首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
《Acta oto-laryngologica》2012,132(8):858-862
Conclusions. Postural responses differed according to the stimulus direction, i.e. vertical visual stimulation induced head rather than trunk displacements. Accordingly, it could be that center of foot pressure (COP) responses tended to underestimate the postural sway during visual stimulation. Objectives. To investigate head and body sway in response to vertical visual surround motion, and to examine the correlation between the displacements of head and body segments derived from video-motion analysis and COP measurements. Material and methods. Postural sway was assessed in 10 young female subjects by video-motion analysis of four different head and body segments, and by use of force-plate posturography. Head and body sway in the pitch plane was induced by rotating a random pattern of dots about the subject's inter-aural axis at a constant acceleration of 1°/s2 or a constant velocity of 60°/s in darkness. Results. Generally, head displacement was greater than that of other body parts during vertical optokinetic stimulation (OKS). In most subjects, maximum head displacements were induced in the same direction as the visual motion. Downward OKS induced a forward head and body sway. The COP trajectory correlated well with the displacements of each head and body segment during downward OKS. In contrast, postural responses to upward OKS were complicated in terms of their time course. The correlation coefficient between each head and body segment and the COP varied among individuals for upward OKS.  相似文献   

2.
We examined the effect of a 3-dimensional stereoscopic scene on segmental stabilization. Eight subjects participated in static sway and locomotion experiments with a visual scene that moved sinusoidally or at constant velocity about the pitch or roll axes. Segmental displacements, Fast Fourier Transforms, and Root Mean Square values were calculated. In both pitch and roll, subjects exhibited greater magnitudes of motion in head and trunk than ankle. Smaller amplitudes and frequent phase reversals suggested control of the ankle by segmental proprioceptive inputs and ground reaction forces rather than by the visual-vestibular signals. Postural controllers may set limits of motion at each body segment rather than be governed solely by a perception of the visual vertical. Two locomotor strategies were also exhibited, implying that some subjects could override the effect of the roll axis optic flow field. Our results demonstrate task dependent differences that argue against using static postural responses to moving visual fields when assessing more dynamic tasks.  相似文献   

3.
CONCLUSION: Our results indicate that the illusion of self-motion is a significant factor leading to spatial disorientation. OBJECTIVE: Under normal circumstances, self-motion is perceived in response to motion of the head and body. However, under certain conditions, such as virtual reality environments, visually induced self-motion can be perceived even though the subject is not actually moving, a phenomenon known as "vection". The aim of this study was to examine the possible influence of illusory self-rotation (circular vection) on postural adjustments. MATERIAL AND METHODS: The subjects were 10 young females with no history of ocular or vestibular disease. Video-motion analysis was applied to measure postural movements during vertical optokinetic stimulation. RESULTS: For most subjects, movement of the visual surroundings induced head and body displacements in the same direction as that of the visual stimulus, regardless of the onset of self-motion perception. However, there was a significant increase in postural instability after the subjects began to perceive false self-motion in the opposite direction to that of the visual stimulus.  相似文献   

4.
CONCLUSIONS: This study supports the hypothesis that postural sway and autonomic responses to moving visual stimuli may be associated with motion sickness susceptibility. Characteristics of the cardiac sympathovagal balance during exposure to provocative stimulation may be a marker of individual susceptibility to motion sickness. OBJECTIVE: To assess the relationship between postural and autonomic responses to a simulated visual motion environment and reported susceptibility to motion sickness. MATERIAL AND METHODS: Fifteen healthy subjects were exposed to sinusoidally oscillating visual motion in roll at frequencies of 0.1-0.4 Hz. Recordings were made of postural sway and respiratory frequency and electrocardiograms were obtained from which heart rate variability (HRV) was computed in order to probe cardiac sympathetic and parasympathetic activity. RESULTS: In subjects with a low susceptibility to motion sickness as rated using a standardized questionnaire, there was no significant effect of visual stimulus on postural sway or HRV at any frequency of motion. Subjects with a high susceptibility to motion sickness showed significant postural instability induced by visual stimuli (p < 0.01). Visual stimuli presented at a frequency of 0.1 Hz significantly increased the low-frequency power (LF) of HRV, decreased the high-frequency power (HF) of HRV and increased the LF:HF ratio in these subjects (p < 0.05).  相似文献   

