首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 281 毫秒
1.
Rationale: GABAA receptor antagonists have been shown to reduce ethanol self-administration and ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in rats, suggesting a role for the GABAA receptor in modulating ethanol’s motivational effects. Objectives: The present experiments examined the effects of the GABAA receptor antagonists, bicuculline and picrotoxin, on the acquisition of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference (CPP) and CTA in male DBA/2J mice. Methods: Mice in the CPP experiments received four pairings of ethanol (2 g/kg) with a distinctive floor stimulus for a 5-min conditioning session (CS+ sessions). During CS+ sessions, mice also received bicuculline (0, 1.0, 3.0, or 5.0 mg/kg) or picrotoxin (2.0 mg/kg) before an injection of ethanol. On intervening days (CS– sessions), the pretreatment injection was always vehicle followed by saline injections that were paired with a different floor type. For the preference test, all mice received saline injections and were placed on a half grid and half hole floor for a 60-min session. For the CTA experiments, mice were adapted to a 2-h per day water restriction regimen followed by five conditioning trials every 48 h. During conditioning trials, subjects received an injection of vehicle, bicuculline (0.5 and 2.0 mg/kg), or picrotoxin (0.75 and 2.5 mg/kg) before injection of 2 g/kg ethanol or saline following 1-h access to a saccharin solution. Results: Both picrotoxin and the lowest dose of bicuculline (1.0 mg/kg) significantly increased the magnitude of CPP relative to vehicle-treated controls. Picrotoxin alone did not produce place conditioning. Ethanol-stimulated locomotor activity was significantly reduced during conditioning trials with picrotoxin and the higher doses of bicuculline (3.0 and 5.0 mg/kg). Bicuculline did not alter ethanol-induced CTA; however, picrotoxin dose-dependently increased the magnitude of ethanol-induced CTA. Bicuculline and picrotoxin did not produce CTA when administered alone. Conclusions: Overall, these results suggest that blockade of GABAA receptors with bicuculline and picrotoxin enhances ethanol’s motivational effects in the CPP paradigm; however, only picrotoxin enhances ethanol’s motivational effects in the CTA paradigm. Received: 12 September 1998 / Final version: 21 December 1998  相似文献   

2.
The present study was designed to determine whether a signal for availability of self-administered ethanol would acquire the ability to elicit a conditioned thermal response and to alter ethanol self-administration. Non-deprived male albino rats (n=8) were exposed to a differential conditioning procedure in which brief (30-min) periods of access to sweetened ethanol on a fixed-ratio operant schedule were either signalled (CS+ trials) or unsignalled (Blank + trials). A different stimulus signalled trials on which barpressing was not reinforced (CS– trials). Body temperature was recorded continuously from implanted telemetry devices. As in previous studies involving experimenter-administered ethanol injections, the stimulus paired with self-administered ethanol (CS+) acquired the ability to elicit a conditioned increase in body temperature. Moreover, barpressing for ethanol was greater on signalled trials (CS+) than on unsignalled trials (Blank +), indicating that ethanol's reinforcing efficacy was altered by CS+. Ethanol self-administration was significantly correlated with the anticipatory increase in body temperature on CS+ trials (Pearsonr=+0.77). When ethanol was removed, leaving sucrose alone as the reinforcer, the signal's effect on barpressing was eliminated. This finding suggests the signal's effect depended on ethanol's pharmacological properties. In general, these data are consistent with theories that attribute the signal's effect to conditioned changes in motivation to obtain ethanol or to an interaction between the conditioned response and ethanol's unconditioned effects. The specific pattern of results appears to support hypotheses linking ethanol's thermal and motivational effects. According to this view, conditioned hyperthermia produced tolerance to an aversive ethanol effect (hypothermia) that normally contributes to termination of an ethanol drinking bout, thereby allowing a longer period of self-administration on signalled trials.  相似文献   

