首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Background: Anagram and Copy Treatment (ACT) and Copy and Recall Treatment (CART) have been shown to improve written communication for those with severe aphasia (Beeson, 1999 Beeson, P. M. 1999. Treating acquired writing impairment: Strengthening graphemic representations. Aphasiology, 13: 767785. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Beeson, Hirsch, & Rewega, 2002 Beeson, P. M., Hirsch, F. M. and Rewega, M. A. 2002. Successful single-word writing treatment: Experimental analysis of four cases. Aphasiology, 16: 473491. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Beeson, Rising, & Volk, 2003 Beeson, P. M., Rising, K. and Volk, J. 2003. Writing treatment for severe aphasia: Who benefits?. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46: 10381060. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). More recently, the addition of a spoken repetition component to the CART programme has been suggested to enhance oral naming in moderate aphasia (Beeson & Egnor, 2006 Beeson, P. M. and Egnor, H. 2006. Combining treatment for written and spoken naming. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12: 816827. [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Wright, Marshall, Wilson, & Page, 2008 Wright, H. H., Marshall, R. C., Wilson, K. B. and Page, J. L. 2008. Using a written cueing hierarchy to improve verbal naming in aphasia. Aphasiology, 22: 522536. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]) and in cases with co-existing apraxia of speech (AOS) (de Riesthal, 2007 de Riesthal, M. Changes in written and spoken naming with a modified CART programme. Paper presented at the American Speech and Hearing Association. Boston, MA [Google Scholar]). No studies have investigated the use of a modified ACT and CART with spoken repetition in individuals with severe aphasia and AOS.

Aims: The purpose of the study was to examine ACT and CART modified with spoken naming repetition, using visual and auditory stimuli in the ACT sessions and home practice videos in the CART sessions, for individuals with severe aphasia.

Methods & Procedures: Three individuals, RC, AC, and MJ, with severe aphasia and coexisting AOS post left middle cerebral artery strokes participated in a 3-month programme. Participants were enrolled in modified ACT and CART with spoken repetition of the target word. For the CART programme a video was created for each word in a treatment set to facilitate repetition in the home practice programme. Probes of spoken and written performance were obtained at the onset of each session, and during baseline, treatment, and follow-up maintenance

Outcomes & Results: All participants improved in their ability to write the treatment stimuli. A 5-point scoring system (Helm-Estabrooks & Albert, 2003 Helm-Estabrooks, N. and Albert, M. L. 2003. Manual of Aphasia and Aphasia Therapy, Austin, TX: Pro-.  [Google Scholar]) was a more sensitive tool than the traditional binary scoring. None of the participants improved in the spoken naming condition with task stimuli. Mild improvement was noted in comparing pre- and post-test naming for MJ.

Conclusions: Our study supports evidence that ACT and CART may improve written naming skills in persons with severe aphasia. The inclusion of spoken repetition in the home practice CART programme may not be appropriate for cases with severe aphasia with AOS. However, further research using the technique with moderate aphasia with AOS may reveal that the technique of practice with auditory and visual stimuli is beneficial. The ease of using digital video tools with computer or augmentative devices has exciting and practical clinical application.  相似文献   

2.
3.
Prism Adaptation Therapy (PAT) is an intervention method in the treatment of the attention disorder neglect (Frassinetti, Angeli, Meneghello, Avanzi, & Ladavas, 2002 Frassinetti, F., Angeli, V., Meneghello, F., Avanzi, S. and Ladavas, E. 2002. Long-lasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. Brain, 125: 608623. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Rossetti et al., 1998 Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., Pisella, L., Farne, A., Li, L.Boisson, D. 1998. Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature, 395(6698): 166169. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). The aim of this study was to investigate whether one session of PAT using a computer-attached touchscreen would produce similar after-effects to the conventional box normally used in PAT.

In four experiments, 81 healthy subjects and 7 brain-injured patients diagnosed with neglect were subjected to a single session of PAT under two conditions: (1) using the original box, and (2) using a computer-based implementation of PAT. The session of PAT included a pre-exposure step involving pointing at 30 targets without feedback; an exposure step involving pointing at 90 targets with prism goggles and feedback; and a post-exposure step involving pointing at 60 targets, with no goggles and no feedback.

The results indicate that the expected similarity in the after-effect produced by the two conditions seems to occur only if subjects receive feedback on pointing precision by seeing their fingertip during the exposure step. Attempts to provide feedback indirectly via icons on the computer screen failed to produce the expected size in the after-effect. The findings have direct implications for computer-based treatment of visuospatial disorders in the future and computer-assisted rehabilitation in general.  相似文献   

4.
Background: Semantic feature analysis (SFA) is a treatment for lexical retrieval impairment in which participants are cued to provide semantic information about concepts they have difficulty naming, in an effort to facilitate accurate lexical retrieval (Boyle, 2004a Boyle, M. 2004a. Discourse treatment for word retrieval impairment in chronic aphasia, Park City, UT: Presented at the Clinical Aphasiology Conference.  [Google Scholar]). Previous work has provided preliminary evidence that persons with aphasia who participated in SFA-focused group aphasia treatment demonstrate improved lexical retrieval in discourse, with additional improvements observed in either general communication informativeness or efficiency (Antonucci, 2009 Antonucci, S. M. 2009. Use of semantic feature analysis in group aphasia treatment. Aphasiology, 23(7–8): 854866. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). Furthermore, results suggested that individuals with differing mechanisms of anomia could derive benefits from participation in SFA-focused group treatment.