5.
OBJECTIVES: (1) To determine the feasibility of the use of a modified postural control test under altered sensory conditions in children over 8 years of age, and (2) to assess how deaf children use sensory information for postural control when they have normal or abnormal vestibular responses, and if hearing input from a unilateral cochlear implant, changes their postural behavior. PATIENTS: We selected 36 children, 8 to 11 years of age, with congenital or early-acquired profound sensorineural hearing loss, 13 of them with unilateral cochlear implantation and 22 normal-hearing children. METHODS: The Postural Control (PC) test consists of a force platform with 2 stimulation paradigm conditions: (1) standing on the platform with opened eyes; (2) standing on foam placed on the force platform with closed eyes. Implanted children were tested with the implant turn on and turn off in this condition, in order to evaluate eventual change in the postural control parameters when they have hearing habilitation. The body center of pressure distribution area (COP) and the body sway velocity (SV) were the parameter to evaluate the postural control. RESULTS: Deaf children were classified into two groups according with the vestibular responses: group A (n=28) Children with normal vestibular rotary responses; group B (n=8) children with hypoactive responses. Children in group A had diagnoses of syndromic and non-syndromic hereditary deafness, and children in group B had inner ear malformations, post-meningitis deafness, and one child had non-syndromic hereditary deafness with hypoactive vestibular response. In condition 1, when vestibular, somatosensory and visual information were enabled, the COP and SV values did not show any statistically significant differences between groups A, B and control. In condition 2, when visual information was removed and the somatosensory input strongly modified by standing on the foam, group B showed significant higher COP and SV values than groups A and control (p<0.05). In addition, the scalograms by wavelets of children in group B had higher amplitudes increasing the sway frequencies contents up to 3 Hz, not allowing them to maintain the up right stance in similar stimulation than in condition. Implanted children of the group A and B with the implant turn on, in the condition 2, did not show any significant difference in the SV, comparing when they had the implanted turn off. Group A p=0.395 and group B p=0.465 (Wilcoxon ranked test). CONCLUSION: These findings allow us to confirm that this postural test can be performed in children over 8 years old. Also our results suggest that deaf children with associated hypoactive vestibular responses included in our study, despite the etiology of the deafness, primarily use visual and somatosensory information to maintain their postural control. Hearing habilitation with a unilateral cochlear implant has no effect on the observed sensory organization strategy.  相似文献   

6.
《Acta oto-laryngologica》2012,132(3):280-285
Conclusions This study supports the hypothesis that postural sway and autonomic responses to moving visual stimuli may be associated with motion sickness susceptibility. Characteristics of the cardiac sympathovagal balance during exposure to provocative stimulation may be a marker of individual susceptibility to motion sickness.

Objective To assess the relationship between postural and autonomic responses to a simulated visual motion environment and reported susceptibility to motion sickness.

Material and methods Fifteen healthy subjects were exposed to sinusoidally oscillating visual motion in roll at frequencies of 0.1–0.4 Hz. Recordings were made of postural sway and respiratory frequency and electrocardiograms were obtained from which heart rate variability (HRV) was computed in order to probe cardiac sympathetic and parasympathetic activity.