3.
In a prior study, ad libitum fed rats learned a strong preference (90%) for a flavored saccharin solution (conditioned stimulus, CS+) paired with concurrent intragastric (IG) infusions of 5% ethanol over another flavor (CS-) paired with water infusions in unlimited access sessions (22 h/day). The present study expanded the investigation of ethanol-conditioned preferences to limited access sessions (30 min/day). Experiment 1 revealed that ad lib or food-restricted rats failed to develop a CS+ preference using the same CS solutions (0.05% Kool-Aid+0.2% saccharin) and IG infusions that were effective with long-term training. Experiments 2 and 3 mimicked the parameters from a report of successful ethanol conditioning in deprived rats: ethanol (0.5 g/kg) or water was infused intragastrically 5 min before access to sweetened CS solutions flavored with HCl or NaCl. Rats learned to prefer the ethanol-paired CS+ when the flavors were mixed with 5% sucrose but not when mixed with 0.2% saccharin. Experiment 4 revealed that 5% sucrose solutions flavored with 0.25% Kool Aid also supported flavor preference conditioning by IG ethanol (0.5 g/kg). CS+ preferences were obtained in rats trained with ethanol infused 5 min before or concurrent with CS+ intake, but not in rats trained with ethanol infused 30 min before CS+ intake. These data confirm that flavor preferences can be conditioned by IG ethanol using a limited access procedure. However, in contrast to 22 h/day training, 30 min/day training requires more intense CS flavors and a nutritive sweetener. The preference reinforcing actions of ethanol may develop slowly and are thus most effective with long training sessions or when intense CS flavors are used in short training sessions.  相似文献   

4.
In our prior studies, systemic administration of the opioid receptor antagonist naltrexone (NTX) did not block flavor preference conditioning by the sweet taste or post-oral actions of sugar despite reducing intake. Because opioid signaling in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) is implicated in food reward, this study determined if NTX administered into the NAc would block the expression of sugar-conditioned preferences. In Experiment 1, food-restricted rats with bilateral NAc shell or core cannulae were trained to drink a fructose (8%) + saccharin (0.2%) solution mixed with one flavor (CS+) and a less-preferred 0.2% saccharin solution mixed with another flavor (CS−) during one-bottle sessions. Two-bottle tests with the two flavors mixed in saccharin solutions occurred 10 min following total bilateral NAc shell or core doses of 0, 1, 25 and 50 μg of NTX. The rats preferred the CS+ over CS− following vehicle (80%) and all NTX doses in the shell and core. The CS+ preference was reduced to 64% and 72% by 50 μg NTX in the shell and core, although only the core effect was significant. In Experiment 2, food-restricted rats were trained to drink one flavored saccharin solution (CS+) paired with an intragastic (IG) glucose (8%) infusion and a second flavored saccharin solution (CS−) paired with an IG water infusion. In subsequent two-bottle tests, the rats displayed significant preferences for the CS+ (81-91%) that were unaltered by any NTX dose in the shell or core. CS+ intake, however, was reduced by NTX in the shell, but not the core. These data indicate that accumbal opioid antagonism slightly attenuated, but did not block the expression of sugar-conditioned flavor preferences. Therefore, while opioid drugs can have potent effects on sugar intake they appear less effective in altering sugar-conditioned flavor preferences.  相似文献   

5.
 Four experiments examined the effect of naloxone pretreatment on the expression and extinction of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference (experiments 1, 2, 4) or conditioned place aversion (experiments 1, 3). DBA/2 J mice received four pairings of a distinctive tactile (floor) stimulus (CS) with injection of ethanol (2 g/kg) given either immediately before or after 5-min exposure to the CS. A different stimulus was paired with injection of saline. Pre-CS injection of ethanol produced conditioned place preference, whereas post-CS injection of ethanol produced conditioned place aversion. Both behaviors extinguished partially during repeated choice testing after vehicle injection. Naloxone (10 mg/kg) had little effect on the initial expression of conditioned place preference, but facilitated its extinction. Moreover, repeated naloxone testing resulted in the expression of a weak conditioned place aversion to the CS that initially elicited a place preference. In contrast, naloxone (1.5 or 10 mg/kg) enhanced expression of conditioned place aversion, thereby increasing its resistance to extinction. A control experiment (experiment 4) indicated that repeated testing with a different aversive drug, lithium chloride, did not affect rate of extinction or produce an aversion to the CS previously paired with ethanol. These findings do not support the suggestion that naloxone facilitates the general processes that underlie extinction of associative learning. Also, these data are not readily explained by the conditioning of place aversion at the time of testing. Rather, naloxone’s effects appear to reflect a selective influence on maintenance of ethanol’s conditioned rewarding effect, an effect that may be mediated by release of endogenous opioids. Overall, these findings encourage further consideration of the use of opiate antagonists in the treatment of alcoholism. Received: 4 December 1997 / Final version: 16 February 1998  相似文献   