Aims: The aim of the current study was to investigate further training of SFA in connected speech during group aphasia treatment. This study expanded and extended previous work (Antonucci, 2009 Antonucci, S. M. 2009. Use of semantic feature analysis in group aphasia treatment. Aphasiology, 23(7–8): 854866. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]), through the addition of participants with more varied aetiologies and severities of aphasia, and through the introduction of home practice. It was hypothesised that lexical retrieval during discourse would improve, as would overall communicative informativeness and/or efficiency.

Methods & Procedures: Four individuals with aphasia participated in biweekly group treatment during which SFA was trained through connected speech tasks. Three participants presented with stroke aphasia, while one demonstrated aphasia consequent to traumatic brain injury. Discourse measures included those for overall communicative informativeness and efficiency (Nicholas & Brookshire, 1993 Nicholas, L. E. and Brookshire, R. H. 1993. A system for quantifying the informativeness and efficiency of the connected speech of adults with aphasia. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 36(2): 338350. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]) and for word-class-specific lexical retrieval (Mayer & Murray, 2003 Mayer, J. F. and Murray, L. L. 2003. Functional measures of naming in aphasia: Word retrieval in confrontation naming versus connected speech. Aphasiology, 17(5): 481497. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). Effect sizes were calculated for all discourse measures. Pre- and post-treatment performance on the spontaneous speech portion of the Western Aphasia Battery-Revised was also analysed relative to discourse measures, to corroborate findings from more frequently repeated probes.

Outcomes & Results: All four participants demonstrated improvement to communicative informativeness and/or efficiency in connected speech tasks.

Conclusions: Results provide additional support for the hypothesis that SFA administered during group aphasia treatment can be used successfully to facilitate improvement of communicative effectiveness. These results also support previous findings that individuals with differing aetiologies and natures of word retrieval impairment may benefit from participation in the same SFA-focused group aphasia treatment. Future work proceeding from this study may be directed towards differentiating which aspects of the treatment are most effective across participants with varied naming impairment, what is the optimal group composition and size, and towards discerning the most effective methods for facilitating and monitoring home practice.  相似文献   

5.
Background: A number of hypotheses have been formulated to account for comprehension data in agrammatic aphasia. They explain deficits in comprehending specific structures, such as semantically reversible non-canonical sentences, for example, Trace Deletion Hypothesis (TDH, Grodzinsky, 1986 Grodzinsky, Y. 1986. Language deficit and the theory of syntax. Brain and Language, 27: 135159. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(86)90009-X[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar], 1990 Grodzinsky, Y. 1990. Theoretical perspectives on language deficits, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Crossref] [Google Scholar], 1995 Grodzinsky, Y. 1995. A restrictive theory of agrammatic comprehension. Brain and Language, 51: 2651. doi:10.1006/brln.1995.1039[Crossref], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]), or functional categories, for example, Tree Pruning Hypothesis (TPH, e.g., Friedmann, 2006 Friedmann, N. 2006. Generalizations on variations in comprehension and production: A further source of variation and a possible account. Brain and Language, 96: 151153. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2005.06.002[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]); Tense Underspecification Hypothesis (TUH, Wenzlaff & Clahsen, 2004 Wenzlaff, M. and Clahsen, H. 2004. Tense and agreement in German agrammatism. Brain and Language, 89: 5768. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00298-0[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar], 2005 Wenzlaff, M. and Clahsen, H. 2005. Finiteness and verb-second in German agrammatism. Brain and Language, 92: 3344. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.05.006[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]); Interpretable Features’ Impairment Hypothesis (IFIH; e.g., Varlokosta et al., 2006 Varlokosta, S., Valeonti, N., Kakavoulia, M., Lazaridou, M., Economou, A. and Protopapas, A. 2006. The breakdown of functional categories in Greek aphasia: Evidence from agreement, tense, and aspect. Aphasiology, 20: 723743. doi:10.1080/02687030500513703[Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). Several studies, however, report evidence contradicting these theories (e.g., Caramazza, Capasso, Capitani, & Miceli, 2005 Caramazza, A., Capasso, R., Capitani, E. and Miceli, G. 2005. Patterns of comprehension performance in agrammatic Broca's aphasia: A test of the Trace Deletion Hypothesis. Brain and Language, 94: 4353. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.11.006[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Dickey, Milman, & Thompson, 2008 Dickey, M. W., Milman, L. H. and Thompson, C. K. 2008. Judgment of functional morphology in agrammatic aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21: 3565. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.08.001[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]) and propose new accounts to explain the comprehension deficits in agrammatic aphasia, for example, Distributed Morphology Account (DMA, Dickey et al., 2008 Dickey, M. W., Milman, L. H. and Thompson, C. K. 2008. Judgment of functional morphology in agrammatic aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21: 3565. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.08.001[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]).

Aims: Against the background above, this study investigates the ability of three Greek-speaking agrammatic individuals to comprehend a wide range of structures and functional categories: semantically reversible (canonical) active and (non-canonical) passive sentences, Complementizer Phrase, subject-verb Agreement, Tense, and Aspect.