Results In subjects with a low susceptibility to motion sickness as rated using a standardized questionnaire, there was no significant effect of visual stimulus on postural sway or HRV at any frequency of motion. Subjects with a high susceptibility to motion sickness showed significant postural instability induced by visual stimuli (p<0.01). Visual stimuli presented at a frequency of 0.1 Hz significantly increased the low-frequency power (LF) of HRV, decreased the high-frequency power (HF) of HRV and increased the LF:HF ratio in these subjects (p<0.05).  相似文献   

7.
Galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) is a technique in which small currents are delivered transcutaneously to the afferent nerve endings of the vestibular system through electrodes placed over the mastoid bones. The applied current alters the firing rates of the peripheral vestibular afferents, causing a shift in a standing subject's vestibular perception and a corresponding postural sway. Previously, we showed that in subjects who are facing forward, stochastic bipolar binaural GVS leads to coherent stochastic mediolateral postural sway. The goal of this pilot study was to extend that work and to test the hypothesis that in subjects who are facing forward, stochastic monopolar binaural GVS leads to coherent stochastic anteroposterior postural sway. Stochastic monopolar binaural GVS was applied to ten healthy young subjects. Twenty-four trials, each containing a different galvanic input stimulus from among eight different frequency ranges, were conducted on each subject. Postural sway was evaluated through analysis of the center-of-pressure (COP) displacements under each subject's feet. Spectral analysis was performed on the galvanic stimuli and the COP displacement time series to calculate the coherence spectra. Significant coherence was found between the galvanic input signal and the anteroposterior COP displacement in some of the trials (i.e., at least one) in nine of the ten subjects. In general, the coherence values were highest for the mid-range frequencies that were tested, and lowest for the low- and high-range frequencies. However, the coherence values we obtained were lower than those we previously reported for stochastic bipolar binaural GVS and mediolateral sway. These differences may be due to fundamental characteristics of the vestibular system such as lower sensitivity to symmetric changes in afferent firing dynamics, and/or differences between the biomechanics of anteroposterior and mediolateral sway.  相似文献   

8.
A pattern of sound-induced paroxysms of the eye and head and other spinal motor neuron synkinesis (Tullio's phenomenon) in human subjects always implies either a pathological contiguity of the tympano-ossicular chain and membranous labyrinth or a dehiscence of the bone overlying the superior semicircular canal. However, it has become clear in the last decade that sound-evoked vestibular stimulation is not only a sign of disease but also a physiological phenomenon, The examination of such physiologically sound-induced vestibular (saccular) responses contributes today to the clinical testing of the vestibular organ, mainly in the form of vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials. In this study it was observed that, in a group of 20 normal subjects, a 500 Hz tonal stimulus of high intensity (105 dB HL = 118.5 dB SPL), applied monoaurally, elicited postural responses. Each subject was studied under 4 different conditions: (i) head facing forwards, eyes open; (ii) head facing forwards, eyes closed; (iii) head rotated approximately 90 degrees to the right, eyes closed: and (iv) head rotated approximately 90 degrees to the left, eyes closed. Body sway, measured using a force platform, was recorded in all subjects, with eyes either open or closed. Postural responses, which were also elicited with a 250 Hz tonal stimulus, were not observed with a tone of 2000 Hz, with legs slightly flexed or with binaural stimulation. The postural sway (head facing forwards, eyes open or closed) was in a lateral direction towards the stimulated ear: with the stimulus applied to the right ear the subject had postural sway towards the right, with the stimulus applied to the left ear towards the left. When the head was rotated approximately 90 degrees sideways and the stimulus was given facing forwards (i.e. head rotated contralaterally to stimulated ear) the postural sway was in a forward direction; when the head was rotated approximately 90 degrees sideways and the stimulus was given facing backwards (i.e. head rotated ipsilaterally to stimulated ear) the postural sway was in a backward direction. The mean values (mm) of body sway obtained with the head facing forwards and the eyes closed were higher than those with the eyes open (21.7 and 22.8 vs 15.7 and 14.7 for the right and left ears, respectively); higher mean values were obtained with the head turned to the side contralateral to the ear stimulated and the eyes closed (29.3 and 24.8 for the right and left ears, respectively). Under this condition the body sway was mainly in a forward direction. The sound-evoked vestibulopostural reflex seems to be a useful test for exploring the saccular function and, as a click-evoked vestibulocollic reflex, can be considered a physiological Tullio phenomenon.  相似文献   

