首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 312 毫秒
1.
Past studies of the neuronal correlates of avoidance conditioning in rabbits have led to a model of information flow among structures of the limbic system. A hypothesis of the model is that unexpected stimuli activate certain hippocampal and cingulate cortical neurons. This activity in turn suppresses or "limits" the firing of limbic thalamic neurons. This hypothesis is tested in relation to stimuli classified as unexpected or expected on the basis of their incidence or "probability." Multi-unit and field potential responses in the anterior and posterior cingulate cortices (AC and PC), the dentate gyrus (DG), and the anterior ventral (AV) and medial dorsal (MD) thalamic nuclei were recorded during the acquisition and performance of a locomotor conditioned response (CR). The CR, stepping in an activity wheel in response to a 0.5-s tone (CS+), prevented the occurrence of a shock US scheduled 5 s after CS+ onset. The rabbits also learned to ignore a different tone (CS-), not predictive of the US. Training was given daily (120 trials, 60 with each CS in an irregular sequence) until behavioral discrimination reached criterion. After criterion, asymmetric probability (AP) sessions were given, in which the CS+/CS- proportions were .2/.8 or .8/.2. The AP sessions were the same as conditioning sessions except for the probability manipulation. A significant discriminative response, i.e., a greater neuronal discharge to the CS+ than to the CS-, developed in all regions during behavioral acquisition. The unit response in the AP sessions was enhanced in all areas by rare presentation of the CS-, compared with the equal and frequent CS- conditions. Rare presentation of the CS+ enhanced the unit response in the cortical areas (AC, PC, and DG), but it suppressed the firing of limbic thalamic (AV and MD) neurons. These results were supportive of the model. Rare CS+ presentations did not alter AV and PC neuronal activity in rabbits with subicular lesions, a result suggesting that an intact hippocampus is essential for normal neuronal responses to stimulus probability in the cingulate cortex and limbic thalamus.  相似文献   

2.
Summary Four groups of male albino rabbits were trained to perform a conditioned response (CR, stepping in an activity wheel) to an acoustic (pure tone) conditional stimulus (CS+). A 1.5–2.0 mA shock unconditional stimulus (US) delivered through the grid floor of the wheel was administered 5 s after CS + onset, but stepping during the CS-US interval prevented the US. The rabbits were also trained to ignore a second tone (a negative conditional stimulus or CS-) of different auditory frequency than the CS+, that was presented in an irregular order on half of the conditioning trials but never followed by the US. One group had bilateral electrolytic lesions in the medial dorsal (MD) thalamic nucleus, a second group had combined bilateral lesions in the MD and the anterior thalamic nuclei, and a third group had no lesions. The fourth group was composed of rabbits with combined lesions that resulted in only partial damage in the anterior and MD nuclei. In all rabbits, multi-unit activity and field potentials were recorded from the cingulate cortical projection targets of the MD and anterior nuclei. The average rate of acquisition in rabbits with MD and partial lesions was not significantly different from that in controls, but the asymptotic performance in rabbits with lesions was significantly impaired, relative to that in controls. None of the rabbits that had the combined MD and anterior thalamic lesions reached the acquisition criterion. The average proportion of trials in which these rabbits performed avoidance responses during their final training sessions was 0.3, compared to 0.8 in controls. The unconditioned response was not significantly affected by the lesions, nor was there any indication that the lesions impaired the sensory processing of the CSs. These results and the massive traininginduced neuronal discharges shown in past studies to occur in the limbic thalamic neurons indicate that these neurons are importantly involved in the circuitry that mediates discriminative avoidance conditioning in rabbits. The training-induced neuronal activity in cingulate cortex was dramatically attenuated in rabbits with lesions. Differences in the degree of this attenuation between lesion conditions and with respect to training stages were discussed in relation to a theoretical working model of limbic thalamic and cingulate cortical associative functions.  相似文献   