Methods & Procedures: We administered: (a) two sentence-picture matching tasks to test comprehension of reversible active and passive sentences, and Tense; (b) a sentence grammaticality judgement task to test judgement of Tense, Aspect, and subject-verb Agreement; and (c) a picture-pointing task to test comprehension of Complementizer Phrase.

Outcomes & Results: (a) Two of the three agrammatic participants performed at chance on reversible passive sentences and above chance on active sentences. The third participant performed equally high on the two sentence types. Two participants performed at chance on Tense comprehension and one above chance. (b) The three participants were selectively or across-the-board impaired in judgement of Agreement, Tense, and Aspect. One of the two selectively affected participants had chance performance on Aspect and above chance performance on Agreement and Tense. The other one performed at chance on Aspect and Tense, and above chance on Agreement. The third participant's performance was equally poor on all three categories. (c) All three agrammatic participants performed above chance on the comprehension of Complementizer Phrase.

Conclusions: In comprehension/judgement, canonical and non-canonical sentences do not dissociate in all agrammatic speakers, while functional categories associated with the verb morphology may be compromised in the face of relatively well-preserved categories that are located higher in the syntactic hierarchy. All three agrammatic participants support the DMA, and two of them support the TDH. Instead, none of them provided support to the TPH, TUH, and IFIH.  相似文献   

6.
Background: Little research has been conducted on functional categories in probable Alzheimer's disease (pAD). Furthermore, the findings are contradictory, since some studies report ceiling performance on tense and/or subject–verb agreement (Kaprinis & Stavrakaki, 2007 Kaprinis, S. and Stavrakaki, S. 2007. Morphological and syntactic abilities in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Brain and Language, 103: 5960. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.07.044[Crossref], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Kavé & Levy, 2003 Kavé, G. and Levy, Y. 2003. Sensitivity to gender, person and tense inflection by persons with Alzheimer's disease. Brain and Language, 87: 267277. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00106-8[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]), whereas others report morphosyntactic deficits and agrammatic profiles (e.g., Altmann, Kempler, & Andersen, 2001 Altmann, L. G. P., Kempler, D. and Andersen, E. S. 2001. Speech errors in Alzheimer's disease: Reevaluating morphosyntactic preservation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44: 10691082. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2001/085)[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]).

Aims: This study investigates the ability of Greek-speaking pAD individuals to produce and judge subject–verb agreement, tense, and aspect. Given pAD individuals have working memory limitations (e.g., Baddeley, 1996 Baddeley, A. 1996. Exploring the central executive. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49: 528. doi:10.1080/713755608[Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]), and given the differential processing demands of agreement, tense and aspect (e.g., Fyndanis, Varlokosta, & Tsapkini, 2012a Fyndanis, V., Varlokosta, S. and Tsapkini, K. 2012a. Agrammatic production: Interpretable features and selective impairment in verb inflection. Lingua, 122: 11341147. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2012.05.004[Crossref], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]), pAD participants are expected to perform better on agreement than on tense/aspect. Based on the hypothesis that reference to the past is computationally more demanding than reference to the future/present (e.g., Bastiaanse et al., 2011 Bastiaanse, R., Bamyaci, E., Hsu, C-J., Lee, J., Yarbay Duman, T. and Thompson, C. K. 2011. Time reference in agrammatic aphasia: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24: 652673. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.07.001[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]), a within-tense dissociation is expected to emerge. Further, on the assumption that unmarked values of functional categories are less demanding than marked values (e.g., Lapointe, 1985 Lapointe, S. 1985. A theory of verb form use in the speech of agrammatic aphasics. Brain and Language, 24: 100155. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(85)90100-2[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]), the imperfective (unmarked) aspect is expected to be better preserved than the perfective (marked) aspect.

Methods & Procedures: Ten Greek-speaking mild pAD individuals and six healthy controls participated in a sentence completion task, a grammaticality judgement task, and a sentence–picture matching task. Non-parametric tests were used for analysis of results.

Outcomes & Results: PAD participants were found to be significantly more impaired in aspect compared to tense and agreement, in both production and grammaticality judgement/comprehension. Agreement was found significantly better preserved than tense in production. Similar patterns of performance have been attested in agrammatism (e.g., Fyndanis et al., 2012a Fyndanis, V., Varlokosta, S. and Tsapkini, K. 2012a. Agrammatic production: Interpretable features and selective impairment in verb inflection. Lingua, 122: 11341147. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2012.05.004[Crossref], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). Reference to the past and reference to the future did not dissociate, whereas the imperfective aspect was found to be significantly more impaired than the perfective aspect in production.

Conclusions: PAD participants' better performance on producing agreement, compared to tense/aspect, is accounted for in terms of the differential demands these categories pose on the processing system. Agreement is computationally less demanding than tense/aspect, because the former involves processing of grammatical information only, whereas the latter involve processing and integration of grammatical and extralinguistic/conceptual information. The preponderance of tense over aspect is attributed to the subjectivity of the latter, which renders it either a category “difficult” to test, or a computationally more demanding condition. The results also show that reference to the past is as demanding as reference to the future. The hypothesis that unmarked values are easier than marked ones is not supported by our data.  相似文献   

7.
Background: Production of passive sentences is often impaired in agrammatic aphasia and has been attributed both to an underlying structural impairment (e.g., Schwartz, Saffran, Fink, Myers, & Martin, 1994 Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., Fink, R. B., Myers, J. L. and Martin, N. 1994. Mapping therapy: A treatment program for agrammatism. Aphasiology, 8: 1954. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]) and to a morphological deficit (e.g., Caplan & Hanna, 1998 Caplan, D. and Hanna, J. 1998. Sentence production by aphasic patients in a constrained task. Brain and Language, 63: 184218. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Faroqi-Shah & Thompson, 2003 Faroqi-Shah, Y. and Thompson, C. K. 2003. Effect of lexical cues on the production of active and passive sentences in Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia. Brain and Language, 85: 409426. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). However, the nature of the deficit in passive sentence production is not clear due to methodological issues present in previous studies.