9.
Patients with different vestibular disorders exhibit changes in postural behaviour when they receive visual stimuli, reproducing environmental stimulation. Postural control was studied using an AMTI Accusway platform, measuring the confidential ellipse (CE) and sway velocity (SV). Postural responses were recorded according to the following stimulation paradigm: i) without specific stimuli; ii) smooth pursuit with pure sinusoids of 0.2 Hz (foveal stimulation); and iii) optokinetic stimulation (retinal stimuli). Patients with central vestibular disorders (CVD), cerebellar damage and unilateral peripheral vestibular lesions (UPVL) in asymptomatic periods were studied. A group of normal subjects was studied as control. Signal processing was done with a scalogram by wavelets in order to observe the relation between time and frequency in postural control. While patients with CVD and cerebellar disease showed a significant increase in CE and SV in the three conditions of the paradigm compared to the normal group, the patients with UPVL showed no change. Wavelets processing showed that the main sway occurs in the Y axis (antero-posterior) and below at 0.4 Hz in normal subjects, while the CVD and cerebellar patients showed sway frequencies in both the X and Y axes. The clinical implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   

10.
A pattern of sound-induced paroxysms of the eye and head and other spinal motor neuron synkinesis (Tullio's phenomenon) in human subjects always implies either a pathological contiguity of the tympano-ossicular chain and membranous labyrinth or a dehiscence of the bone overlying the superior semicircular canal. However, it has become clear in the last decade that sound-evoked vestibular stimulation is not only a sign of disease but also a physiological phenomenon. The examination of such physiologically sound-induced vestibular (saccular) responses contributes today to the clinical testing of the vestibular organ, mainly in the form of vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials. In this study it was observed that, in a group of 20 normal subjects, a 500 Hz tonal stimulus of high intensity (105 dB HL=118.5 dB SPL), applied monoaurally, elicited postural responses. Each subject was studied under 4 different conditions: (i) head facing forwards, eyes open; (ii) head facing forwards, eyes closed; (iii) head rotated &;#44 90° to the right, eyes closed; and (iv) head rotated 90° to the left, eyes closed. Body sway, measured using a force platform, was recorded in all subjects, with eyes either open or closed. Postural responses, which were also elicited with a 250 Hz tonal stimulus, were not observed with a tone of 2000 Hz, with legs slightly flexed or with binaural stimulation. The postural sway (head facing forwards, eyes open or closed) was in a lateral direction towards the stimulated ear: with the stimulus applied to the right ear the subject had postural sway towards the right, with the stimulus applied to the left ear towards the left. When the head was rotated &;#44 90° sideways and the stimulus was given facing forwards (i.e. head rotated contralaterally to stimulated ear) the postural sway was in a forward direction; when the head was rotated &;#44 90° sideways and the stimulus was given facing backwards (i.e. head rotated ipsilaterally to stimulated ear) the postural sway was in a backward direction. The mean values (mm) of body sway obtained with the head facing forwards and the eyes closed were higher than those with the eyes open (21.7 and 22.8 vs 15.7 and 14.7 for the right and left ears, respectively); higher mean values were obtained with the head turned to the side contralateral to the ear stimulated and the eyes closed (29.3 and 24.8 for the right and left ears, respectively). Under this condition the body sway was mainly in a forward direction. The sound-evoked vestibulopostural reflex seems to be a useful test for exploring the saccular function and, as a click-evoked vestibulocollic reflex, can be considered a physiological Tullio phenomenon.  相似文献   

