首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Summary Interactions between the direct (unconditioned) behavioural effects apomorphine and its conditioned effects after pairing with previously neutral stimuli were studied. Rats were injected once daily for 3–12 times, with apomorphine (2.0 mg/kg or 0.5 mg/kg or 0.07 mg/kg s.c. the dose kept constant in each series), in the presence of defined environmental stimuli (a wire cage in association with an acoustic and an olfactory stimulus) as conditional stimuli. The two larger doses produced stereotyped sniffing, licking, and gnawing, the smallest dose akinesia, ptosis, yawning and penile erections. During the conditioning phase, the drug produced most of the effects with increasing intensity and in the case of the stereotypies, there also was a shift to higher scores of stereotypy, with a reduced latency in onset of the signs. On the test day, 1 day after the last administration of apomorphine, the conditioned rats as well as pseudoconditioned controls were treated with a test dose of apomorphine in the presence of the conditional stimuli. Pseudoconditioned rats had been treated with the same pharmacological schedule of apomorphine and had the same familiarity with the stimuli, but both were kept separate. A test dose of 0.5 mg/kg of apomorphine produced stereotypies with a significantly higher score and shorter latency in onset in conditioned than in pseudoconditioned rats. Rats conditioned with the lowest dose showed a significantly longer total duration and a shorter latency in onset of akinesia and ptosis. In rats conditioned with the largest dose (2.0 mg/kg), administration of the lowest dose on the test day produced no stereotypies; neither the akinesia nor the ptosis were different between conditioned and pseudoconditioned rats, but yawning occurred with a higher frequency and a shorter latency in pseudoconditioned rats. When rats were conditioned with the lowest dose and tested with 0.5 mg/kg, the level of stereotypies was identical in both groups of rats, whereas akinesia and ptosis were not observed. Yawning and penile erections occurred more frequently, but for short periods only, in conditioned rats.The results showed that apomorphine-induced stereotypies, akinesia and ptosis could be conditioned, and the conditioned effects mimicked the unconditioned responses, which depended on the dose. Conditioned and unconditioned signs of an increased dopaminergic neurotransmission, observed after large doses of apomorphine, thus acted in a synergistic way; the same applied to conditioned and unconditioned signs observed after a small dose and were perhaps due to a decreased dopaminergic transmission. In contrast, when conditioned and unconditioned signs acted in a mutually antagonistic way (increased vs. decreased dopaminergic transmission), the unconditioned signs predominated. Send offprint requests to K. Kuschinsky at the above address  相似文献   

2.
We investigated whether pharmacological effects of the dopamine agonist apomorphine can be conditioned by establishing an association of apomorphine administration with exteroceptive cues. Apomorphine was repeatedly administered and subsequently, the rat was put into a test cage and exposed to an acoustic and an olfactory stimulus (conditioned rats). Control animals (pseudoconditioned rats) were treated with the same pharmacological schedule of apomorphine not temporally associated with the stimuli. On the test day, both groups were injected with saline and exposed to the stimuli described.The stereotyped behaviour produced by large doses of apomorphine (0.5 or 2.0 mg/kg SC), namely sniffing, licking and gnawing, could be conditioned in a pronounced way. During the conditioning period, a change in the stereotypies was observed with regard to the time-course (earlier occurrence) and to the character of the stereotypies (from sniffing to licking and gnawing), when 0.5 mg/kg apomorphine was used, but not with the dose of 2.0 mg/kg. The conditioned responses showed a relatively uniform distribution during the observation period with some increase towards the end of the observation period. Some signs produced by a low dose of apomorphine (0.07 mg/kg SC), namely hypomotility and ptosis, but not yawning, could also be conditioned, although in a less pronounced way. An intermediate dose of apomorphine (0.18 mg/kg SC) produced both signs observed after large doses and those observed after a small dose, occurring alternatingly. Both types of signs could be conditioned using this dosage. Conditioning did not alter striatal or mesolimbic dopamine turnover.These results suggest that only behavioural signs due to an activation of postsynaptic dopamine receptors, but also some symptoms produced by an activation of dopamine autoreceptors can be conditioned.  相似文献   