3.
Neuronal activity of the auditory thalamus, amygdala, cingulate cortex, and substantia nigra was recorded during the administration of a behavioral test for latent inhibition (LI) or the retardation of behavioral conditioning because of preexposure of the conditional stimulus (CS). Following CS preexposure, both the preexposed CS and a control CS predicted avoidable footshock. LI occurred as significantly fewer avoidance conditioned avoidance responses after the preexposed CS than after the control CS. Attenuation of neuronal responses to the preexposed CS, or neural LI, occurred in all monitored areas. One group of subjects (Oryctolagus cuniculus) then received context extinction, and additional groups experienced novel context exposure or handling. Context extinction enhanced behavioral responding to the preexposed CS, eliminating LI. Context extinction also eliminated cingulate cortical neural LI by enhancing posterior cingulate cortical responses to the preexposed CS and attenuating anterior cingulate cortical responses to the control CS. Present and past results are interpreted to indicate that LI is (a) a failure of response retrieval and/or expression mediated by interfering CS-context associations and (b) a product of interactions of the posterior cingulate cortex and the hippocampus.  相似文献   

4.
Extracellular multiple- and single-unit recordings were made from the neostriatum of rabbits during classical eyelid conditioning. Neostriatal neurons processed information regarding the conditioned auditory stimulus (CS) and conditioned eyelid response (CR) as well as the unconditioned stimulus/response (US/UR). These data are consistent with previous reports that neostriatal neurons respond to movement and movement-related sensory stimuli. In most cases, neostriatal neurons increased activity to the US during the early phase of training, but to the CR as training progressed. A close temporal correlation was found between neuronal activity and CR onset with unit discharges typically preceding CR onset by 10–50 ms. The activity of some multiple and single units was monitored after injection of haloperidol, a neuroleptic and dopamine antagonist known to disrupt neostriatal function. Interestingly, haloperidol caused a greater disruption of CRs at low-intensity than at high-intensity CSs, but conditioning-related neuronal activity was disrupted equally at both intensities. These data are discussed in terms of a possible role for the neostriatum in eyelid conditioning.  相似文献   

5.
 Rabbits given either electrolytic lesions of the entorhinal cortex or sham-lesions were trained to prevent a foot-shock by stepping in an activity wheel after one tone, a positive conditioned stimulus (CS+), and to ignore a different tone, a negative conditioned stimulus (CS–). Neuronal activity was recorded simultaneously in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala, the CA1 cell field of hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortical area 24b and posterior cingulate cortical area 29c/d. The activity of neurons in the entorhinal cortex was recorded in the controls. Acquisition of conditioned avoidance responses (CRs) was not affected by lesions of the entorhinal cortex. Discriminative neuronal activity (greater neuronal responses to the CS+ than to the CS–) during CR acquisition was significantly enhanced in hippocampal area CA1 and attenuated in the basolateral amygdala in rabbits with lesions. Following acquisition to a criterion, two counterbalanced extinction tests were administered, one in the original context and the other in the presence of novel contextual stimuli. CR frequency was significantly reduced in controls but not in rabbits with lesions, during extinction with novel contextual stimuli, relative to performance in the original context. The rabbits with lesions also showed fewer inter-trial responses than controls during extinction in the original context but inter-trial response frequency in rabbits with lesions did not differ from the frequency in controls during extinction in the novel context. Neurons in the basolateral amygdala in controls showed discriminative activity during extinction in the original context but not in the novel context. Amygdalar neurons in the rabbits with lesions did not show discriminative activity during extinction in either context. Posterior cingulate cortical neurons in control rabbits did not show discriminative activity during extinction in the original context but these neurons exhibited robust discriminative activity in the novel context. Posterior cingulate cortical neurons in rabbits with lesions showed discriminative activity in both extinction sessions. The results indicated that the entorhinal cortex does not play a significant role in the acquisition of discriminative avoidance behavior, under the employed conditions of training. However, the interactions of neurons in the entorhinal cortex, amygdala and cingulate cortex are essential for contextual modulation of CRs during extinction. Received: 17 September 1996 / Accepted: 14 January 1997  相似文献   