Aims: This study examined active and passive sentence production in nine agrammatic aphasic speakers under conditions of structural priming using eyetracking to test whether structural impairments occur independently of morphological impairments and whether the underlying nature of error types is reflected in on-line measures, i.e., eye movements and speech onset latencies.

Methods & Procedures: Nine participants viewed and listened to a prime sentence in either active or passive voice, and then repeated it aloud. Next, a target picture appeared on the computer monitor and participants were instructed to describe it using the primed sentence structure.

Outcomes & Results: Participants made substantial errors in sentence structure, i.e., passives with role reversals (RRs) and actives-for-passives, but few errors in passive morphology. Longer gaze durations to the first-produced noun for passives with RRs as compared to correct passives were found before and during speech. For actives-for-passives, however, this pattern was found during speech, but not before speech.

Conclusions: The deficit in passive sentence production does not solely arise from a morphological deficit, rather it stems, at least in part, from a structural level impairment. The underlying nature of passives with RRs is qualitatively different from that of actives-for-passives, which cannot be clearly differentiated with off-line testing methodology.  相似文献   

8.
Background: Standardised language tests are the most commonly used assessment tool of aphasia by Speech-Language Pathologists (Katz et al., 2000 Katz, R. C., Hallowell, B., Code, C., Armstrong, E., Roberts, P.Pound, C. 2000. A multinational comparison of aphasia management practices. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35: 303314. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]), yet they are limited in their ability to provide information regarding a person's ability to communicate in daily life. Social validation is a less commonly used type of assessment, but one that may provide additional information regarding language and communication abilities, based on the perceptions of persons not directly involved in the treatment. Despite its potential, there remain questions regarding its optimal use.

Aims: The goals of this study were to examine the usefulness of social validation in measuring communicative change after aphasia treatment, and to investigate the potential influence of rater characteristics on raters' perceptions.

Methods & Procedures: Narratives were elicited before and after naming treatment from 11 participants with aphasia: 7 participants were treated, 4 of whom improved and 3 of whom did not; 4 participants did not receive treatment. Three groups of 10 raters (speech-language pathologists, older and younger naïve adults) evaluated the narratives, and their ratings of the pre- and post-treatment narratives were analysed for perceived improvement.

Outcomes & Results: Results indicated that all three groups of raters observed improvement in the two treated groups of participants with aphasia, but not in the untreated group of participants with aphasia.

Conclusions: Raters' perceptions of narratives (i.e., social validation) provide a useful measure of change after aphasia treatment, which can easily be used to supplement objective language measures. Additionally, factors such as the rater's experience with aphasia or life experience do not appear to have an important influence on rater perceptions.  相似文献   

9.
Deep brain stimulation (DBS) now plays an important role in the treatment of Parkinson's disease, tremor, and dystonia. DBS may also have a role in the treatment of other disorders such as obsessive-compulsive disorder, Tourette's syndrome, and depression. The neuropsychologist plays a crucial role in patient selection, follow-up, and management of intra-operative and post-operative effects (Pillon, 2002 Pillon , B. ( 2002 ). Neuropsychological assessment for management of patients with deep brain stimulation . Movement Disorders , 17 ( Suppl 3 ), S116122 .[Crossref], [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Saint-Cyr & Trepanier, 2000 Trepanier , L. L. , Kumar , R. , Lozano , A. M. , Lang , A. E. , & Saint-Cyr , J. A. ( 2000 ). Neuropsychological outcome of GPi pallidotomy and GPi or STN deep brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease . Brain and Cognition , 42 ( 3 ), 324347 .[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). There is now emerging evidence that DBS can induce mood, cognitive, and behavioral changes. These changes can have dramatic effects on patient outcome. There have been methodological problems with many of the studies of DBS on mood, cognition, and behavior. The neuropsychologist needs to be aware of these issues when following up patients, and constructing future studies. Additionally, this article will review all aspects of the DBS procedure that can result in mood, cognitive, and behavioral effects and what role(s) the neuropsychologist should play in screening and follow-up.  相似文献   

10.
Background: One of the few studies to describe therapy specifically for phonological assembly difficulties in aphasia is a single-case study by Franklin, Buerk, and Howard (2002 Franklin, S., Buerk, F. and Howard, D. 2002. Generalised improvement in speech production for a subject with reproduction conduction aphasia. Aphasiology, 16(10/11): 10871114. [Taylor & Francis Online] [Google Scholar]). Their client made a significant improvement in picture naming for both treated and untreated words after therapy comprising two phases, the first aimed at improving auditory awareness and the second aimed at improving self-monitoring.