3.
Summary In previous studies it was established that stereo-typies (sniffing, licking, gnawing) produced by apomorphine can be conditioned and after repeated pairings with defined conditioned stimuli (auditory, tactile + olfactory) these stereotypies can be observed in the presence of the conditioned stimuli alone. In the present experiments, the extinction of these conditioned stereotypies was studied in one series; in another series, the possible inhibition of conditioned stereotypies by the blocker of dopamine receptors, haloperidol, was measured. The rats were conditioned (or the controls pseudoconditioned, respectively) for either 3 or 10 days with 2.0 mg/kg s. c. apomorphine or 6 days with 0.5 mg/kg s. c. of the drug and by placing them into particular cages in the presence of an auditory and an olfactory stimulus. Under all these conditions, episodes of conditioned stereotypies were observed, when solvent + conditioned stimuli instead of apomorphine was applied 1 day after the last conditioning session (first session of extinction). The conditioned responses seemed to be on the highest level after conditioning with 2.0 mg/kg apomorphine 3 days, lower after conditioning with the same dose on 10 days, and even lower after conditioning for 6 days with 0.5 mg/kg. Under all these conditions, the stereotypies summed up and averaged for the total observation period of 60 min rapidly decreased during the extinction period, so that on day 4 of the extinction period, no further significant differences between conditioned and pseudoconditioned animals were observed, although a short initial period was still observed on the fourth day. On day 3 of extinction, not only an early, but also a late episode of conditioned stereotypies was manifest, interrupted by an almost silent period. The acute (unconditioned) stereotypies produced by 0.5 mg/kg s. c. apomorphine were almost completely suppressed by pre-treatment with 0.1 mg/kg i. p. haloperidol. In contrast, the same dose of haloperidol produced a much less pronounced inhibition of conditioned stereotypies after conditioning with the same dose of apomorphine for 6 times. These results, together with previous findings, suggest that the conditioned behavioural effects are not due to an activation of dopaminergic mechanisms during conditioning with apomorphine. Send offprint requests to K. Kuschinsky at the above address  相似文献   

4.
Summary Nicotine produces behavioural signs which are, in part, characteristic of dopaminergic activation. In the present study, it was investigated, to which degree these signs can be conditioned. The drug produced dose-dependent (0.15–0.60 mg/kg s.c.) increases in locomotor activity, hyperkinesia and stereotyped sniffing. The effects produced by 0.6 mg/kg nicotine were significantly inhibited by mecamylamine (1 mg/kg i. p.), but only in part by haloperidol (0.2 mg/kg i. p.). In a subsequent series, the administration of nicotine (0.6 mg/kg s.c.) was repeatedly associated with well-defined environmental (conditioned) stimuli: a wire cage associated with an auditory and an olfactory stimulus. Another group was pseudoconditioned, a third group remained drug-naive. When the animals were given saline in presence of the conditioned stimuli 24 h after the last conditioning session, locomotor activity, hyperkinesia and stereotyped sniffing were significantly higher in conditioned than in pseudoconditioned and drug-naive rats. Similarly, when the rats were injected with nicotine (0.6 mg/kg s. c.) in presence of the conditioned stimuli 24 h after the last conditioning session, locomotor activity and stereotyped sniffing were most pronounced in the conditioned animals. These results demonstrated that behavioural effects of nicotine can be conditioned. Phenomena of this kind might contribute to the addictive behaviour to nicotine. Send offprint requests to K. Kuschinsky at the above address  相似文献   

