首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
A commissurotomized subject, L.B., was shown asterisks flashed at random locations, up to four in each field, and attempted either to compare the numbers in the two fields or to report the total number. The main results were: (a) Report was more accurate with unilateral than with bilateral presentation, suggesting that the difficulty integrating across fields was partly attentional; (b) in integrating across fields, attention was focused on one field, with only crude 'one-or-many' information from the other; (c) in cross-field comparisons, the focus was on the LVF, but in reporting the number it was on the RVF when report was oral or right-handed, and on the LVF when report was left-handed; (d) cross-field comparisons were improved when the locations were mirrored across the midline.  相似文献   

2.
A commissurotomized patient, L.B., was tested on several imagery tasks, in which the stimuli were flashed tachistoscopically in the left or right visual half-field. On tests requiring the generation of images of lowercase letters from their uppercase versions, or the generation of the positions of the hands on a clockface from digitally presented times, there was a strong right half-field (left-hemispheric) advantage in accuracy, but a left half-field (right-hemispheric) advantage in reaction time (RT). On tests requiring the mental rotation of letters or stickfigures to the upright, however, there was a strong left half-field (right-hemispheric) advantage in accuracy, RT, and conformity of RTs to an ideal "mental-rotation" function when plotted against angular orientation. These data provide strong evidence that the right hemisphere was capable of mental rotation comparable to that of normal subjects; the left hemisphere, by contrast, seemed virtually incapable of mental rotation in the early testing sessions, and never achieved the proficiency of the right.  相似文献   

3.
Mental rotation (MR) performance may be used as an index of mental slowing or bradyphrenia, and may reflect, in particular, speed of motor preparation. MR was employed with a sample of both melancholic (n=8) and non-melancholic (n=9) unipolar depressed patients and healthy controls (n=10) to determine if motor slowing associated with depression might be reflected in slowed motor preparation (as reflected in slope of the MR function) independent of actual motor slowing (overall response time). Both melancholic and non-melancholic patients showed a generalised slowing relative to controls, perhaps reflecting bradykinesia and akinesia. This effect was significantly greater in the melancholic group than in the non-melancholic group. Relative to both the controls and the non-melancholic groups, the melancholic patients showed a progressive slowing with increasing angle of orientation indicating a specific slowing of MR. This deficit suggests a role of slowed motor planning in the psychomotor retardation of patients with melancholic depression.  相似文献   

4.
When subjects decide whether two visual stimuli presented in various orientations are identical or mirror-images, reaction time increases with the angular disparity between the stimuli. The interpretation of this well-known observation is that subjects mentally rotate images of the stimuli until they are in congruence, in order to solve the task. Here we review studies involving mental rotation of tactile stimuli. Mental rotation in tactile tasks is specifically associated with the requirement for mirror-image discrimination, as opposed to identity judgments. The key brain region mediating mental rotation of tactile stimuli seems to be the parietal cortex. Visual processing appears to facilitate task performance. We report an experiment from our laboratory addressing the nature of the reference frame for mental rotation of tactile stimuli. Our observations indicate that when the hand is directly in front of the body, with the head facing forward, the shortest reaction times for mirror-image discrimination of stimuli applied to the fingerpad are obtained when the longitudinal axis of the stimulus is in or parallel to the sagittal plane, even when this is perpendicular to the long axis of the finger. Thus, the reference frame for mental rotation of tactile stimuli is not purely hand-centered. This is consistent with other findings indicating variable assignment of reference frames for tactile perception.  相似文献   