6.
In four classical conditioning experiments heart period and eyeblink responses were assessed concomitantly. The conditioned stimuli (CSs) were tones and the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) was a brief paraorbital electric shock. Using a 0.5-s duration CS, bradycardiac conditioned responses that consisted of a 4–5 ms change from pre-CS baseline occurred within 10–20 CS-UCS presentations. However, eyeblink conditioned responses began to occur only after 100–150 CS–UCS presentations. A 1.0-s CS duration resulted in bradycardiac conditioned responses of 15–30 ms change from pre-CS baseline, which again reached asymptote within 10–20 trials. Using a 4-s CS duration, in a differential classical conditioning paradigm, heart period discrimination between a reinforced CS+ and a nonreinforced CS? occurred within 10 trials; asymptotic performance of the heart period conditioned response occurred within 15 CS-UCS presentations and consisted of a bradycardiac response of 40–50 ms for the 12th interbeat interval following tone onset. These data thus indicate that these two model systems of mammalian learning (based on heart period and eyeblink responses) show quite different acquisition functions. It is also significant that heart rate slowing always accompanied the eyeblink conditioned responses, even though increases in general electromyographic activity as well as eyeblink conditioned responses were simultaneously observed during CS presentation.  相似文献   

7.
Summary This study extends an ongoing analysis of the neural mediation of discriminative avoidance learning in rabbits. Electrolytic lesions encompassing anterior and posterior cingulate cortex (area 24 and 29) or ibotenic acid lesions in area 24 only were made prior to avoidance conditioning wherein rabbits learned to step in response to a tone conditional stimulus (CS+) in order to avoid a brief, response-terminated 1.5 mA. foot-shock unconditional stimulus (US). The US was presented 5 s after CS+ onset, in the absence of a prior stepping response. The rabbits also learned to ignore a different tone (CS-) not followed by the US. Multi-unit activity of the caudate and medial dorsal (MD) thalamic nuclei, projection targets of the cingulate cortex, was recorded during learning in all rabbits. Activity was also recorded in area 29 in the rabbits with area 24 lesions. Learning in rabbits with combined lesions was severely impaired and it was moderately retarded after lesions in area 24. MD thalamic and caudate training-induced neuronal discharge increments elicited by the CS+ were enhanced in rabbits with lesions, suggesting a suppressive influence of cingulate cortical projections on this activity. Early-, but not late-developing training-induced unit activity in area 29c/d was absent in rabbits with area 24 lesions, indicating that area 24 is a source of early-developing area 29 plasticity. These results are consistent with hypotheses of a theoretical working model, stating that: a) learning depends on the integrity of two functional systems, a mnemonic recency system comprised by circuitry involving area 24 and the MD nucleus and a mnemonic primacy system comprised by circuitry involving area 29 and the anterior thalamic nuclei; b) corticothalamic information flow in these systems suppresses thalamic CS elicited activity in trained rabbits; c) corticostriatal information flow is involved in avoidance response initiation. An absence of rhythmic theta-like neuronal bursts in area 29b in rabbits with area 24 lesions is attributable to passing fiber damage.  相似文献   

8.
Acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioning involves the association of a conditioned stimulus (CS) with an unconditioned stimulus (US) separated by a stimulus-free trace interval. This form of conditioning is dependent upon the hippocampus and the caudal anterior cingulate cortex (AC), in addition to brain stem and cerebellar circuitry. Hippocampal involvement in trace eyeblink conditioning has been studied extensively, but the involvement of caudal AC is less well understood. In the present study, we compared neuronal responses from rabbits given either paired (trace conditioning) or unpaired (pseudoconditioning) presentations of the CS and US. Presentation of the CS elicited significant increases in neuronal activity at the onset of both trace conditioning and pseudoconditioning. A robust CS-elicited neuronal response persisted throughout the first 2 days of trace conditioning, declining gradually across subsequent training sessions. In contrast, the magnitude of the CS-elicited excitatory response during pseudoconditioning began to decline within the first 10 trials. Neurons exhibiting excitatory responses to the CS during trace conditioning also exhibited excitatory responses to the US that were significantly greater in magnitude than US-elicited responses during pseudoconditioning. CS-elicited decreases in neuronal activity became more robust over the course of trace conditioning compared to pseudoconditioning. Reductions in activity during the CS interval consistently preceded excitation in both training groups, suggesting that the CS-elicited decreases in neuronal activity may serve to increase the signal-to-noise ratio of the excitatory response to the tone. Taken together, these data suggest that the caudal AC is involved early in trace eyeblink conditioning and that maintenance of the CS-elicited excitatory response may serve to signal the salience of the tone.  相似文献   