5.
Conditioning of behavioural effects produced by two drugs acting differently upon dopaminergic neurotransmission was studied. Nomifensine and the putative dopamine autoreceptor agonist B-HT 920 produce contrasting effects on motility, namely increases in locomotor activity and stereotypies as compared to hypokinesia and ptosis. The administration of each of these drugs (US) was repeatedly associated with well-defined environmental stimuli (CS): a wire cage associated with an auditory and on olfactory stimulus. The rats were conditioned for 7 days with 20 mg/kg nomifensine IP each day. After conditioning, the rats were treated with the solvent alone in presence of the CS. Not only did sniffing and licking occur, but also gnawing, even though the latter response was not evident after acute administration of the drug or during the conditioning period. Nomifensine (20 mg/kg IP) also acutely decreased the ratio of 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid/dopamine concentrations (DOPAC/DOPAMINE); this ratio was not altered in the conditioned rats, 60 min after solvent administration in presence of the CS. Rats were conditioned with 0.02 mg/kg IP B-HT 920 daily for 8 days. During the conditioning phase, akinesia and ptosis showed a slight enhancement and a faster onset. After conditioning, when the rats were treated with the solvent alone, the majority of them showed akinesia and/or ptosis during the observation period, in contrast to pseudoconditioned controls. When these rats were conditioned or pseudoconditioned, respectively, with B-HT 920 for further 5 days using 0.02 mg/kg again, treatment with the same dose in presence of the CS produced a significant enhancement and acceleration of these signs in conditioned as compared with pseudoconditioned control rats. The results show that stereotypies producd by nomifensine and akinesia and ptosis produced by B-HT 920 can be conditioned and that, in addition, a sign of stereotypies which was not manifest during the conditioning period appeared as conditioned response.  相似文献   

6.
The effect of classical conditioning with d-amphetamine on the extracellular concentrations of dopamine (DA), 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) and homovanillic acid (HVA) in the striatum of awake, freely moving rats was studied using microdialysis. This was done in order to test, whether there occurred alterations in DA release as conditioned responses in the striatum. The first series of experiments studied the acute effects of d-amphetamine on the concentration of DA and its metabolites DOPAC and HVA. d-Amphetamine (2 mg/kg, s.c.) increased extracellular DA and decreased DOPAC and HVA. Behaviorally, it led to stereotyped locomotor activation and sniffing. In principle, these observations confirmed earlier findings. In a second series, conditioned responses to d-amphetamine were studied. Rats were implanted with guide cannulas prior conditioning experiments started. For conditioning experiments, the rats were divided into three groups: conditioned group, pseudoconditioned group and a drug-naive control group. After 7 daily training sessions with d-amphetamine (2 mg/kg), on the 8th day, the test day, rats were injected with saline and exposed to the conditional stimuli, while they were observed for their stereotyped, conditioned response. Additionally, microdialysis was performed in order to observe possible changes in the extracellular transmitter or metabolite concentrations. Conditioning with amphetamine led to conditioned stereotypic behavior. In comparison with the pseudoconditioned rats, there was an increase in DA release as conditioned response to amphetamine. In pseudoconditioned rats. DOPAC and HVA were slightly higher than in both other groups. DOPAC and HVA were lower in rats conditioned to d-amphetamine when compared with the pseudoconditioned ones. The results suggest that with regard to DA release, the conditioned responses to d-amphetamine mimicked the acute pharmacological responses. The same is valid for the DA metabolites, although in the opposite direction — they mimicked decreases. Furthermore, the conditioned DA responses to d-amphetamine might contribute to conditioned behavioral responses observed in these experiments. Correspondence to: K. Kuschinsky at the above address  相似文献   