Aims: This study aimed to determine whether the treatment programme used by Franklin et al. (2002 Franklin, S., Buerk, F. and Howard, D. 2002. Generalised improvement in speech production for a subject with reproduction conduction aphasia. Aphasiology, 16(10/11): 10871114. [Taylor & Francis Online] [Google Scholar]) is replicable with other people with impaired phonological assembly as the main part of their aphasia, and to explore any differences in the outcomes for participants.

Methods & Procedures: A case series of four participants with aphasia with mixed impairments including phonological assembly difficulties received a replication of the treatment protocol described by Franklin et al. (2002 Franklin, S., Buerk, F. and Howard, D. 2002. Generalised improvement in speech production for a subject with reproduction conduction aphasia. Aphasiology, 16(10/11): 10871114. [Taylor & Francis Online] [Google Scholar]).

Outcomes & Results: None of the participants responded to therapy in the same way as Franklin et al.’s (2002 Franklin, S., Buerk, F. and Howard, D. 2002. Generalised improvement in speech production for a subject with reproduction conduction aphasia. Aphasiology, 16(10/11): 10871114. [Taylor & Francis Online] [Google Scholar]) original client. Three participants improved on naming after the first, auditory discrimination, phase of therapy, but only one participant made further gains in naming following the second, monitoring, phase of therapy, and all improvements seen were for treated items only. One participant did not show any significant improvement on naming of treated or untreated items after either phase of therapy.

Conclusions: Whereas Franklin et al.’s (2002 Franklin, S., Buerk, F. and Howard, D. 2002. Generalised improvement in speech production for a subject with reproduction conduction aphasia. Aphasiology, 16(10/11): 10871114. [Taylor & Francis Online] [Google Scholar]) original client had a relatively pure post-lexical phonological assembly impairment, the three participants in the current study whose speech improved after therapy had a combination of lexical and post-lexical phonological impairments, and it is proposed that their item-specific improvements in picture naming occurred as a result of improved mapping between semantics and the phonological output lexicon. The participant in this study whose speech did not improve following therapy had a combination of phonological assembly difficulties and apraxia of speech. This study demonstrates that the same therapy can work at different levels for different individuals, depending on many factors, including their profile of linguistic and cognitive impairments.  相似文献   

11.
This study was conducted to assess the psychometric properties of 2 assessment instruments, the Behavior Problems Inventory-01 (BPI-01; Rojahn, Matson, Lott, Esbensen, & Smalls, 2001 Rojahn, J., Matson, J. L., Lott, D., Esbensen, A. J. and Smalls, Y. 2001. The Behavior Problems Inventory: An instrument for the assessment of self-injury, stereotyped behavior, and aggressive/destructive behavior in individuals with developmental disabilities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31: 577588. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]) and the Nisonger Child Behavior Rating Form (NCBRF; Aman, Tassé, Rojahn, & Hammer, 1996 Aman, M. G., Tassé, M. J., Rojahn, J. and Hammer, D. 1996. The Nisonger CBRF: A child behavior rating form for children with developmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 17: 4157. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). The sample consisted of 237 ethnically diverse children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities who ranged in age from 4 to 22 years. Reliability parameters included internal consistency, interteacher agreement, teacher-parent agreement, and test-retest reliability. Factorial validity was assessed first by bivariate Spearman rank (ρ) correlations and then by examining the factor structure fit via confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). Convergent and discriminant validity was assessed by multiple regression analyses across the 2 instruments. Reliability coefficients (internal consistency, interteacher agreement, and test-retest reliability) of the BPI-01 and of the NCBRF subscales ranged from fair to excellent and from poor to excellent, respectively. The CFA suggested a poor fit between the present and the original BPI-01 and NCBRF factor structures, although item-total correlations were reasonable. Convergent and discriminant validity between the BPI-01 and the NCBRF, however, was strong. Limitations of the study are discussed and recommendations for future studies are presented.  相似文献   

12.
The purpose of the present study was to examine depressive symptoms as a function of eating disorder (ED) diagnostic status and gender. A demographic questionnaire, the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer & Brown, 1996 Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A. and Brown, G. K. 1996. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd, San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.  [Google Scholar]), and the Questionnaire for Eating Disorder Diagnoses (Q-EDD; Mintz, O'Halloran, Mulholland & Schneider, 1997 Mintz, L. B., O'Halloran, M. S, Mulholland, A. M. and Schneider, P. A. 1997. Questionnaire for Eating Disorder Diagnoses: Reliability and validity of operationalizing DSM–IV criteria into a self-report format: Correction. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44: 6379. [Crossref], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]) were administered to a nonclinical sample of college-aged men (n = 115) and women (n = 136). Contrast tests revealed significant differences in mean BDI-II scores as a function of Q-EDD diagnostic status. This effect did not differ as a function of gender and the pattern was the same for both cognitive and somatic-affective depressive symptoms. The nature of the comorbid relationship between ED and depressive symptoms appears markedly similar across both genders.  相似文献   