5.
Deficits in visual-spatial ability can be associated with Parkinson's disease (PD), and there are several possible reasons for these deficits. Dysfunction in frontal-striatal and/or frontal-parietal systems, associated with dopamine deficiency, might disrupt cognitive processes either supporting (e.g., working memory) or subserving visual-spatial computations. The goal of this study was to assess visual-spatial orientation ability in individuals with PD using the Mental Rotations Test (MRT), along with other measures of cognitive function. Non-demented men with PD were significantly less accurate on this test than matched control men. In contrast, women with PD performed similarly to matched control women, but both groups of women did not perform much better than chance. Further, mental rotation accuracy in men correlated with their executive skills involving mental processing and psychomotor speed. In women with PD, however, mental rotation accuracy correlated negatively with verbal memory, indicating that higher mental rotation performance was associated with lower ability in verbal memory. These results indicate that PD is associated with visual-spatial orientation deficits in men. Women with PD and control women both performed poorly on the MRT, possibly reflecting a floor effect. Although men and women with PD appear to engage different cognitive processes in this task, the reason for the sex difference remains to be elucidated.  相似文献   

6.
We investigated the importance of retinal and body inclination in the recognition of spatial environment. The paradigm involved the recognition, in body upright and tilted conditions, of tilted images -intervals of 15 degrees from 0 degrees to 90 degrees leftward and rightward respective to head coordinates - of known spatial layouts encountered while walking in Paris. The analysis of reaction times was consistent with the subjects mentally rotating the spatial layout so that the environment was subjectively vertical before making their decisions. In contrast, when the body was roll-tilted (33 degrees ), overall reaction time was not affected; however, reaction time and spatial layout tilt with respect to the head were correlated when the body was tilted but not when upright. Both results indicate that gravity was slightly important in performing the task.  相似文献   

7.
Three commissurotomized and two left-hemispherectomized subjects were tested on spoken report of sequences of three dichotic pairs of digits. With instruction to report only one digit from each pair, there was an overall advantage to the ear contralateral to the hemisphere mediating speech, but report of ipsilateral-ear digits ranged from 40 to 100%. In commissurotomized subjects, the more extreme ipsilateral suppression under instructions to report all digits may be due to failure to gain access to unattended information stored in the right hemisphere, rather than to suppression of the ipsilateral pathway. However one commissurotmized patient did appear to have access to right-hemisphere items, the result either of subcortical transfer or of external cross-cueing. The hemispherectomized subjects seemed able to store both attended and unattended information in the same hemisphere.  相似文献   

8.
Mental rotation of congenitally absent hands.   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
We compared motor imagery performance of normally limbed individuals with that of individuals with one or both hands missing since birth (i.e., hand amelia). To this aim, 14 unilaterally and 2 bilaterally amelic participants performed a task requiring the classification of hands depicted in different degrees of rotation as either a left or a right hand. On the same task, 24 normally limbed participants recapitulated previously reported effects; that is, that the hand motor dominance and, more generally, a lifelong use of hands are important determinants of left-right decisions. Unilaterally amelic participants responded slower to hands corresponding to their absent, compared with their existing, hand. Moreover, left and right hand amelic participants showed prolonged reaction times to hands (whether left or right) depicted in unnatural orientations compared with natural orientations. Among the bilateral amelics, the individual with phantom sensations, but not the one without, showed similar differentiation. These findings demonstrate that the visual recognition of a hand never physically developed is prolonged, but still modulated by different rotation angles. They are further compatible with the view that phantom limbs in hand amelia may constrain motor imagery as much as do amputation phantoms.  相似文献   

9.
Tomasino B  Budai R  Mondani M  Skrap M  Rumiati RI 《Neuroreport》2005,16(16):1795-1800
We investigated the effects of cortical stimulation on mental rotation tasks in a patient with an electrode array placed over his left primary motor cortex. The array was implanted to relieve chronic pain resulting from right brachial plexus damage. Tasks involving motor imagery were slowed down by cortical stimulation, whereas those involving visual imagery were not. When the patient performed the motor-imagery task, the interference effect on response times disappeared if the stimulator was switched off. We also probed two of the sites (anterior-lateral and posterior-medial position), and found that stimulation of the more anterior-lateral one consistently disrupted motor imagery.  相似文献   

