共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The focus of the present study was to examine covert orienting responses to peripheral flash cues among children with autism in a situation where attentional processes were taxed by the presence of distractors in the visual field. Fourteen children with autism (MA = 6-7 years) were compared to their MA-matched peers without autism on a forced choice RT covert orienting paradigm. The task conditions varied with regard to the target location, the validity of the cue, and the presence or absence of distractors. The results showed no group differences as both children with autism and their MA-matched peers showed similar effects of cue validity and distractors. These findings are inconsistent with the view that orienting is generally impaired in children with autism. 相似文献
2.
Goldberg MC Mostow AJ Vecera SP Larson JC Mostofsky SH Mahone EM Denckla MB 《Journal of autism and developmental disorders》2008,38(8):1405-1413
We examined the ability to use static line drawings of eye gaze cues to orient visual-spatial attention in children with high functioning autism (HFA) compared to typically developing children (TD). The task was organized such that on valid trials, gaze cues were directed toward the same spatial location as the appearance of an upcoming target, while on invalid trials gaze cues were directed to an opposite location. Unlike TD children, children with HFA showed no advantage in reaction time (RT) on valid trials compared to invalid trials (i.e., no significant validity effect). The two stimulus onset asynchronies (200 ms, 700 ms) did not differentially affect these findings. The results suggest that children with HFA show impairments in utilizing static line drawings of gaze cues to orient visual-spatial attention. 相似文献
3.
Turner-Brown LM Perry TD Dichter GS Bodfish JW Penn DL 《Journal of autism and developmental disorders》2008,38(9):1777-1784
The goal of this study was to evaluate the feasibility and utility of a group-based cognitive behavioral intervention to improve
social-cognitive functioning in adults with high-functioning autism (HFA). We modified the treatment manual of a previously
validated intervention, Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT), for optimal use with HFA adults (SCIT-A). We then
conducted a pilot study to compare SCIT-A (n = 6) to treatment as usual (TAU) (n = 5) for adults with HFA. Feasibility was supported; attendance was high (92%) and satisfaction reports were primarily positive.
Participants in SCIT-A showed significant improvement in theory-of-mind skills and trend level improvements in social communication
skills; TAU participants did not show these improvements. Findings indicate SCIT-A shows promise as an intervention for adults
with HFA. 相似文献
4.
Acts of dyadic orienting (responses to attention bids by a researcher) and acts of joint attention (e.g. pointing and showing behaviors) were observed in preschool children with autism and children with developmental delay. Children with autism responded to fewer adult vocal and non-vocal attention bids that were made singly and by combining modalities (e.g. name call plus touch). Sensitivity in dyadic orienting was significantly related to child-initiated acts of joint attention (IJA). Sensitivity to dyadic orienting was also significantly related to language and non-verbal ability. These findings indicate that dyadic orienting difficulties are found alongside triadic joint attention difficulties in children with autism. 相似文献
5.
6.
Freitag CM Kleser C Schneider M von Gontard A 《Journal of autism and developmental disorders》2007,37(5):948-959
Background: Motor impairment in children with Asperger Syndrome (AS) or High functioning autism (HFA) has been reported previously. This
study presents results of a quantitative assessment of neuromotor skills in 14–22 year old HFA/AS.
Methods: 16 HFA/AS and 16 IQ-matched controls were assessed by the Zurich Neuromotor Assessment (ZNA).
Results: The HFA/AS group showed strongest impairments of dynamic balance skills and diadochokinesis. Motor abilities were associated
with degree of social withdrawal in the full sample and severity of current autistic symptoms in the HFA/AS group.
Conclusion: Similar motor patterns as in younger children were found in the older adolescents. The association of autistic symptoms with
motor performance points towards an essential role of motor impairment in autism spectrum disorders. 相似文献
7.
Diane L. Williams Gerald Goldstein Nancy J. Minshew 《Journal of autism and developmental disorders》2013,43(4):794-806
This study used the modality shift experiment, a relatively simple reaction time measure to visual and auditory stimuli, to examine attentional shifting within and across modalities in 33 children and 42 adults with high-functioning autism as compared to matched numbers of age- and ability-matched typical controls. An exaggerated “modality shift effect” relative to the TD children occurred for the children with autism in conditions involving the reaction time when shifting from sound to light but not from light to sound. No exaggerated MSE was found for the adults with autism; rather, their responses were characterized by a generalized slowness relative to the adults with TD. These results suggest a lag in maturational development in autism in basic information processing mechanisms. 相似文献
8.
Cuccaro ML Nations L Brinkley J Abramson RK Wright HH Hall A Gilbert J Pericak-Vance MA 《Child psychiatry and human development》2007,37(4):347-360
In this study we compared 33 IQ and age matched pairs of individuals with Aspergers Disorder (ASP) and high functioning autism
(HFA) on measures of repetitive behavior. On the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R), the ASP and HFA groups showed
no differences in RBS-R Intensity score (severity) score or Frequency score (number of problems present). This suggests that
the two groups are similar with respect to␣the intensity or severity of repetitive behaviors and the presence of repetitive
behaviors. At the item level there were no differences on scales typically associated with autism (Stereotyped Behavior) and
ASP (Restricted Interests). Similarly, there were no differences between the groups on the Aberrant Behavior Checklist Stereotypy
scale. These findings add to the body of literature showing that HFA and ASP fail to differ with respect to repetitive behaviors.
The implications of the findings for neurobiologic and genetic studies are discussed. 相似文献
9.