6.
Sprague-Dawley rats were trained 22 h/day to associate a flavored solution [conditioned stimulus (CS+)] with intragastric infusions of 6% ethanol and another flavored solution (CS-) with water infusions. The infusions were matched to the CS intakes so that the animals determined their timing and size. In Phase 1, chow and water were available ad libitum, and both CS flavors were initially sweetened with saccharin that was then faded out. The rats displayed a preference for the CS+ over the CS- under both reinforced and extinction conditions. When food-restricted in Phase 2, the rats displayed an increased preference for the CS+. In Phase 3, the rats were fed ad libitum chow and given preference tests with the CS+ paired with ethanol infusions of increasing concentration (6%, 12%, 18%, and 24%). Their preference for the CS+ over the CS- persisted, and self-administered ethanol dose increased with concentration to 5 g/kg/day. The ethanol-based conditioned flavor preference resembled those conditioned by carbohydrate and fat infusions, suggesting that at least some of reinforcing ability of ethanol may be related to its postingestive nutritive effects.  相似文献   

7.
Shram MJ  Li Z  Lê AD 《Psychopharmacology》2008,197(1):45-58
Rationale Epidemiological evidence suggests that adolescents may exhibit a unique susceptibility to the motivational effects of nicotine compared to adults. In contrast to the hypothesis of an enhanced vulnerability to nicotine during adolescence, we have observed that nicotine is less reinforcing in adolescent compared to adult rats using a progressive ratio reinforcement schedule in an operant self-administration procedure, although prior operant conditioning experience may have masked differences in initial sensitivity to nicotine. Objectives This study examined the spontaneous acquisition of nicotine self-administration in adolescent (postnatal day (PD) 31) and adult (PD87) male Wistar and Long–Evans rats. Materials and methods Rats self-administered nicotine (0.015 or 0.03 mg/kg/infusion, i.v.) during 2-h operant conditioning sessions under fixed-ratio-1 (FR1) and FR3 reinforcement schedules for six sessions each. A subset of rats (adolescents: PD42, adults: PD98) underwent extinction of responding and nicotine priming-induced reinstatement (0.15 mg/kg, s.c.). In a separate group of rats, saccharin self-administration (0.1 ml of 0.2% w/v) was tested to determine the specificity of our findings with nicotine. Results A greater proportion of adult compared to adolescent rats acquired self-administration of 0.015 mg/kg/infusion nicotine, but both age groups readily acquired self-administration of 0.03 mg/kg/infusion nicotine and saccharin. Age differences in extinction of responding for nicotine or saccharin depended upon strain, but priming-induced reinstatement was similar across age and strain. Conclusions The current findings are consistent with those obtained under a more demanding progressive ratio reinforcement schedule and suggest that adolescents, compared to adults, may not be as sensitive to the reinforcing effects of nicotine.  相似文献   

8.
A previous report showed that outbred rats acquired preferences for a sweetened conditioned stimulus (CS) flavor paired with intragastric ethanol. To evaluate the role of sweet taste in ethanol conditioning, this study compared training with sweetened and unsweetened flavors. In Experiment 1, nondeprived rats were trained to drink one flavored solution (CS+, e.g., grape) paired with intragastric infusion of 5% ethanol and another (CS-, e.g., cherry) paired with intragastric water on alternate days. The volume of ethanol solution infused was matched to the volume of flavored solution the rats consumed. The sweet group's flavors initially contained 0.2% saccharin, reduced to 0.1%, 0.05%, and 0% over days; the plain group's flavors were unsweetened. The sweet group drank more and self-infused more ethanol during training and its preference for the CS+ over the CS- (without saccharin) exceeded that of the plain group (75% versus 62%). Experiment 2 equated total ethanol intake in rats trained with two combinations of flavor quality and ethanol concentration. The Sweet5 group drank flavors with 0.2% saccharin throughout training and tests and received 5% ethanol when they drank CS+, while the Plain10 group drank unsweetened flavors and the CS+ was paired with 10% ethanol. Despite equal daily ethanol doses, the Sweet5 group strongly preferred the CS+ (89%) while the Plain10 group avoided it (31%). The two groups continued to show opposite CS+ preference profiles even when both were tested with sweet CS flavors and 10% ethanol infusions. Thus, sweet taste contributes to the development of ethanol-conditioned flavor preferences, and this effect is not explained by a simple enhancement of ethanol intake.  相似文献   