7.
Summary The possible mechanisms of conditioned tolerance to the cataleptogenic effects of haloperidol and droperidol were studied in order to discriminate between classical and conditioned tolerance. Rats were conditioned by repeated administration (19–27 times) of haloperidol (1.5 mg/kg i. p.) or droperidol (1.5 mg/kg i. p.), respectively, in the presence of a sum of defined environmental (auditory, olfactory and tactile) stimuli. The animals were compared with pseudoconditioned rats, which underwent the same number of drug injections and exposures to the environmental stimuli, but neither were associated. In part of the experiments, one further group of rats was repeatedly treated with only solvent in the presence of the environmental stimuli. Rats conditioned with haloperidol or droperidol showed tolerance to the cataleptogenic effect of a test dose of haloperidol (1.5 mg/kg i. p.) or droperidol (1.5 mg/kg i.p.), respectively, when they were tested in presence of the defined conditioning stimuli. The rats conditioned with droperidol showed significantly less catalepsy than the pseudoconditioned animals 30 min after droperidol administration, whereas in rats conditioned with haloperidol, the catalepsy was less pronounced no sooner than 120 min after haloperidol administration. This was a manifestation of conditioned tolerance. In rats pseudoconditioned with droperidol, the catalepsy was similar to that produced by the drug in drug-naive rats, suggesting no classical tolerance due to repeated administration of the neuroleptic drug. The dopamine turnover in striatum or nucleus accumbens after administration of 1.5 mg/kg of haloperidol i.p. was not altered in rats conditioned with haloperidol when compared with pseudoconditioned animals. The stereotypies produced by apomorphine (0.16 mg/kg s. c.) were most pronounced in conditioned, less pronounced in pseudoconditioned and very small in drug-naive rats. These result suggest that the conditioned tolerance to the neuroleptic drugs is connected with an increased sensitivity of the basal ganglia to dopaminergic drugs. In another series of experiments, rats were conditioned with apomorphine 8 times and tested with 0.66 mg/kg of droperidol i.p. Rather unexpectedly, the catalepsy was significantly more pronounced in conditioned than in pseudoconditioned animals. This observation suggests that the type of conditioned effect, which becomes manifest, depends in part on the drug used in the presence of the conditioned stimuli.Send offprint requests to K. Kuschinsky  相似文献   

8.
Summary Previously, in own studies, it was shown that stereotyped behaviour produced by apomorphine can be conditioned if the drug is repeatedly paired with defined environmental stimuli (conditioned stimuli, CS). Eventually, the presentation of CS alone produces stereotyped behaviour as conditioned response (CR). Furthermore, in electrocorticographic recordings it could be demonstrated that the characteristic pattern following acute apomorphine treatment, namely a selective increase in the power of the alpha-1 band, could be conditioned as well.In the present study, regional EEG was recorded in the striatum and in the hippocampus of freely moving rats. For conditioning, apomorphine (0.5 mg/kg s.c.) was paired with auditory and olfactory stimuli as CS for seven times, and on the eighth day the drug was substituted by the solvent in the presence of the CS. The effects were compared with those obtained in pseudoconditioned controls.Acute apomorphine administration led to an increase in power in the alpha-1 band (7.00–9.50 Hz), which effect was obvious in the hippocampus, above the cortex and in the striatum. After performing the conditioning procedure, these effects in regional EEG were found to be conditioned as well: as CR, activation in power in the alpha-1 band in hippocampus and striatum were manifest in the presence of the CS, but in absence of the drug. These effects occurred sporadically, but with a significantly higher frequency than in the pseudoconditioned controls.The results suggest that both the hippocampus and the striatum play important roles in classical conditioning of apomorphine effects which are primarily mediated by the striatum. Correspondence to K. Kuschinsky at the above address  相似文献   

9.
Summary The possible conditioning of pharmacological effects of apomorphine on the electroencephalogram was studied using telemetric recordings in rats. Previous studies have shown that apomorphine-induced stereotyped behaviour can be conditioned: after repeated pairings of defined stimuli with the drug effect, the presentation of the external stimuli alone elicited stereotype sniffing, licking, and gnawing. Since apomorphine, an agonist at dopamine receptors, also produces a characteristic EEG pattern with an increase of power in the alpha-1 band, the possibility that this effect could also be conditioned was studied.In fact, conditioning with a dose of 0.5 mg/kg apomorphine (s. c.) led to a significant increase in the number of short-lasting episodes with enhancement of the power in the alpha-1 range in the presence of the conditioned stimuli, according to a comparison of the results obtained in the conditioned group and those of the controls (pseudoconditioned).Moreover, behavioural studies were performed simultaneously in order to find possible correlations between conditioned effects on EEG and conditioned alterations in behaviour. In general, a fair correlation between the increase of power in the alpha-1 band and stereotyped behaviour was found. This was also the case during extinction, when the conditioned stimuli were repeatedly uncoupled from apomorphine administration: both behavioural parameters and EEG alterations showed similar time-courses and had almost disappeared during the fourth extinction session. Send offprint requests to: K. Kuschinsky at the above address  相似文献   