9.
Summary Past studies of the neural determinants of discriminative avoidance conditioning in rabbits have fostered a theoretical model that describes the interactive functioning of the cingulate cortex (Brodmann's Areas 24 and 29), the anterior ventral and medial dorsal thalamic nuclei (AVN and MDN) and the hippocampus. Here we test hypotheses of the model concerning the influence of the hippocampus on cortical and thalamic information processing. The rabbits learned to perform locomotory conditioned responses (CRs) in an activity wheel in response to an acoustic (pure tone) positive conditional stimulus (CS+). A shock unconditional stimulus (US) was given 5 s after CS+ onset, but locomotion during the CS+ -US interval prevented the US. The rabbits also learned to ignore a second tone (a negative conditional stimulus, CS-) of different auditory frequency than the CS+, that did not predict the US. Multi-unit activity and intracranial macropotentials were recorded in the cingulate cortex and the AVN uring acquisition, overtraining, extinction, reacquisition and reversal training. Data were obtained in intact rabbits and in rabbits with bilateral lesions of the subicular complex, the origin of projections of the hippocampal formation to the cingulate cortex and AVN. In addition, the activity in the AVN was recorded in a separate group of rabbits with posterior cingulate cortical (Area 29) lesions. Subicular and Area 29 lesions were associated with an enhancement of the training-induced CS+ elicited neuronal response in the AVN. The frequency of CRs was enhanced in animals with subicular lesions. CS elicited unit responses in the cingulate cortices were attenuated in rabbits with subicular lesions. Both of the lesions were associated with significantly increased amplitudes of the CS elicited average cortical and thalamic macropotentials. These results suggested the following conclusions: a) subiculocortical afferents provide an enabling influence that is essential for CS elicited excitation in the cingulate cortex; b) the cingulate cortical excitatory response in intact animals exerts a limiting influence on the activity in the AVN; c) the enhanced AVN neuronal response in rabbits with lesions is due to the absence of the limiting influence and it contributes to the increased CR frequency in those animals. It is hypothesized that the hippocampus via subiculocortical projections, governs the flow of CR-inducing thalamocortical excitatory volleys. This governance determines the timing of CR output. The results of hippocampal processing of contextual information acting through the subiculocortical projection determines the moment most appropriate for the CR.  相似文献   

10.
 In classical trace conditioning the acquisition of a conditioned response (CR) is possible even though an interval (the trace interval) elapses between the conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US). This implies that some neural representation of the CS (the stimulus trace) is able to support association between the two stimuli. The medial geniculate nucleus (MGN), particularly the medial division (mMGN), has been identified as one site in the auditory pathway where associative related changes in neural activity occur. If neurons in the MGN are involved in such a sensory trace and in acquisition of a CR, then it is expected that activity following an acoustic CS should be related to both stimulus and response. This study examined the extracellular activity of single units in the MGN during differential auditory trace conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response (NMR). Two 150-ms tones (600 Hz and 1200 Hz) served as CS+ and CS–, and the US was periorbital electrostimulation. Changes in activity during the stimulus and trace interval were largest in the medial and dorsal MGN divisions on CS+ trials and on trials in which a CR was made. Examination of probe stimuli of short (50 ms) and long (600 ms) duration suggested that both CR latency and activity changes in the trace interval were related to stimulus duration and time-locked to stimulus offset. Comparisons of neural activity on the basis of fast or slow CR responses revealed different patterns of response – activity on fast CR trials was generally greater and tended to occur earlier. These results suggest that MGN neurons are involved in the maintenance of a sensory memory trace and possibly play a part in CR generation and timing. Received: 2 June 1995 / Accepted: 30 August 1996  相似文献   