9.
Rationale There is evidence to suggest that acetaldehyde is involved in the control of ethanol-seeking behavior and reward. d-penicillamine, a thiol amino acid, is a highly selective agent for the inactivation of acetaldehyde. Previous studies from our laboratory have demonstrated that d-penicillamine prevents both behavioral stimulation induced by ethanol and acetaldehyde-produced locomotor depression in mice. Objectives The contribution of ethanol-derived acetaldehyde to the affective effects of ethanol (preference and aversion) was assessed using an unbiased place conditioning design. Methods Male mice received four pairings of a distinctive floor stimulus (CS+: GRID+ or HOLE+) with injections of saline and ethanol (2 g/kg) given before (preference) or after (aversion) the 5-min exposure to the place conditioning apparatus. A different floor stimulus (CS−: GRID− or HOLE−), associated with saline-saline injections on alternate days, was presented. For a different group of animals, the pairings with the CS+ were associated with saline and ethanol injections, but on alternate days, they received d-penicillamine (50 or 75 mg/kg) and ethanol injections paired with the CS−floor stimulus. A 60-min preference test was carried out 24 h after the last conditioning trial. A similar procedure was followed to test the effect of d-penicillamine on morphine (16 mg/kg) and cocaine-induced (20 mg/kg) conditioned place preference (CPP). Results CPP and conditioned place aversion (CPA) were observed for ethanol, but d-penicillamine only blocked CPP. d-penicillamine, by itself, did not produce either rewarding or aversive effects. CPP observed for morphine and cocaine was unaffected by d-penicillamine pretreatment. Conclusions The results of the present study suggest that the selective inactivation of acetaldehyde blocked the rewarding, but not aversive, effects of ethanol and support the role of this ethanol metabolite in the affective properties of ethanol.  相似文献   

10.
Rationale Recent research indicates that blockade of mGluR5 modifies the reinforcing properties of ethanol.Objectives The present studies examined the effects of mGluR5 receptor blockade in a genetic model of high ethanol intake, the alcohol-preferring (P) rat, on the maintenance of operant ethanol self-administration. In addition, we determined the effect of 2-methyl-6-(phenylethyl)-pyridine (MPEP) on the repeated alcohol deprivation effect.Methods Twelve male (P) rats were trained in experimental sessions to self-administer 10% w/v ethanol via a sucrose-fading procedure. After the establishment of operant ethanol self-administration, subjects were treated with various metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR) subtype antagonists immediately prior to experimental sessions: the mGluR5 antagonist MPEP (1, 3, and 10 mg/kg); the mGluR2–3 antagonist LY-341495 (1, 3, and 10 mg/kg); and the mGluR1 antagonist CPCCOEt (1, 3, and 10 mg/kg). After determining the role of mGluR5 in the maintenance of operant ethanol self-administration, we examined the role of this receptor in relapse following repeated periods of alcohol deprivation by depriving subjects of ethanol exposure for three 2-week deprivation periods.Results The mGluR5 antagonist MPEP dose-dependently decreased operant ethanol self-administration. In addition, rats that received saline immediately prior to repeated alcohol deprivation sessions self-administered ethanol at increasing levels that were above those achieved in the last operant-conditioning session prior to the initial 2-week deprivation period. This repeated alcohol deprivation effect was prevented in subjects pretreated with MPEP (10 mg/kg).Conclusions These findings suggest that mGluR5 receptors may modulate both the maintenance of operant ethanol self-administration and abstinence-induced increases in ethanol intake.  相似文献   

11.
Rationale Although many studies have shown Pavlovian conditioned approach to cues paired with natural reinforcers, it has been quite difficult to induce such behavior with drug reinforcers. Objectives This experiment tested a novel Pavlovian procedure for inducing approach to a conditioned stimulus (CS) paired with ethanol. Methods Mice (NZB/B1NJ, DBA/2J) received intraperitoneal injections of ethanol (2 g/kg) immediately before 10-min exposure to a rectangular chamber that contained a distinctive visual cue (star) at one end (Paired group, CS+ trials). On alternate days, saline injection preceded apparatus exposure with no distinctive cues (CS− trials). Unpaired control mice received ethanol in the home cage 60–75 min after each CS+ trial. Results NZB/B1NJ Paired group mice spent increasing amounts of time (>85% of the session) in proximity to the star, whereas Unpaired group mice did not. DBA/2J Paired group mice spent slightly more time on the star side than Unpaired group mice but did not show an acquisition curve. Postconditioning tests showed a strong preference for the star side in Paired groups from both strains after saline injection. However, only NZB/B1NJ mice showed a preference after ethanol. Conclusions This study provides the first unambiguous demonstration of Pavlovian conditioned approach to an ethanol-paired visual stimulus in the absence of any contingency between the animal’s behavior and drug exposure. This effect, which is remarkable both in terms of its magnitude and the rapidity with which it was produced (within 2–3 trials), may be related to the cue-associated craving that accompanies alcohol and drug addiction.  相似文献   