10.
The dopamine agonist apomorphine elicits protracted pecking when injected systemically (1 mg/kg) into pigeons. In two experiments it was investigated whether apomorphine would function as an unconditioned stimulus in the classical conditioning of pecking in these animals. An experimental design based on a differentiation procedure was used so that possible pseudoconditioning effects were controlled. Two differently coloured test chambers served as negative (CS-) and positive conditioned (CS+) stimuli. During the training phase the subjects experienced the former while injected with saline, and the latter while injected with apomorphine. In later tests not involving any injections the pigeons made significantly more pecks (conditioned response) in the CS+ chamber than in the CS-chamber. In the first and second experiments the conditioned stimuli were, respectively, discrete and diffuse visual cues, but both had similar effects. The conditioning obtained may explain sensitization effects that are observed with repeated apomorphine injections. Apomorphine probably also functions as a positive reinforcer for instrumental conditioning in pigeons.  相似文献   

11.
OBJECTIVES: An unbiased place preference conditioning procedure was used to examine the influence of the non-opioid peptide, dynorphin A 2-17 (DYN 2-17), upon the conditioned and unconditioned effects of opiate withdrawal in the rat. METHODS: Rats were implanted SC with two pellets containing 75 mg morphine or placebo. Single-trial place conditioning sessions with saline and the opioid receptor antagonist naloxone (0.1-1.0 mg/kg; SC) commenced 4 days later. Ten minutes before SC injections, animals received an IV infusion of saline or DYN 2-17 (0.1-5.0 mg/kg). Additional groups of placebo- and morphine-pelleted animals were conditioned with saline and DYN 2-17. During each 30-min conditioning session, somatic signs of withdrawal were quantified. Tests of place conditioning were conducted in pelleted animals 24 h later. RESULTS: Naloxone produced wet-dog shakes, body weight loss, ptosis and diarrhea in morphine-pelleted animals. Morphine-pelleted animals also exhibited significant aversions for an environment previously associated with the administration of naloxone. These effects were not observed in placebo-pelleted animals. DYN 2-17 pretreatment resulted in a dose-related attenuation of somatic withdrawal signs. However, conditioned place aversions were still observed in morphine-pelleted animals that had received DYN 2-17 in combination with naloxone. Furthermore, the magnitude of this effect did not differ from control animals. CONCLUSIONS: These data demonstrate that the administration of DYN 2-17 attenuates the somatic, but not the conditioned aversive effects of antagonist-precipitated withdrawal from morphine in the rat. Differential effects of this peptide in modulating the conditioned and unconditioned effects of opiate withdrawal are suggested.  相似文献   

12.
It was shown previously that behavioural effects of apomorphine (locomotor activation and stereotyped behaviour) can be conditioned when they are associated with well-defined environmental stimuli. In the present study, the hypothesis was tested that glutamatergic mechanisms play an important role either in formation of conditioned responses to apomorphine or in the expression of previously established conditioned responses. For this purpose, two blockers of glutamate receptors were applied, either MK-801 (dizocilpine), a non-competitive, but selective blocker of NMDA-type receptors or MLV-6976, a non-selective blocker of glutamate receptors. MK-801 produced some locomotor activation by itself in a dose-dependent way (0.125–0.50 mg/kg ip). The locomotor activation produced by 0.25 mg/kg could not be conditioned. When rats were conditioned 9 times with 2 mg/kg apomorphine after pretreatment with 0,25 mg/kg of MK-801, this pretreatment did not prevent the development of apomorphine-conditioned locomotor activity or stereotypies which appeared when the rats were treated with saline in presence of the conditioned stimuli. Similar results were obtained when rats were conditioned 7 times with the same dose of apomorphine after pretreatment with 20 mg/kg ip MLV-6976, which drug did not induce any visible alterations in motility by itself. When rats were conditioned 7 times with 2 mg/kg apomorphine alone and tested with MK-801 (0.25 mg/kg) in the presence of the conditioned stimuli, neither locomotor activity nor stereotypies appeared as conditioned responses. When rats were conditioned with the same dose of apomorphine alone and tested with MLV-6976 (20 mg/kg ip), stereotypies did not appear as conditioned responses, but some locomotor activity occurred. The results suggest that glutamatergic mechanisms are not relevant for the development of conditioned responses to apomorphine, but might be of some relevance for the expression of previously established conditioned responses.  相似文献   