11.
CONCLUSION: The results suggest that after a sudden change in the visual input, postural adaptation is impaired in patients with instability and central nervous system disorders (CNSD). OBJECTIVE: The aim of this study was the assessment of postural adaptation, as a transient phenomenon, when sudden changes in visual perception occur in normal subjects and in patients with instability due to different CNSD. SUBJECTS AND METHODS: Horizontal optokinetic stimulation (40 s and suddenly stopped) was performed in 16 patients with CNSD, and also in 22 normal subjects. Measurements were made of the body center of pressure area (COP) and the body sway velocity (SV) during 10 s before and after the stop and labeling the COP trajectory. RESULTS: Values of COP and SV (Wilcoxon signed rank test, p=0.979 and 0.496, respectively) in normal subjects did not show any significant change before and after the stop. In 15 of the 16 assessed patients with instability associated with CNSD an increase of the COP and SV values (Wilcoxon signed test, p=0.001 and 0.004, respectively) was observed in the 10 s after the visual stop. COP labeling showed 'roaming' of the COP spatial evolution approaching the limits of stability.  相似文献   

12.
The aim of this study was to analyze the composition of sway in adults and "healthy" elderly people and to evaluate the influence of vibration sensation and asymmetric vestibular function on the sway pattern. Ten adults with a mean age of 37.5 years and 40 healthy senior citizens with a mean age of 74.6 years living independently in the community were studied. Vibration-induced body sway was measured on a force platform. The sway was analyzed and separated into its high and low frequency components above and below 0.1 Hz, respectively. Additionally the elderly subjects were observed for the occurrence of spontaneous gaze and head shake-induced nystagmus using infrared charge-coupled device cameras and the vibration perception in the lower limbs was tested with a tuning fork. Vibration perception was the major determinant for postural control in the elderly subjects. Postural control among the elderly subjects with intact vibration perception in their lower limbs was very similar to that of the adults. The elderly subjects with impaired vibration sensation had increased high frequency sway compared to adults and the elderly subjects with intact sensation. Regardless of the strong influence of vibration sensation on postural control, asymmetric vestibular function might also be a contributing factor to postural instability in the elderly. Age per se had little effect on the outcome of the tests except that the elderly subjects had diminished ability to use visual cues to reduce postural sway. We concluded that sensory status in the lower limbs is of utmost importance for postural control in the elderly. Rehabilitation programs for senior citizens should therefore include exercises to preserve recognition of body motion by the lower limbs. Exercises to facilitate vestibular compensation could be useful for elderly people with vestibular dysfunction.  相似文献   

13.
《Acta oto-laryngologica》2012,132(6):700-706
The aim of this study was to analyze the composition of sway in adults and "healthy" elderly people and to evaluate the influence of vibration sensation and asymmetric vestibular function on the sway pattern. Ten adults with a mean age of 37.5 years and 40 healthy senior citizens with a mean age of 74.6 years living independently in the community were studied. Vibration-induced body sway was measured on a force platform. The sway was analyzed and separated into its high and low frequency components above and below 0.1 Hz, respectively. Additionally the elderly subjects were observed for the occurrence of spontaneous gaze and head shake-induced nystagmus using infrared charge-coupled device cameras and the vibration perception in the lower limbs was tested with a tuning fork. Vibration perception was the major determinant for postural control in the elderly subjects. Postural control among the elderly subjects with intact vibration perception in their lower limbs was very similar to that of the adults. The elderly subjects with impaired vibration sensation had increased high frequency sway compared to adults and the elderly subjects with intact sensation. Regardless of the strong influence of vibration sensation on postural control, asymmetric vestibular function might also be a contributing factor to postural instability in the elderly. Age per se had little effect on the outcome of the tests except that the elderly subjects had diminished ability to use visual cues to reduce postural sway. We concluded that sensory status in the lower limbs is of utmost importance for postural control in the elderly. Rehabilitation programs for senior citizens should therefore include exercises to preserve recognition of body motion by the lower limbs. Exercises to facilitate vestibular compensation could be useful for elderly people with vestibular dysfunction.  相似文献   