11.
During Pavlovian fear conditioning a conditioned stimulus (CS) is repeatedly paired with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (UCS). In many studies the CS and UCS are paired on every trial, whereas in others the CS and UCS are paired intermittently. To better understand the influence of the CS-UCS pairing rate on brain activity, the experimenters presented continuously, intermittently, and non-paired CSs during fear conditioning. Amygdala, anterior cingulate, and fusiform gyrus activity increased linearly with the CS-UCS pairing rate. In contrast, insula and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex responses were larger during intermittently paired CS presentations relative to continuously and non-paired CSs. These results demonstrate two distinct patterns of activity in disparate brain regions. Amygdala, anterior cingulate, and fusiform gyrus activity paralleled the CS-UCS pairing rate, whereas the insula and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex appeared to respond to the uncertainty inherent in intermittent CS-UCS pairing procedures. These findings may further clarify the role of these brain regions in Pavlovian fear conditioning.  相似文献   

12.
Daily injections of cocaine administered to pregnant rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) throughout gestation were associated with neural and behavioral changes during development and in adulthood, including altered neuron structure and function in areas receiving dopaminergic projections and retarded Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning with low-salient conditional stimuli. Studies of discriminative avoidance learning have shown changes in learning-related cingulothalamic neuronal activity, but no behavioral learning impairment in cocaine-exposed offspring. Here, low-salient stimuli were used during discriminative avoidance conditioning. Impairments early in behavioral acquisition were found, as well as alterations of anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortical, medial dorsal thalamic, and amygdalar neuronal response profiles and learning-related neuronal activity. These results elucidate the neural processes, impaired by prenatal cocaine, that support conditioning with low-salient stimuli.  相似文献   

13.
Single and multiple unit activity was recorded from the amygdaloid nuclei in awake unanesthestized rabbits during classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane (NM) response. Over half o the unit recordings from the amygdaloid complex demonstrated changes in firing rate following the presentation of the tone conditioned stimulus (CS) or corneal air-puff unconditioned stimulus (US). Of the unit records that were responsive to the training stimuli, 58% showed responses to the CS and 73% showed responses to the US. Amygdaloid responses to either the CS or the US tended to be a long latency (greater than 70 msec), long duration (greater than 250 msec), modest increase (less than 2 fold) in unit firing. There were no statistically significant differences between the spontaneous firing rates, response latencies, response magnitudes, or response type distributions seen during unpaired (control) and paired stimulus presentations. However, four of 18 single and multiple unit groups that were tested during both unpaired and paired training developed new or enhanced responses following the onset of paired training. All four of these records were from the basolateral or lateral amygdaloid nuclei. Although these few records did develop response alterations after CS-US pairing, the majority of the records indicated that essentially the same amygdaloid response patterns occur during unpaired training as during paired training. it is therefore unlikely that the critical neuronal changes that underlying NM conditioning occur within the amygdaloid complex. Other possible roles for the amygdaloid complex during conditioning are considered.  相似文献   

14.
This study explored the time course of conditioned fear response expression. Two neutral male facial expressions served as conditioned stimuli (CS) in a differential trace conditioning that involved either an aversive (n=14) or a nonaversive (n=12) unconditioned stimulus (UCS) in a between-subjects design. Skin conductance response (SCR) to the CSs and startle response magnitudes to acoustic probes presented at early (250 ms) or late (1,750 ms) probe times after CS onset were measured. As expected, conditioned SCR discrimination was observed in both aversive and nonaversive learning, whereas the conditioned potentiation of the startle response was only observed for the aversive UCS condition. Interestingly, conditioned startle discrimination was specific for the later probe time. In contrast, conditioned fear potentiation of the startle response at the early probe time was equally pronounced for CS+ and CS-. These findings suggest that fear-eliciting neural structures are rapidly activated in fear learning, whereas the expression of inhibitory conditioning requires more time, presumably reflecting the involvement of cortical top-down control processes.  相似文献   