13.
Dose-dependent changes in behavioural patterns and in local cerebral glucose utilization (LCGU) following subcutaneous application of apomorphine were measured in conscious, unrestrained rats by means of a scoring system and of the autoradiographic [C14]2-deoxyglucose technique, respectively. The behavioural patterns of akinesia, ptosis, yawning and penile erections were scored. Akinesia and ptosis were most prominent after 0.02 and 0.07 mg/kg apomorphine but not after 0.18 mg/kg. Maximal scores for yawning and penile erections were obtained after 0.07 mg/kg. LCGU was not significantly changed after 0.07 mg/kg except for decreases in the cingulate cortex and hypothalamus. Apomorphine 0.5 mg/kg decreased LCGU in the cingulate, parietal and occipital cortex, anteromedial and lateral thalamus and lateral habenula but increased it in laminae IV and VI of the sensorimotor cortex, in the parafascicular nucleus of the thalamus, and in some parts of the basal ganglia and related nuclei. Similar changes in LCGU occurred after 2.0 mg/kg apomorphine, which also increased LCGU in the ventral tegmental area. The lower dose did not produce changes in LCGU opposite to those occurring after larger doses. The data obtained with LCGU do not support the idea that behavioural effects after low doses of apomorphine are elicited by activation of dopamine autoreceptors.  相似文献   

14.
Our aim is to study the relationship of drug activation of the dopamine neurotransmission system and the conditioning of environmental stimuli present at the time of drug administration. We injected a single dose of apomorphine (0.05 mg/kg SC) in rats with the nigrostriatal dopamine pathways unilaterally denervated with 6-hydroxydopamine, which generates rotational behavior contralateral to the lesioned hemisphere. We observed rotational behavior without apomorphine administration when animals were reexposed at different time intervals to the same environment in which they performed turning behavior. The present findings show that this rotational behavior can be conditioned to environmental stimuli in a strong and long-lasting way. In light of the relationship between opioids and the dopaminergic system, similar conditioning could take place in the learning processes implicated in drug addiction.  相似文献   

15.
This report shows that stimulus generalization occurs in rats conditioned by a single injection of apomorphine. The data suggest that apomorphine initially acts as an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) of an unconditioned response (UCR) that, in turn, produces stimuli which become conditioned stimuli (CS) of a conditioned response (CR) having a nature identical to that of the UCR. The study also shows that behaviour elicited by a subcutaneous injection of apomorphine depends on the part of the body selected for administration. The mentioned properties should be taken into account when apomorphine is used as a tool in studies on brain and behaviour.  相似文献   

16.
The conditioning of body temperature changes using an injection of morphine sulphate as the conditioned stimulus was studied in 30 male Wistar rats. Three groups of animals received daily i.p. injections of either 5, 25, or an increasing dose to 200 mg/kg morphine; a fourth group received saline injections throughout. Rectal temperature was measured in three different environments five times during the day: in a neutral environment, the home cage; in a pre-injection environment, in which animals were placed for a period before the daily injection; and in an injection environment, in which animals remained after the injection. Conditioning trials were followed by a period of abstinence from morphine. Tests for conditioned effects were carried out both during conditioning and after the period of abstinence. During conditioning, animals in the morphine groups, when compared to the saline control animals, showed a conditioned anticipatory hypothermia in the preinjection environment that was opposite in direction to the unconditioned hyperthermia to morphine. In contrast, in the injection environment, animals in the morphine groups showed a conditioned hyperthermia when tested after the period of abstinence. These results suggest a complex interaction between the conditioned and unconditioned temperature responses to morphine.  相似文献   