14.
OBJECTIVE: Several reports have shown that the direction of the postural responses induced by vestibular stimulation is affected by the positions of the neck and torso. The aim of this study was to investigate whether the postural responses to vibratory proprioceptive stimulation of the calf muscles are affected by the position of the head and thus by proprioceptive and vestibular information from the neck and head. MATERIAL AND METHODS: Ten normal subjects were exposed to vibratory proprioceptive stimulation of the calf muscles when the head was maintained in five different positions: in a neutral position facing forwards, with the head turned to the right or left sides or with the head tilted backwards or forwards. Body movements were evaluated by analyzing the anteroposterior and lateral torques induced towards the supporting surface. RESULTS: The analysis showed that only the anteroposterior body sway was significantly affected by the position of the head. The anteroposterior postural responses were primarily increased during the tests with the head tilted backwards or forwards, whereas the postural responses were unaffected by head torsion towards the sides. The lateral responses were primarily affected by vision and not by the position of the head. CONCLUSIONS: The findings suggest that the responses evoked by vibratory proprioceptive stimulation of the calf muscles may be affected by different mechanisms, either by purely proprioceptive information or by an interaction between proprioceptive and vestibular information. Moreover, the increasing difference between the test conditions over time suggests that fatigue of the neck muscles may be one of the factors affecting the responses induced by the perturbations.  相似文献   

15.
《Acta oto-laryngologica》2012,132(2):168-172
Patients with different vestibular disorders exhibit changes in postural behaviour when they receive visual stimuli, reproducing environmental stimulation. Postural control was studied using an AMTI Accusway platform, measuring the confidential ellipse (CE) and sway velocity (SV). Postural responses were recorded according to the following stimulation paradigm: i) without specific stimuli; ii) smooth pursuit with pure sinusoids of 0.2 Hz (foveal stimulation); and iii) optokinetic stimulation (retinal stimuli). Patients with central vestibular disorders (CVD), cerebellar damage and unilateral peripheral vestibular lesions (UPVL) in asymptomatic periods were studied. A group of normal subjects was studied as control. Signal processing was done with a scalogram by wavelets in order to observe the relation between time and frequency in postural control. While patients with CVD and cerebellar disease showed a significant increase in CE and SV in the three conditions of the paradigm compared to the normal group, the patients with UPVL showed no change. Wavelets processing showed that the main sway occurs in the Y axis (antero-posterior) and below at 0.4 Hz in normal subjects, while the CVD and cerebellar patients showed sway frequencies in both the X and Y axes. The clinical implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   

16.
Body sway was investigated in 20 healthy subjects to determine whether visual input must contain motion feedback information from the surroundings in order to influence postural control. Posturography was used to record body sway under the following visual conditions: eyes open with or without a restricted visual field; eyes open in ganzfield white light; eyes open in darkness with a head-fixed visual target; eyes open in darkness; and eyes closed in darkness. Stance was perturbed by means of a pseudorandomly applied vibratory stimulation to the calf muscles. Least sway was found with eyes open in an unrestricted visual field but increased in a restricted visual field. Greatest sway was found without visual motion feedback, i.e. under the following conditions: eyes closed; eyes open in darkness; eyes open in ganzfield white light; and with a head-mounted fixation point. Sway was significantly (p < 0.05) greater with eyes open in darkness compared with eyes closed during the initial 50 s with perturbations. After 150 s, sway was almost identical under the four test conditions without visual motion feedback. Standing with eyes open in darkness was initially a disadvantage compared with having the eyes closed. The postural control system may be programmed to expect visual feedback information when the eyes are open, which may delay changes in postural strategy.  相似文献   

17.
《Acta oto-laryngologica》2012,132(1):60-64
Conclusion Our results indicate that the illusion of self-motion is a significant factor leading to spatial disorientation.