12.
Rationale In Pavlovian conditioning research, nicotine is typically conceptualized as the unconditioned stimulus (US) that becomes associated with an exteroceptive conditioned stimulus (CS). This research has not explored the possibility that nicotine can also function as a CS.Objectives The present research examined whether nicotine served as a CS for the presence (CS+) or absence (CS–) of sucrose and started defining its specificity.Methods and results Rats trained in the CS+ condition had nicotine (0.4 mg/kg, base) paired intermittently with brief access to sucrose. Intermixed were saline sessions without sucrose. Nicotine acquired the ability to evoke goal tracking. This conditioned response (CR) decreased across extinction sessions. The CR was sensitive to nicotine dose (ED50=0.113 mg/kg) and administration to testing interval; 0-min and 100-min delays produced no CR. The CS properties were specific to nicotine in that amphetamine and bupropion substitution was incomplete. Rats in the CS– condition received similar discrimination training except that sucrose was paired with saline. Nicotine also served as a CS–; the saline state CS+ acquired control of goal tracking. Mecamylamine, but not hexamethonium, blocked nicotines ability to serve as a CS+ and CS–, indicating a role for central nicotinic acetylcholine receptors.Conclusions Nicotine served as a signal for the presence or absence of sucrose. The extinction, CS–, and substitution results eliminated a psychomotor stimulant account. The conceptualization of nicotine as a CS suggests novel empirical research in which a drug acquires additional inhibitory and/or excitatory value based on other outcomes present during its effects.  相似文献   

13.
Previous work suggest aversive and appetitive hedonic effects of intraorally delivered EtOH in pre-weanling rats. Pups are reluctant to perform an operant response when reinforced with intraoral EtOH infusions, a result suggesting aversive orosensory properties of EtOH. Yet, post-absorptive effects of ethanol seem capable of supporting appetitive conditioning. Two experiments were conducted to test this phenomenon. Both included a pre-exposure phase (postnatal day 13, PD13) comprising intraoral stimulation with water or EtOH. In Experiment 1, pups were given pairings between a tactile conditioned stimulus (CS) and intraoral infusions of EtOH or water. A subsequent tactile preference test revealed that pups spent significantly less on the EtOH-related CS relative to time spent on the alternative CS. In Experiment 2 pups were exposed to a texture CS (sandpaper) while intraorally infused with EtOH or during a later EtOH post-infusion interval. A tactile locational test conducted on PD16 indicated that EtOH-pre-exposed animals that experienced sandpaper paired with EtOH's post-absorptive effects exhibited a significant preference for the CS, even relative to a control group that experienced non-reinforced exposure to the tactile CS during conditioning. These results confirm that intraoral ethanol acts as an aversive tastant. A brief pre-exposure to EtOH allows later expression of appetitive learning mediated by the drug's post-ingestive effects.  相似文献   