17.
Dose-dependent effects of 7-OH-DPAT on several behaviors, including place preference, were assessed. Three 2-day conditioning trials were conducted. On 1 day, animals received an injection of one of eight doses of 7-OH-DPAT (0–5 mg/kg) and were placed into a distinct compartment for 40 min. On the other day, animals received an injection of saline and were placed into a different compartment for 40 min. Locomotion, sniffing, and yawning were measured following the first and last injection of 7-OH-DPAT. Place conditioning was assessed on the day following the last trial. 7-OH-DPAT produced a U-shaped dose-dependent change in locomotion and sniffing, and an inverted U-shaped dose-dependent change in yawning. Additionally, repeated administration of 0.1 mg/kg sensitized yawning, whereas 5 mg/kg sensitized locomotion. None of the doses of 7-OH-DPAT produced conditioned place preference, however, there was a trend for conditioned place aversion at 0.03 mg/kg. By contrast, LiCl (127 mg/kg) produced conditioned place aversion and amphetamine (1 mg/kg) produced conditioned place preference using the same conditioning parameters. A subsequent experiment in which the number of animals and conditioning trials were increased demonstrated that the 0.03 mg/kg dose of 7-OH-DPAT produced conditioned place aversion. 7-OH-DPAT has a higher affinity for D3 receptors relative to D2 receptors. Therefore, it is suggested that intermediate doses (0.01–0.1 mg/kg) that increase yawning, and decrease locomotion and sniffing, may preferentially occupy D3 receptors. Furthermore, the results suggest that these putative D3-preferring doses have weak aversive effects.  相似文献   

18.
Apomorphine (50 μg/kg) induced repeated episodes of yawning, penile erection and genital grooming in rats. A dose of cycloheximide, which inhibited brain protein synthesis by 50% totally prevented apomorphine-induced yawning and reduced by approximately 50% the occurrence of episodes of penile erection and genital grooming. However, this treatment failed to modify the stereotyped behaviour induced by 200 μg/kg of apomorphine. These results suggest that protein synthesis is required for the behavioural effects of small doses of apomorphine.  相似文献   

19.
The role of dopamine (DA) receptors in the expression of opioid dependence was examined by use of an unbiased conditioned place preference paradigm. Male Sprague-Dawley rats were implanted s.c. with two pellets containing placebo or 75mg morphine. Animals received one conditioning session with saline and one with the DA D1 receptor antagonist SCH23390 (0.01-0.05mg, s.c.) or the DA D2 receptor antagonist raclopride (0.25-1.0mg/kg, s.c.). Conditioning sessions were conducted 4 days after pellet implantation. During each of these sessions, physical signs of withdrawal were quantified. In morphine-pelleted animals, the D2 receptor antagonist raclopride produced conditioned place aversions, with a minimum effective dose of 0.5mg/kg. Administration of a higher dose also resulted in wet-dog shakes, ptosis and diarrhea in morphine-pelleted animals. This effect was not observed in response to lower doses of raclopride or in placebo-pelleted animals. The D1 receptor antagonist SCH23390 failed to produce conditioned place aversions in either morphine- or placebo-pelleted animals after single-trial conditioning. This antagonist was also ineffective in producing physical withdrawal signs. After two conditioning sessions with SCH23390, both the morphine- and placebo-pelleted animals exhibited a marked aversion for the SCH23390-paired place. However, there was no difference between groups in the magnitude of this effect. These data demonstrate that the acute blockade of D2 receptors produces aversive effects in opioid-dependent animals and that this effect occurs in the presence of few, if any, prototypic physical withdrawal signs. Furthermore, the inability of a selective D1 receptor antagonist to produce conditioned aversive effects or physical signs of withdrawal suggests an important role of D2 as compared to D1 receptors in the expression of morphine withdrawal signs.  相似文献   

20.
Clinical evidence suggests the possibility of conditioning of narcotic abstinence symptoms. Addicts report subjective and objective signs of withdrawal/craving when exposed to certain stimuli. This may partially explain the high rate of relapse to drug seeking behavior when treated addicts return to their home environment. Conditioning of narcotic abstinence symptoms was produced experimentally in five of eight volunteer subjects. Brief naloxone precipitated abstinence was the unconditioned response. The conditioned stimulus was a tone and odor. After an average of seven training trials, the tone and odor produced a conditioned abstinence response. The conditioned response consisted of subjective components (feelings of sickness, nausea, cramps, craving) and objective components (yawning, tearing, rhinorrhea, irregular respiration and transiently increased blood pressure). These laboratory findings support the anecdotal evidence regarding the existence of conditioned abstinence phenomena.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号