Objective Under normal circumstances, self-motion is perceived in response to motion of the head and body. However, under certain conditions, such as virtual reality environments, visually induced self-motion can be perceived even though the subject is not actually moving, a phenomenon known as “vection”. The aim of this study was to examine the possible influence of illusory self-rotation (circular vection) on postural adjustments.

Material and methods The subjects were 10 young females with no history of ocular or vestibular disease. Video-motion analysis was applied to measure postural movements during vertical optokinetic stimulation.

Results For most subjects, movement of the visual surroundings induced head and body displacements in the same direction as that of the visual stimulus, regardless of the onset of self-motion perception. However, there was a significant increase in postural instability after the subjects began to perceive false self-motion in the opposite direction to that of the visual stimulus.  相似文献   

18.
《Acta oto-laryngologica》2012,132(4):392-397
Body sway was investigated in 20 healthy subjects to determine whether visual input must contain motion feedback information from the surroundings in order to influence postural control. Posturography was used to record body sway under the following visual conditions: eyes open with or without a restricted visual field; eyes open in ganzfield white light; eyes open in darkness with a head-fixed visual target; eyes open in darkness; and eyes closed in darkness. Stance was perturbed by means of a pseudorandomly applied vibratory stimulation to the calf muscles. Least sway was found with eyes open in an unrestricted visual field but increased in a restricted visual field. Greatest sway was found without visual motion feedback, i.e. under the following conditions: eyes closed; eyes open in darkness; eyes open in ganzfield white light; and with a head-mounted fixation point. Sway was significantly ( p < 0.05) greater with eyes open in darkness compared with eyes closed during the initial 50 s with perturbations. After 150 s, sway was almost identical under the four test conditions without visual motion feedback. Standing with eyes open in darkness was initially a disadvantage compared with having the eyes closed. The postural control system may be programmed to expect visual feedback information when the eyes are open, which may delay changes in postural strategy.  相似文献   

19.
《Acta oto-laryngologica》2012,132(5):594-599
Objective --Several reports have shown that the direction of the postural responses induced by vestibular stimulation is affected by the positions of the neck and torso. The aim of this study was to investigate whether the postural responses to vibratory proprioceptive stimulation of the calf muscles are affected by the position of the head and thus by proprioceptive and vestibular information from the neck and head. Material and Methods --Ten normal subjects were exposed to vibratory proprioceptive stimulation of the calf muscles when the head was maintained in five different positions: in a neutral position facing forwards, with the head turned to the right or left sides or with the head tilted backwards or forwards. Body movements were evaluated by analyzing the anteroposterior and lateral torques induced towards the supporting surface. Results --The analysis showed that only the anteroposterior body sway was significantly affected by the position of the head. The anteroposterior postural responses were primarily increased during the tests with the head tilted backwards or forwards, whereas the postural responses were unaffected by head torsion towards the sides. The lateral responses were primarily affected by vision and not by the position of the head. Conclusion --The findings suggest that the responses evoked by vibratory proprioceptive stimulation of the calf muscles may be affected by different mechanisms, either by purely proprioceptive information or by an interaction between proprioceptive and vestibular information. Moreover, the increasing difference between the test conditions over time suggests that fatigue of the neck muscles may be one of the factors affecting the responses induced by the perturbations.  相似文献   

20.
Postural responses to support surface displacements were measured in 214 normal human subjects ranging in age from 7 to 81 y. Motor tests measured leg muscle electromyographic (EMG) latencies, body sway, and the amplitude and timing of changes in center of pressure displacements in response to sudden forward and backward horizontal translations of the support surface upon which the subjects stood. There were small increases in both EMG latencies and the time to reach the peak amplitude of center of pressure responses with increasing age. The amplitude of center of pressure responses showed no change with age if the amplitude measures were normalized by a factor related to subject height. In general, postural responses to sudden translations showed minimal changes with age, and all age-related trends that were identified were small relative to the variability within the population.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号