10.
11.
The time it takes humans to discriminate rotated objects from their mirror images increases linearly with the rotation angle. This phenomenon is probably due to an analogue mode of visual information processing during which an object's mental representation is rotated in a time-consuming process called mental rotation. As the speed of mental rotation in humans depends on rotation axis, we tested the ability of a California sea lion to mentally rotate perspective line drawings of three-dimensional objects about four axes. In a matching-to-sample experiment the animal was presented with the image and a mirror image of a block sample that had previously been shown upright. Both image and mirror image were rotated by a multiple of 60 degrees about the object's x-, y-, z-axis, or a skew axis (an axis oblique to these standard orthogonal axes). The animal's choice and reaction times were recorded using a computer-controlled touch-screen device. Mean reaction times and errors generally increased with angular disparity supporting the model of mental rotation for three-dimensional objects. Linear regression analysis of mean reaction times yielded high correlation coefficients only for three axes. The slope of reaction time functions indicated the highest mental rotation speed for the skew axis. This contrasts with the priority of mental rotation axes in humans suggesting that due to special ecological demands a different mode of orientation invariance evolved in marine mammals.  相似文献   

12.
We compared the performance of normal-reading (N = 14) and dyslexic children (N = 14) in a chronometric mental rotation task (cMRT) using letters, animals and pseudo-letters, which are objects that look like letters. In a typical chronometric mental rotation task two items are presented simultaneously on a screen whereby the right item is a rotated version of the left item and could be the same or a mirror version of the left item. The mental rotation paradigm is an appropriate method to test predictions of two different approaches trying to explain the problems for dyslexics when reading. According to the functional coordination deficit (FCD) model dyslexics show a failure in suppression of symmetry in the representation of graphemic material and therefore cannot decide whether the letter is normal or mirrored because of an ambiguous mapping between phoneme and grapheme representations. Therefore, the deficits of dyslexic children regarding mental rotation performance are restricted to the stimulus “letters”. According to findings that propose the involvement of the cerebellum in mental rotation tasks and a cerebellar deficit in dyslexia, an impaired mental rotation is expected affecting all types of stimuli. To investigate the involvement of the cerebellum, motor performance was additionally assessed because the cerebellum plays an important role in motor functions and motor imagery. For the cMRT we found that the dyslexic children show both slower reaction times regarding the stimulus “letters” and “pseudo-letters” and increased overall reaction times compared to non-dyslexic children. The mental rotation effect was more pronounced in dyslexic children than in normal readers. In contrast to previous approaches, the results of our study support the idea that poor results in mental rotation result from deficits in mental rotation itself rather than from a decision problem after mental rotation which supports the predictions of the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. However, since the impairment of dyslexics regarding mental rotation performance is letter-specific and motor results show no differences between dyslexic and non-dyslexic children, further approaches next to the cerebellar deficit hypothesis must be taken into account, especially in consideration of the fact that there are a number of causes for the failure in reading.  相似文献   

13.
This pilot study was designed to examine mental rotation ability in individuals with Down syndrome. 7 individuals with Down syndrome (mean mental age = 8.18 +/- 2.73 years; mean chronological age = 29.8 +/- 5.4 years) and a group of 9 typically developing children, matched for mental age, (mean mental age = 8.40 +/- 1.73 years; mean chronological age = 7.2 +/- 1.2 years) were given a version of Cooper and Shepherd's (1973) mental rotation paradigm. On each trial, participants viewed a symbol representing an upper case 'F' or a mirror image of an 'F'. The symbol was presented at one of eight different orientations. The participant's task was to determine whether the letter was reversed or non-reversed. Interestingly, both groups showed similar trends in increased reaction times with increasing angular disparity, suggesting that both groups were performing mental rotations. There was no significant difference in reaction time between the typically developing and Down syndrome groups, however, the Down syndrome group made significantly more errors than the typically developing group. Participants with Down syndrome were able to carry out the mental rotations at well above chance level and mental rotation ability was shown to correlate with mental age.  相似文献   