13.
Assistive technologies for cognition (ATC) provide an effective means to compensate for prospective memory failures among adults with acquired brain injury (ABI; de Joode, van Heugten, Verhey, & van Boxtel, 2010 de Joode, E., van Heugten, C., Verhey, F. and van Boxtel, M. 2010. Efficacy and usability of assistive technology for patients with cognitive deficits: A systematic review. Clinical Rehabilitation, 24: 701714. doi:10.1177/0269215510367551[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Sohlberg et al., 2007 Sohlberg, M. M., Kennedy, M., Avery, J., Coelho, C., Turkstra, L., Ylvisaker, M. and Yorkston, K. 2007. Evidence-based practice for the use of external aids as a memory compensation technique. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 15(1): 1551.  [Google Scholar]). This study evaluated a novel ATC device, the Television Assisted Prompting (TAP) system, which provides audiovisual reminders at scheduled prospective times on a person's home television. A randomised, controlled crossover design evaluated task completion for two preferred, two non-preferred, and two structured experimental tasks among 23 adults with ABI between two conditions: TAP prompting or typical (TYP) practice, without TAP reminders. Main outcomes showed a significant advantage of prospective memory prompting (72% completion) over no prompting (43% completion) and higher task completion with TAP prompting for researcher-assigned experimental tasks (81%) compared to self-selected preferred (68%) or non-preferred (68%) tasks. Results are discussed in the context of ATC efficacy to support prospective memory prompting following ABI, with contributions and future directions for continued investigation of customisation of prompts to maximise task completion.  相似文献   

14.
Recently, Kaiser (2003) Kaiser, J. 2003. Manganese: A high-octane dispute. Science, 300: 926928.  [Google Scholar] raised concerns over the increase in brain damage claims reportedly due to exposure to welding fumes. In the present article, we discuss methodological problems in conducting neuropsychological research on the effects of welding exposure, using a recent paper by Bowler et al. (2003) Bowler, R. M., Gysens, S., Diamond, E., Booty, A., Hartney, C. and Roels, H. A. 2003. Neuropsychological sequelae of exposure to welding fumes in a group of occupationally exposed men. International Journal of Hygiene & Environmental Health, 206: 517529. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar] as an example to illustrate problems common in the neurotoxicity literature. Our analysis highlights difficulties in conducting such quasi-experimental investigations, including subject selection bias, litigation effects on symptom report and neuropsychological test performance, response bias, and scientifically inadequate casual reasoning.  相似文献   

15.
A manual form of the Letter Memory Test (LMT: Orey, Cragar, & Berry, 2000 Orey , S. A. , Cragar , D. E. , & Berry , D. T. R. ( 2000 ). The effects of two motivational manipulations on the neuropsychological performance of mildly head-injured college students . Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology , 15 , 335348 .[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]) was compared in neuropsychological evaluees classified as honest (HON: n = 39) or probable cognitive feigners (PCF: n = 10) using results from two well-validated motivational tests. With the exception of lower educational level and higher rate of compensation seeking in the PCF, the groups were equivalent on most important demographic and injury severity parameters. PCF participants scored significantly lower on most neuropsychological tests (median Cohen's d = 1.2), as well as on the manual LMT (Cohen's d = 4.2). Operating characteristics of the manual LMT in the present sample were comparable to those reported in a similar study using the computerized version of the LMT in neuropsychiatric patients (Vagnini et al., 2006 Vagnini , V. L. , Sollman , M. J. , Berry , D. T. R. , Granacher , R. P. , Clark , J. A. , et al. . ( 2006 ). Known-groups cross-validation of the Letter Memory Test in a sample of compensation-seeking mixed neurologic sample . The Clinical Neuropsychologist , 20 , 289304 .[Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]).  相似文献   

16.
Background: Awareness and knowledge of aphasia is limited among the public and in the healthcare sector despite considerable efforts being made internationally to address this. Advances in medical interventions have improved life expectancy following stroke so it is assumed that there are more people living with aphasia now than at any time in the past. Given this increase and the attempts to provide information in an accessible and timely manner we predicted that awareness and knowledge of aphasia would be improved compared to when it was last examined 10 years ago.

Aims: This study sought to investigate whether public awareness of aphasia has improved since the original international study by Simmons-Mackie et al. in 2002 Simmons-Mackie, N., Code, C., Armstrong, E., Stiegler, L. and Elman, R. J. 2002. What is aphasia? Results of an international survey. Aphasiology, 16: 837848. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]. A second aim was to compare health professionals' awareness and knowledge of aphasia specifically with that of the general public.

Methods & Procedures: A brief face-to-face survey was administered to ascertain awareness and knowledge of Parkinson's disease (PD), stroke, and aphasia, as well as the causes and symptoms of aphasia. A total of 300 people were surveyed (200 were members of the general public and a further 100 were health sector workers). Information was collected on age (ranging from 21–85 years), sex (125 males and 175 females), employment status, and ethnicity. The data were analysed to determine the number of individuals who had awareness and knowledge of the three conditions. A binary logistic regression was calculated to predict the factors most likely to influence awareness of aphasia.

Outcomes & Results: Aphasia awareness was 30% for the whole sample and aphasia knowledge was 8%. When we separate out the general public from those in the health sector the results are quite different: 11% of the general public had awareness of aphasia and only 1.5% had knowledge, whereas 68% of those in the health sector had awareness and 21% met the criteria for basic knowledge of aphasia. When compared to knowledge and awareness of stroke and PD, these figures are very poor.