14.
 Approach responses, consummatory behaviors, and directed motor responses maintained by food reward resemble autoshaping CRs and are increased by lower doses of ethanol. This study evaluated the effects of presession IP injections of ethanol doses (0.00, 0.25, 0.50, 0.70, or 1.00 g/kg) on the acquisition of lever-press autoshaping CR performance in groups of male Long-Evans hooded rats. Paired groups received 15 daily sessions of Pavlovian autoshaping procedures, wherein the insertion of a retractable lever for 5 s (CS) was followed by the response-independent presentation of food (US). Ethanol facilitated lever-press autoshaping CR acquisition, as revealed by dose-related increases in the number of trials on which CRs were performed. The form of the dose-effect curve was inverted U-shaped with maximal responding induced during sessions 1–5 by the 0.70 g/kg ethanol dose. A similar dose-effect curve was observed during sessions 11–15, revealing that the effects of ethanol on autoshaping CR performance were relatively stable. A pseudoconditioning control group injected presession with 0.50 g/kg ethanol received training wherein the food US was presented randomly with respect to the lever CS. Few lever-presses were performed by the Random 0.50 group, indicating that ethanol’s effects on autoshaping CR acquisition and maintenance observed in the Paired 0.50 group were not due to its psychomotor activating effects. A non-injection control group performed more autoshaping CRs than did the control group injected presession with saline, indicating that daily presession IP injections per se suppress autoshaping CR performance. Results reveal that low doses of ethanol enhance Pavlovian conditioning of directed motor and consummatory-like responding maintained by food reward. Implications for autoshaping accounts of impulsivity and drug abuse are considered. Received: 15 December 1997 / Final version: 27 March 1998  相似文献   

15.
In three separate place preference conditioning (PPC) experiments, groups of rats were exposed to different modes of receiving cocaine: IP cocaine doses (7.5 mg/kg), PO cocaine self-administered bolus doses (15 mg/kg), and 1-h schedule-induced cocaine-solution drinking sessions (19.1 mg/kg). Oral cocaine self-administration of PO bolus and schedule induction took place in situations that preceded transfer into an apparatus for PPC sessions. Thus, the reinforcing efficacies of the pharmacological consequences of both oral cocaine self-administration methods were evaluated by a procedure separate from the self-administration behavior itself. The IP cocaine dose imposition and the two oral cocaine self-administration arrangements all resulted in dose-exposure conditions sufficient for the production of PPC. The serum and brain cocaine pharmacokinetics sufficient for the production of reinforcing efficacy were measured and related to previous data.  相似文献   

16.
The study examined the effects of cocaine on learning and performance of a classically conditioned heart rate (HR) discrimination in rats involving two auditory conditioned stimuli (CSs). In the discrimination protocol, one CS (CS+) was paired with the shock unconditioned stimulus (US) on a consistent basis and the other CS (CS–) was always presented alone. Four groups received an IP injection of 1, 3, 10, or 30 mg/kg cocaine and a fifth group received saline. Shortly after the injections, all groups were given six CS-alone trials, followed by 24 randomly sequenced discrimination conditioning trials (12 CS+ and 12 CS–). Approximately 72 h later, all groups were given six test trials with each CS in the absence of cocaine to evaluate the presence or absence of discrimination learning. All cocaine groups showed impaired discrimination performance on the discrimination conditioning trials, reductions in early pretest CS-alone responses, and reductions in resting HR. However, on the non-drug test trials discrimination performance was normal in all cocaine groups. The results established that in spite of major changes in HR dynamics, learning of the HR discrimination was not affected by cocaine but that cocaine did interfere with the performance of the discrimination. Except for the highest 30 mg group, the performance decrement appeared to be related to a cocaine-produced reduction in the capacity to inhibit bradycardia responding to the safe CS–. It was suggested that this loss of inhibitory control may have been due to cocaine changes in a corticothalamic pathway that controls inhibition of bradycardia to a safe CS–.  相似文献   

17.
In our prior studies, administration of the opioid receptor antagonist naltrexone did not block conditioned preferences for a flavor paired with a preferred sugar solution over a flavor paired with saccharin. This may be because both training solutions were sweet, and their attractiveness was reduced by naltrexone. The present study compared the effects of naltrexone on preferences for flavors paired with sugar or starch drinks that have distinctive tastes to rats. Experiment 1 assessed naltrexone's effect on the preference for unflavored 8% cornstarch and 8% sucrose aqueous solutions/suspensions. The food-restricted rats displayed a significant sucrose preference which increased following systemic treatment with naltrexone (1 or 3 mg/kg) even though total intake of both solutions declined. In Experiment 2, rats were trained to drink flavored (cherry or grape) starch and sucrose solutions in separate one-bottle sessions. In a two-bottle choice test with both flavors presented in a sucrose-starch mixture, the rats significantly preferred the starch-paired flavor. Naltrexone treatment blocked the expression of this starch-conditioned preference. In Experiment 3, rats were treated with saline or naltrexone throughout one-bottle training with flavored sucrose and starch solutions. In a subsequent choice test, both the saline and naltrexone groups displayed significant preferences for the starch-paired flavor, indicating that opioid antagonism failed to alter the acquisition of this conditioned preference. In summary, novel outcomes of this study included the increased rather than the predicted decrease in sucrose preference produced by naltrexone. Also, starch unexpectedly conditioned the stronger flavor preference, although this can be explained by the differential post-oral reinforcing actions of starch and sucrose, and naltrexone blocked the expression, but not the acquisition, of this preference. These findings suggest that the reward value of starch in liquid form is more dependent upon opioid signaling than is that of sugar.  相似文献   

