indices. Symptoms of childhood ADD persist into adulthood in many cases. However, many adults without a history of childhood ADD also complain of difficulties with attention, presumably due to other etiologies than developmental ADD. This study investigated whether performance on neuropsychological measures of attention and memory could differentiate adults with attentional complaints and history of childhood ADD from those without childhood ADD. Adults with a history of childhood ADD demonstrated reduced scores on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task and Delayed Free Recall on the California Verbal Learning Test as well as on a verbal fluency task relative to adults who denied attentional problems in childhood. Discriminant function analysis using verbal fluency, performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, verbal learning and recall, Digit Span Backward, and performance on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task as predictors correctly classified adults with and without a history of childhood ADD into diagnostic groups with 75% accuracy.  相似文献   

18.
Introduction Ideomotor limb apraxia is the disturbance of planning and of execution of motor activity,which is not caused by a dysfunction of the motor or sensory nervous system. Apraxia is a diagnostic criterion in dementialike Alzheimer's disease. However, this symptom may also occur in dementia with subcortical lesions like Huntington's disease (HD), a hereditary, devastating neurodegenerative disease leading to neurological and psychiatric dysfunction. The aim of our study is to determine the correlation between the occurrence of ideomotor limb apraxia and neuropsychological deficits in HD. Methods To assess the correlation between apraxia and neuropsychological abilities in HD, 41 patients with HD and 33 age- and sex-matched controls were examined. The De Renzi test for apraxia and an apraxia test battery containing tests of i) imitation of meaningless gestures of hands, ii) imitation of meaningless gestures of fingers, iii) performance of meaningful gestures on demand, and iv) pantomime of tool use were used to assess apraxia. Moreover, neuropsychological function was rated by the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), the Rey Complex Figure Memory Test, the Trail Making Test A and B, the California Verbal Learning Test (German version), the Stroop Color and Word Test, the Controlled Oral Word Association Test, and the Mehrfachwahl- Wortschatz-Intelligenztest for measuring verbal intelligence. Motor function was assessed in all HD patients by the Unified HD Rating Scale (UHDRS), rating oculomotor and orolingual function, fine motor tasks, parkinsonism, dystonia, chorea and statics and gait. Results Apraxic HD patients showed worse results than non-apraxic HD patients in three items of the Rey Complex Figure Memory Test (Organisation, short-term and longterm memory), but not in other assessed neuropsychological tests. In assessment of meaningful gestures on demand 39.3% of HD patients were apraxic, in assessment of pantomime of tool use 67.9% of HD patients showed apraxia. Patients with HD showed highly significant worse results than controls in the De Renzi test, in hands' and fingers' imitation, in performance of gestures on demand, in pantomime of tool use and every neuropsychological test except for the test measuring verbal intelligence. Apraxic HD patients showed worse results than non-apraxic HD patients in the UHDRS total motor score and the score for oculomotor function. Conclusion This is the largest study on apraxia in HD. Ideomotor limb apraxia is a common sign in HD patients, occurring in a high percentage. In contrast to the opinion of several authors, occurrence of apraxia in HD is independent from neuropsychological decline and the severity of most neurological symptoms.  相似文献   

19.

Background and purpose

Cognitive impairments are frequent consequences of epilepsy, with intellectual ability reportedly being lower in patients with idiopathic generalized epilepsies than in the general population. However, neuropsychological investigations have been rarely performed in patients with juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME). We aimed to quantify the cognitive function in JME patients using various neuropsychological tests.

Methods

We compared cognitive function in 27 JME patients with that in 27 healthy volunteers using tests examining cognitive performance, such as the verbal and visual memory, frontal function, attention, IQ score, and mood. In the JME group, we examined risk factors for cognitive function such as age, sex, family history, education level, age at seizure onset, seizure frequency, EEG abnormality, disease duration, and previous intake of antiepileptic drugs.

Results

Verbal learning was significantly lower in JME patients than in controls, and attention and verbal fluency were impaired in JME patients compared with controls. However, general intellectual ability and mood did not differ between the groups. Early onset of seizure and long duration of disease were closely related to impaired cognitive function.

Conclusions

JME patients may exhibit impaired cognitive function, in terms of memory and execution, despite having normal intelligence and mood.  相似文献   

20.
Tests of memory and verbal fluency were administered to 19 neurologically impaired Wilson's patients, 12 non-neurologically impaired Wilson's patients and 15 normal control subjects. Wilson's patients with neurologic disease recalled significantly fewer words on the delayed recall version of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test than control subjects (p less than .05) but they showed no impairment on the recognition version of this test. On a verbal fluency test (FAS) requiring the subject to retrieve items from different categories, Wilson's patients with neurologic disease generated significantly fewer words than control subjects (p less than .01). Results suggest that retrieval memory is mildly impaired in the neurologic WD group but rate of learning and rate of forgetting is normal.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号