Conclusions: Ten years after the first surveys, awareness and knowledge of aphasia remain disappointingly low. We suggest that, whatever the efforts to date, we need to substantially increase them to improve public and health sector awareness and knowledge of aphasia. Future initiatives must be directed towards providing adequate knowledge in order to increase the health outcomes for people with aphasia and their families.  相似文献   

17.
Background: In addition to the canonical subject-verb-object (SVO) word order, German also allows for non-canonical order (OVS), and the case-marking system supports thematic role interpretation. Previous eye-tracking studies (Kamide et al., 2003 Kamide, Y., Scheepers, C. and Altmann, G. M. 2003. Integration of syntactic and semantic information in predictive processing: Cross-linguistic evidence from German and English. Journal of Psycholinguistics Research, 32: 3754. doi: 10.1023/A:1021933015362 [Google Scholar]; Knoeferle, 2007 Knoeferle, P. 2007. “Comparing the time-course of processing initially ambiguous and unambiguous German SVO/OVS sentences in depicted events”. In Eye movement research. A window on mind and brain, Edited by: van Gompel, R., Fischer, M., Murray, W. and Hill, R. 517531. Oxford, , UK/Amsterdam: Elsevier.  [Google Scholar]) have shown that unambiguous case information in non-canonical sentences is processed incrementally. For individuals with agrammatic aphasia, comprehension of non-canonical sentences is at chance level (Burchert et al., 2003 Burchert, F., De Bleser, R. and Sonntag, K. 2003. Does morphology make the difference? Agrammatic sentence comprehension in German. Brain and Language, 87: 323342. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00132-9[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). The trace deletion hypothesis (Grodzinsky 1995 Grodzinsky, Y. 1995. A restrictive theory of agrammatic comprehension. Brain and Language, 50: 2751. doi: 10.1006/brln.1995.1039[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar], 2000 Grodzinsky, Y. 2000. The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca's area. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23: 171. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00002399[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]) claims that this is due to structural impairments in syntactic representations, which force the individual with aphasia (IWA) to apply a guessing strategy. However, recent studies investigating online sentence processing in aphasia (Caplan et al., 2007 Caplan, D., Waters, G., DeDe, G., Michaud, J. and Reddy, A. 2007. A study of syntactic processing in aphasia I: Behavioral (psycholinguistic) aspects. Brain and Language, 101: 103150. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.06.225[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Dickey et al., 2007 Dickey, M. W., Choy, J. J. and Thompson, C. K. 2007. Real-time comprehension of Wh-movement in aphasia: Evidence from eyetracking while listening. Brain and Language, 100: 122. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.06.004[Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]) found that divergences exist in IWAs' sentence-processing routines depending on whether they comprehended non-canonical sentences correctly or not, pointing rather to a processing deficit explanation.

Aims: The aim of the current study was to investigate agrammatic IWAs' online and offline sentence comprehension simultaneously in order to reveal what online sentence-processing strategies they rely on and how these differ from controls' processing routines. We further asked whether IWAs' offline chance performance for non-canonical sentences does indeed result from guessing.

Methods & Procedures: We used the visual-world paradigm and measured eye movements (as an index of online sentence processing) of controls (N?=?8) and individuals with aphasia (N?=?7) during a sentence–picture matching task. Additional offline measures were accuracy and reaction times.

Outcomes & Results: While the offline accuracy results corresponded to the pattern predicted by the TDH, IWAs' eye movements revealed systematic differences depending on the response accuracy.

Conclusions: These findings constitute evidence against attributing IWAs' chance performance for non-canonical structures to mere guessing. Instead, our results support processing deficit explanations and characterise the agrammatic parser as deterministic and inefficient: it is slowed down, affected by intermittent deficiencies in performing syntactic operations, and fails to compute reanalysis even when one is detected.  相似文献   

18.
Background: Working memory (WM) has gained recent attention as a cognitive construct that may account for language comprehension deficits in persons with aphasia (PWA) (Caspari, Parkinson, LaPointe, & Katz, 1998 Caspari, I., Parkinson, S. R., LaPointe, L. L. and Katz, R. C. 1998. Working memory and aphasia.. Brain and Cognition, 37: 205223. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Martin, Kohen, & Kalinyak‐Fliszar, 2008 Martin, N., Kohen, F. and Kalinyak‐Fliszar, M. 2008. A diagnostic battery to assess language and short‐term memory deficits in aphasia Poster presentation at Clinical Aphasiology Conference, Teton Village, WY [Google Scholar]; Wright, Downey, Gravier, Love, & Shapiro, 2007 Wright, H. H., Downey, R. A., Gravier, M., Love, T. and Shapiro, L. P. 2007. Processing distinct linguistic information types in working memory in aphasia.. Aphasiology, 21: 802813. [Taylor & Francis Online], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). However, few studies have investigated individual differences in performance on sentence comprehension tasks as a function of WM capacity in PWA when WM demands are manipulated.

Aims: The purposes of the current study were: (1) to examine the relationships among verbal WM, sentence comprehension, and severity of impairment in PWA and (2) to investigate the differential performance of high versus low verbal WM groups on sentence comprehension tasks in which task demands were manipulated by the length of the sentence stimuli, complexity of syntactic structure, and by presentation method which varied the time over which the linguistic material was available for computation.

Methods & Procedures: A total of 20 PWA were divided into high and low WM groups based on a listening version of a WM sentence span task. Each participant completed a listening version (CRTT) and three reading versions (CRTT‐R) of the Computerised Revised Token Test as the sentence comprehension tasks.