14.
Status epilepticus of either a nonconvulsive, partial, or generalized form was provoked in rats by 60 min of electrical stimulation of a kindled focus in the posterior-ventral hippocampus. Following spontaneous offset of the nonconvulsive status epilepticus, minor pathology occurred which was largely restricted to the hippocampus, whereas partial or generalized status epilepticus produced considerable bilateral damage in the hippocampus, basolateral amygdala, and pyriform cortex. Bisection of the anterior half of the corpus callosum lateralized the forelimb motor seizures and bisection of the hippocampal commissure lateralized the hippocampal afterdischarge and the associated brain pathology. Paradoxically, commissurotomy also increased the probability of developing status epilepticus while reducing its severity. Further, it was shown that the right hippocampus precipitated status epilepticus with a higher probability than the left hippocampus in both intact and split-brain rats.  相似文献   

15.
Normal and reading disabled children, aged from 11 to 13 years and matched for I.Q., were timed as they discriminated bs from ds. When the letters were presented only in their normal upright orientations, normal readers responded more quickly when they were presented in the right than in the left visual hemifield, while the disabled readers showed a slight but insignificant left hemifield advantage. When the letters were presented in varying angular orientations the reaction times indicated that both groups "mentally rotated" an internal representation of each letter to the upright in order to discriminate them. The two groups did not differ in the accuracy of discrimination or in the estimated rate of mental rotation, and there were no significant hemifield differences in this phase of the experiment. These data offer no support for the view that disabled readers are deficient in spatial ability, but confirm earlier evidence that they may suffer a lack of left-hemispheric specialization.  相似文献   

16.
Sergent (Brain 113, 537–568, 1990) flashed pairs of digits to the opposite visual fields of three commissurotomized subjects (L.B., N.G. and A.A.), and found that they were highly accurate in deciding which digit was the larger, but at little or no better than chance in deciding whether the digits were the same or different. Experiment 1 confirmed that these subjects were better at relative than at same-different judgements. However their performance on relative judgements was considerably lower than in Sergent's study and could be explained largely in terms of the subjects' use of information available to a single hemisphere. A fourth subject, D.K., with section of the posterior corpus callosum only, had little difficulty with either task despite previous evidence of visual disconnection, and appeared able to transfer the information verbally. In Experiment 2, L.B. was better able to make both relative and same-different judgements when the digits were presented within either the left or right visual fields than when they were presented in opposite fields. These results suggest little, if any, interhemispheric transfer of either shape or numerical information following complete forebrain commissurotomy.  相似文献   

17.
We describe the case of a callosotomized man, D.D.V., who shows unusual neglect of stimuli in the left visual field (LVF). This is manifest in simple reaction time (RT) to stimuli flashed in the LVF and in judging whether pairs of filled circles in the LVF are of the same or different color. It may reflect strong left-hemispheric control and consequent attention restricted to the right side of space. It is not evident in simple RT when there are continuous markers in the visual fields to indicate the locations of the stimuli. In this condition, his RTs are actually faster to LVF than to right visual field (RVF) stimuli, suggesting a switch to right-hemispheric control that eliminates the hemineglect. Neglect is also not evident when D.D.V. responds by pointing to or touching the locations of the stimuli, perhaps because these responses are controlled by the dorsal rather than the ventral visual system. Despite his atypical manifestations of hemineglect, D.D.V. showed evidence of functional disconnection typical of split-brained subjects, including prolonged crossed-uncrossed different in simple reaction time, inability to match colors between visual fields, and enhanced redundancy gain in simple RT to bilateral stimuli even when the stimulus in the LVF was neglected.  相似文献   