15.
Levels of neuronal excitability to injected current were measured intracellularly in facial motoneurons and other neurons in and near the facial nuclei of three groups of awake cats: a "Conditioned" group consisting of animals that had previously received sufficient numbers of paired presentations of click CSs and glabella tap USs to produce eyeblink CRs; a "US-only" group that had received presentations of the USs only; and a "Naive" group that had received neither of these stimuli. Thresholds of intracellularly applied, depolarizing pulse currents required to elicit repeatable spike activity were significantly lower in the "Conditioned" and "US-only" groups than in the "Naive" group. The increased levels of neuronal excitability were correlated with increases in neuronal input resistance. Levels of neuronal excitability remained elevated when measured more than a month after presentations of both CSs and USs, whereas the increases in neuronal excitability decayed within a few weeks in animals given USs only. The increases in neuronal excitability and input resistance following repetitive presentations of glabella tap USs alone appeared to support a latent facilitation of motor performance reflected by an absence of a blink CR to click CS after such presentations but an increased rate of acquisition of subsequent eyeblink conditioning using paired click CS and tap US. The rate of eyeblink conditioning was found to be accelerated in a group of cats given repetitive presentations of tap USs seven days prior to conditioning with paired CSs and USs, compared to a group that was not given USs or CSs before similar conditioning. These findings provide direct, in vivo evidence that increases in the excitability and input resistance of neurons in and near the facial nucleus can occur in cats following presentations of the stimuli used for conditioning.  相似文献   

16.
Two experiments investigated phasic and tonic electrodermal responding to long, variable-duration stimuli in aversive conditioning procedures. Experiment I demonstrated reliable differential conditioning on both phasic (first interval response, FIR) and tonic (change in skin conductance level, delta SCL; spontaneous fluctuations) measures, using 10-40-s slides as conditioned stimuli (CSs) and electric shock as the unconditioned stimulus (UCS). Experiment 2 examined the sensitivity of phasic and tonic measures to UCS aversiveness. Both FIR and delta SCL were greater to a conditioned stimulus paired with shock than one paired with an innocuous reaction-time warning tone. Responding was reversed when the reinforcement contingencies were reversed. It was suggested that, with long duration CSs and an aversive UCS, phasic measures to stimulus onset, such as FIR, may reflect both stimulus significance and threat appraisal, whereas tonic measures reflect primarily arousal or anxiety arising from anticipation of the aversive unconditioned stimulus.  相似文献   

17.
Rabbits received classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response (NMR) in a trace conditioning paradigm. In this paradigm, a 250-ms tone conditioned stimulus (CS) occurs, after which there is a 500-ms period of time in which no stimuli occur (the trace interval), followed by a 100-ms air puff unconditioned stimulus (UCS). In Experiment 1, lesions of the hippocampus or cingulate/retrosplenial cortex disrupted acquisition of the long-latency or adaptive conditioned response relative to unoperated controls and animals that received neocortical lesions that spared the cingulate/retrosplenial areas. When animals with hippocampal or cingulate/retrosplenial lesions were switched to a standard delay paradigm in which the CS and UCS were contiguous in time, they acquired in about the same number of trials as naive rabbits. In a second experiment multiple-unit activity in area CA1 of the hippocampus was examined during acquisition of the trace conditioned response (CR). Three groups of animals were tested: animals that had a 500-ms trace interval (Group T-500), animals that received explicitly unpaired presentations of the CS and UCS (Group UP), and animals that underwent conditioning with a 2,000-ms trace interval (Group T-2000). Animals in Group T-500 acquired the CR in about 500 trials. Early in training, and well before any CRs occurred, there was a substantial increase in neuronal activity in the hippocampus that began during the CS and persisted through the trace interval. There was also an increase in the UCS period that modeled the amplitude-time course of the behavioral unconditioned response. Later in conditioning as CRs emerged, there was no longer neuronal bursting throughout the CS + trace period. Rather, the activity shifted to later in the trace interval and formed a model of the amplitude-time course of the behavioral CR. Activity during the UCS period was similar to that seen earlier in conditioning. Animals in Group UP showed no behavioral conditioning and no increase in neuronal activity. Animals in Group T-2000 showed no long-latency behavioral conditioning and no increase in neuronal activity. The data are discussed in terms of the role of the hippocampus in conditioning during situations in which the CS and UCS are not contiguous in time.  相似文献   