18.
Genetic and environmental factors in ethanol self-administration   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Findings presented in this paper from pharmacogenetic studies of oral ethanol self-administration suggest a correlation between ethanol preference and self-administration and indicate that there are important genetic as well as environmental determinants of ethanol reinforced behavior. AA (alcohol accepting) and ANA (alcohol nonaccepting) rats, animals bred selectively for differential ethanol preference, showed large differences in operant responding for ethanol. AA rats drank significantly more ethanol than water, and their intake varied as a function of ethanol concentration. Intake of water and ethanol solutions did not differ in the ANA rats. In two inbred strains of rats, F344 and LEWIS, ethanol maintained higher response rates and was consumed in larger volumes than the water vehicle. In a third series of studies, C57BL/6J mice, which exhibit high ethanol preference and low sensitivity, readily self-administered ethanol in an operant situation. Conversely, BALB/cJ mice, which exhibit low preference and high sensitivity, were not positively reinforced by ethanol. The results demonstrate the experimental control possible by the utilization of genetically defined animals, even when complex learned behavioral sequences are being measured, and indicate that genotype and environment interact in a complex but definable way to determine the degree to which ethanol comes to function as a positive reinforcer.  相似文献   

19.
Dopamine D3 receptor knockout mice and the motivational effects of ethanol   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
Dopamine D3 receptors have been implicated in the behavioral effects of abused drugs including ethanol. The present experiments characterized the acquisition of ethanol-induced place conditioning and ethanol self-administration in D3 knockout (D3 KO) mice compared with C57BL/6J (C57) mice. For place conditioning, D3 KO and C57 mice received six pairings of a tactile stimulus with ethanol (3 g/kg i.p.). D3 KO mice showed higher basal locomotor activity levels in comparison with the C57 mice during conditioning. Ethanol produced similar magnitudes of conditioned place preference in both genotypes. In a two-bottle drinking procedure, mice of each genotype received 24 h access to water and either 3% or 10% v/v ethanol. No difference was noted between D3 KO and C57 mice in either consumption or preference. In an operant self-administration procedure using 23 h sessions, D3 KO and C57 mice received access to 10% v/v ethanol on an FR4 schedule of reinforcement, food on an FR1 schedule of reinforcement and water from a sipper tube. D3 KO and C57 mice had similar response rates of ethanol and food as well as similar water intakes. Overall, these results indicate that elimination of D3 receptor function has little influence on ethanol reward or intake.  相似文献   

20.
Rationale and objective Effects of corticosterone on place conditioning to ethanol were investigated in mice using two conditioning schedules; the conventional method and a rapid conditioning schedule in which exposure to the CS+ followed immediately on exposure to the CS–.Methods Effects of administration of corticosterone, 10 mg/kg, on the acquisition of place conditioning produced by ethanol, 1–2.5 g/kg, were investigated using the conventional method of conditioning, with exposure to the CS+ and the CS– on alternate days, and also using the rapid conditioning method. Total and free blood corticosterone concentrations were measured after administration of ethanol and corticosterone.Results In the conventional, alternate day, conditioning schedule, ethanol produced significant place preference at 2 and at 2.5 g/kg, but when these alcohol doses were given with corticosterone 10 mg/kg, significant place conditioning was not seen. In contrast, in the rapid, same day, conditioning schedule corticosterone significantly decreased the dose at which ethanol produced an apparent place preference, with significant place conditioning being seen with ethanol at 1 and 1.5 g/kg in combination with corticosterone, 10 mg/kg. Total and free corticosterone concentrations were increased after ethanol, 1.5 g/kg, compared with controls, and administration of corticosterone, 10 mg/kg, caused a significantly greater increase. There were no significant differences in spontaneous locomotor activity or brain alcohol concentrations between any of the treatment groups.Conclusions The effects of corticosterone on ethanol-induced place conditioning are substantially affected by the conditioning schedule used.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号