18.
Fixed dystonia is an uncommon but severely disabling condition typically affecting young women following a minor peripheral injury. There is no evidence of any structural lesions of the central nervous system nor any clear peripheral nerve or root damage. Electrophysiological techniques such as short intracortical inhibition, cortical silent period and a plasticity inducing protocol have revealed similarities but also differences compared to classical mobile dystonia. To further explore the pathophysiology of fixed dystonia we compared mental rotation of body parts and sensory temporal discrimination in 11 patients with fixed dystonia, 11 patients with classical mobile dystonia and 10 healthy controls. In the mental rotation task subjects were presented with realistic photos of left or right hands, feet and the head of a young women with a black patch covering the left or the right eye in six different orientations. Subjects had to verbally report the laterality of the presented stimuli. To assess sensory temporal discrimination subjects were asked to discriminate whether pairs of visual, tactile (electrical), or visuo‐tactile stimuli were simultaneous or sequential (temporal discrimination threshold) and in the latter case which stimulus preceded the other (temporal order judgement). In accordance with previous studies patients with mobile dystonia were abnormal in mental rotation and temporal discrimination, whereas patients with fixed dystonia were only impaired in mental rotation. Possible explanations for this deficit may include the influence of the abnormal body posture itself, a shared predisposing pathophysiology for mobile and fixed dystonia, or a body image disturbance. These findings add information to the developing pathophysiological picture of fixed dystonia. © 2010 Movement Disorder Society  相似文献   

19.
The mode of visual information processing during visuospatial tasks differs across species and is supposed to depend on evolutionary and ecological factors. Humans show reaction times that increase with angular disparity when tested in mental rotation tasks. Pigeons show a time-independent rotational invariance that possibly evolved in response to the horizontal reference plane birds perceive while flying. As it was suggested that hominids may have secondarily lost the ability of rotational invariance while retreating from arboreal living and evolving upright gait where the vertical reference plane is more important, mental rotation tests with various recent primate species promise to model the evolution of the respective modes of information processing. The results of recent corresponding experiments with a mainly arboreal living primate species could not be explained by either mode, thus supporting the idea of information processing systems having gradually evolved. Here, we conducted mental rotation experiments with three Rhesus monkeys, a more terrestrial living primate species. In a two-alternative matching-to-sample procedure, we measured the monkeys' reaction times for decisions between rotated figures representing image and mirror-image of a previously shown upright sample. The results of our three monkeys were inconsistent. Linear regression analyses showed for one test animal significant correlation coefficients for mean reaction times depending on angular disparity and thus clearly indicated mental rotation. The other two test animals showed reaction times not consistent with mental rotation, whereas rotational invariance might explain the responses to the smaller angles of rotation. Our results suggest that two separately evolved information processing systems may be coexisting to a certain extent in species with correspondingly overlapping ecological demands.  相似文献   

20.
Mental rotation of body parts is performed through inner simulation of actual movements, and is likely to rely upon cortical and subcortical systems (e.g. motor and premotor areas and basal ganglia) involved in motor planning and execution. Studies indicate that sensory and motor deficits, such as for example pain, limb amputation or focal hand dystonia, bring about a specific impairment in mental rotation of the affected body parts. Here we explored the ability of patients affected by idiopathic cervical dystonia (CD) to mentally rotate affected (neck) and unaffected (hands and feet) body districts. The experimental stimuli consisted of realistic photos of left or right hands or feet and the head of a young men with a black patch on the left or the right eye. As non-corporeal stimulus the front view of a car with a black patch on the left or the right headlight was used. The stimuli were presented at six different degrees of orientations. Twelve CD patients and 12 healthy participants were asked to verbally report whether the hands or feet were left or right, or whether the patch was on the left or the right eye or headlight. Reaction times and accuracy in performing the laterality tasks on the four stimuli were collected. Results showed that CD patients are slow in mental rotation of stimuli representing body parts, namely hand, foot and head. This abnormality was not due to a general impairment in mental rotation per se, since patients' ability to rotate a non-corporeal object (a car) was not significantly different from that of healthy participants. We posit that the deficit in mental rotation of body parts in CD patients may derive from a defective integration of body- and world-related knowledge, a process that is likely to allow a general representation of "me in the external world".  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号