18.
Summary The activity of neurons in the interposed and dentate nuclei of the cerebellum was investigated during differential classical conditioning of the rabbit eye blink/ nictitating membrane response. Forty-seven percent of the 165 cells in the study responded to the orbital stimulation used as the unconditioned stimulus (US). The latency distribution of US-elicited responses was bimodal with peaks at 7 and 19 ms. Twenty-one percent of the cells responded with short latencies to the tones used as conditioned stimuli (CSs). These cells typically responded to both the reinforced and nonreinforced CSs. Forty-one percent of the cells responded on conditioned response (CR) trials but not on trials without CRs. The average lead of the neural response to the CR was 71.4 ms. Cells that responded on CR trials were more likely to respond to the CSs, or to the CSs and the US, than cells that did not respond on CR trials. For about half of the cells that responded on CR trials the latency of response followed trial-by-trial variations of CR latency. For the remainder, the response was time-locked to CS-onset. Cells whose responses paralleled the CR may be involved in the initiation or modulation of the CR, while those whose responses were time-locked to the CS may be involved in sensory processing underlying the initiation of the movement. The pathways that may underlie the US- and CS-elicited responses are also discussed.  相似文献   

19.
The discharges of 22 single neurons were recorded in the secondary auditory cortical field (AII) during acquisition of the pupillary dilation conditioned defensive response in chronically prepared cats. All 22 neurons developed discharge plasticity in background activity, and 21/22 cells developed plasticity in their responses to the acoustic conditioned stimulus (CS). Nonassociative factors were ruled out by the use of a sensitization phase (CS and US [unconditioned stimulus] unpaired) preceding the conditioning phase and by ensuring stimulus constancy at the periphery by neuromuscular paralysis. Changes in background neuronal activity were related to measures of behavioral learning or to changes in the level of arousal. Specifically, decreases in background activity (17/22 cells) developed at the time that subjects began to display conditioned responses. Increases in background activity (5/22) developed in animals that became more tonically aroused during conditioning. However, both increases (11/22) and decreases (10/22) in evoked activity developed independently of the rate of pupillary learning, tonic arousal level, or changes in background activity. These findings indicate that changes in background activity are closely related to behavioral processes of learning and arousal whereas stimulus-evoked discharge plasticity develops solely as a consequence of stimulus pairing. A comparative analysis of the effects of conditioning on secondary and primary (AI) auditory cortex indicates that both regions develop neuronal discharge plasticity early in the conditioning phase and that increases in background activity in primary auditory cortex are also associated with elevated levels of tonic arousal. In addition, the overall incidence of single neurons developing learning-related discharge plasticity is significantly greater in AII than in AI. The relevance of these findings is discussed in terms of parallel processing in sensory systems and multiple sensory cortical fields.  相似文献   

20.
Previous results have suggested that electrodermal responses classically conditioned to potentially phobic CSs (e.g., pictures of snakes or spiders) are highly resistant to extinction and occur largely independently of cognitive expectancies. In order to test stringently for these possibilities, 144 college student subjects were administered differential classical conditioning acquisition and extinction paradigms while expectancies of the shock UCS were closely monitored. Half the subjects had potentially phobic CSs, whereas the other half had neutral CSs. Regardless of type of CS, during acquisition no evidence of electrodermal conditioning was found among subjects unaware of the CS?UCS contingency, nor was conditioning found on the pre-aware trials of subjects who became aware. During extinction, there was significantly greater resistance to extinction of electrodermal responses conditioned to potentially phobic CSs as well as a similar trend with expectancies of the UCS. However, when expectancies were equated, there was no greater resistance to extinction of electrodermal responses conditioned to potentially phobic CSs. Thus, while electrodermal responses conditioned to potentially phobic CSs did exhibit greater resistance to extinction, this conditioning was no more independent of expectancies than is conditioning with neutral CSs.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号