Outcomes & Results: The WM task significantly predicted performance on the CRTT conditions in which information was only temporarily available, thereby imposing greater WM demands on sentence comprehension. The verbal WM task was significantly correlated with aphasia severity and a principal components analysis revealed that the WM task, overall aphasia severity, and overall reading impairment level loaded on a single factor with 76% of shared variance. The low WM group's performance was significantly lower than the high WM group on the CRTT subtests with syntactically more complex structures and on the CRTT conditions with temporally restricted presentation methods.

Conclusions: This verbal WM task was significantly and moderately correlated with the overall severity of aphasia as well as with both listening and reading sentence comprehension. The WM group differences emerged only in sentence comprehension tasks with greater WM demands. These results are consistent with the notion that WM effects are most evident when WM capacity is sufficiently taxed by the task demands (e.g., Caplan & Waters, 1999 Caplan, D. and Waters, G. S. 1999. Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension.. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22: 77126. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]; Just & Carpenter, 1992 Just, M. A. and Carpenter, P. A. 1992. A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory.. Psychological Review, 99: 122149. [Crossref], [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]).  相似文献   

19.
Background: Augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) has primarily been utilised for motor speech deficits or as an aid for communicating basic needs in the acute stages of aphasia rehabilitation. However, AAC strategies can be employed for individuals with aphasia across a wide range of severity levels and at different stages of recovery. For individuals with chronic impairments in language, voice recognition software can further enhance communication by providing an alternative means for written expression.

Aims: To investigate the functional and linguistic effects of a treatment program that used voice recognition software to improve written communication in an individual with chronic aphasia.

Methods & Procedures: A 65-year-old woman with conduction aphasia participated in a prospective case study. During baseline testing the individual was evaluated on measures of language (i.e., Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, 3rd edition, Goodglass, Kaplan, & Baressi, 2001 Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E. and Barresi, B. 2001. Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, 3rd, Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.  [Google Scholar]; Boston Naming Test, Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 2001 Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H. and Weintraub, S. 2001. Boston Naming Test, 2nd, Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.  [Google Scholar]), computer skills, and functional communication (i.e., ASHA FACs, Frattali et al., 1995 Frattali, C., Thompson, C. K., Holland, A. L., Wohl, C. B. and Ferketic, M. K. 1995. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Functional Assessment of Communication Skills for Adults, Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.  [Google Scholar]; Quality of Communication Life Scales, Paul et al., 2004 Paul, D., Frattali, C., Holland, A., Thompson, C., Caperton, C. and Slater, S. 2004. Quality of Communication Life Scale (ASHA QCL), Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.  [Google Scholar]). A cursory examination of the oral peripheral structure was administered. Treatment consisted of 10 one-hour sessions of training on the Dragon NaturallySpeaking© Program. Following treatment, measures of language, computer skills, quality of life, and functional communication were re-administered. Further, to evaluate if skills in written expression could be utilised to communicate via e-mail, a distance-learning program was implemented.

Outcomes & Results: Findings revealed that, with intensive instruction, the participant was able to independently access the computer and the Dragon NaturallySpeaking© Program. At the conclusion of the treatment phase, functional writing abilities approximated spoken communication. Some minor changes in reading, repetition, and conversation were suggested on formal and informal assessment measures. The participant made substantial gains in using the program in her activities of daily living, but only achieved minor success in demonstrating her skills during distance learning.

Conclusions: The potential benefits of AAC devices may change throughout the course of recovery from aphasia. New technologies may facilitate gains in communication in individuals with aphasia throughout their lifespan. This study demonstrated that intensive training in the use of voice recognition software can enhance functional writing in an individual with chronic aphasia. Although marked progress in written expression was achieved, transfer of skills for use on the Internet was limited. Clinical management should include assessment of various assistive technologies across different modalities of communication for people at different stages of recovery from aphasia.  相似文献   

20.
The equivalence of forms CFL and PRW of the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT; Benton, de Hamsher, & Sivan; 1994 Benton , A. L. , de Hamsher , S. K. , & Sivan , A. B. ( 1994 ). Multilingual aphasia examination () , 3rd ed. . Iowa City : AJA Associates . [Google Scholar]) was examined in samples of healthy college students. Both novel words and qualitative scores (e.g., clusters, switches) were examined using null hypothesis statistical testing (NHST) and inferential confidence intervals (Tryon, 2001 Tryon , W. W. ( 2001 ). Evaluating statistical difference, equivalence, and indeterminacy using inferential confidence interval: An integrated alternative method of conducting null hypothesis statistical tests . Psychological Methods , 6 , 371386 . [INFOTRIEVE] [CSA] [PubMed], [Web of Science ®] [Google Scholar]). Study 1 used a within-subjects design (n = 66) to examine form equivalence in persons who completed versions CFL and PRW. No mean differences were found for any index, and only one (mean cluster size) differed when examining the standard deviations across forms. Inferential confidence intervals were constructed and Rg (maximum probable difference) values were small and, when compared to prior research, less than the standard error of measure associated with using a single form. In Study 2, we employed a between-subjects design (n = 208) and found that the use of each version generated comparable normative data and correlations among scores. These studies suggest that versions CFL and PRW are best viewed as alternate (rather than parallel) forms at this time; however, they are likely to produce comparable results for most purposes. We discuss the use of inferential confidence intervals to examine form equivalence and directions for future work.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号