Zwaigenbaum L Szatmari P Mahoney W Bryson S Bartolucci G MacLean J 《Journal of autism and developmental disorders》2000,30(2):121-126
Childhood Disintegrative Disorder (CDD) is grouped with autism as a subtype of Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD) in ICD-10 and DSM-IV. This is the first report of autism and CDD cosegregating within a sibship. J. P. and M. P. are half-brothers with the same mother. J. P. is an 18-year-old with impairments in communication, social reciprocity, and stereotypies and was diagnosed with autism. M. P. is a 7-year-old who developed normally to 2 years 4 months. He then underwent a profound regression, becoming nonverbal and socially withdrawn, and lost adaptive skills. Investigations did not reveal any neurodegenerative process. M. P. was diagnosed with CDD. The rarity of the two conditions suggests a shared transmissible mechanism. The implications for autism/PDD genetic studies are discussed. 相似文献
10.
11.
Elison JT Sasson NJ Turner-Brown LM Dichter G Bodfish JW 《Research in autism spectrum disorders》2012,6(2):842-851
Because previous studies of attention in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have been restricted in age range examined, little is known about how these processes develop over the course of childhood. In this study we examined cross-sectional age effects on patterns of visual attention to social and nonsocial information in 43 typically developing children and 51 children with ASD ranging in age from 2 to 18. Results indicated a sharp increase in visual exploration with age and a decrease in perseverative and detail-focused attention for both groups of children. However, increased age was associated with greater increases in visual exploration for typically developing children than for those children with ASD. The developmental differences were most pronounced for attention to certain nonsocial stimuli as children with ASD demonstrated a disproportionate attentional bias for these stimuli from very early in life. Disproportionate visual attention to certain nonsocial objects relative to social stimuli in ASD spanned from early to late childhood, and thus may represent both an early and a persistent characteristic of the disorder. 相似文献
12.
Paul R Shriberg LD McSweeny J Cicchetti D Klin A Volkmar F 《Journal of autism and developmental disorders》2005,35(6):861-869
Shriberg et al. [Shriberg, L. et al. (2001). Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 44, 1097–1115] described prosody–voice features of 30 high functioning speakers with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) compared to age-matched control speakers. The present study reports additional information on the speakers with ASD, including associations among prosody–voice variables and ratings of communication social abilities. Results suggest that the inappropriate sentential stress and hypernasality previously identified in some of these speakers is related to communication/sociability ratings. These findings and associated trends are interpreted to indicate important links between prosodic performance and social and communicative competence. They suggest the need for careful assessment of inappropriate prosody and voice features in speakers with ASD, and for effective intervention programs aimed at reducing the stigmatization of individuals with these conditions. 相似文献
13.
This study examined language and executive functions (EF) in high-functioning school-aged individuals with autism and individually
matched controls. Relationships between executive, language, and social functioning were also examined. Participants with
autism exhibited difficulty on measures of expressive grammar, figurative language, planning, and spatial working memory.
A mixed profile of impaired and enhanced abilities was noted in set-shifting. While controls showed the typical increase in
errors when shifting sets from an intra-dimensional to an extra-dimensional stimulus, this pattern was not noted in participants
with autism. Relationships between EF, language, and social performance were weak to non-existent. Implications for theories
of core deficit in autism and dissociable nature of the language and executive impairments in autism are discussed. 相似文献
14.
This investigation examined communication patterns between high functioning children with autism and their families and typically
developing children and their families within traditional dinner time conversation. Twenty families with a child with autism
(3.5–7 years.) and ten families with typically developing children (3.5–6 years) were video recorded during dinner and their
interactions were coded. Results revealed that children with autism initiated fewer bids for interactions, commented less
often, continued ongoing interactions through fewer conversational turns, and responded less often to family member communication
bids. Results are interpreted with respect to how communication patterns may be indicative of social communication deficits
not previously examined in high functioning children with autism. Strategies for social communication interventions within
the family and other natural contexts are discussed. 相似文献
15.
16.
Inge A. C. Grootscholten Bob van Wijngaarden Cornelis C. Kan 《Journal of autism and developmental disorders》2018,48(6):1920-1931
Primary caregivers experience consequences from being in close contact to a person with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This study used the Involvement Evaluation Questionnaire to explore the level of consequences of 104 caregivers involved with adults with High Functioning ASD (HF-ASD) and compared these with the consequences reported by caregivers of patients suffering from depression and schizophrenia. Caregivers involved with adults with an HF-ASD experience overall consequences comparable to those involved with patients with depression or schizophrenia. Worrying was the most reported consequence. More tension was experienced by the caregivers of ASD patients, especially by spouses. More care and attention for spouses of adults with an HF-ASD appears to be needed. 相似文献
17.
《Journal of mental health research in intellectual disabilities》2013,6(3):129-139
Sleep disturbances are a significant problem for persons with developmental disabilities. These problems occur at a higher rate than what is observed in the typically developing population, and persons with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) appear to be at a higher risk than individuals with other developmental disabilities. However, another major risk is intellectual disability (ID). These two groups of disorders overlap to a substantial degree. Thus, persons with ASD and ID appear to be particularly susceptible to sleep disturbances. These sleep problems can have serious consequences beyond sleep, particularly with respect to increased challenging behaviors and as an impediment to learning. Despite these concerns, adults with ASD and ID have been largely neglected with respect to the study of these nocturnal difficulties. In this report, 168 adults with ASD and ID were compared to 166 adults with ID alone in regard to sleep disturbance and related difficulties. Individuals in the ASD group presented with much higher rates of sleep disturbances, and greater severity of sleep disorder was related to the expression of more serious challenging behaviors. The implications of our results are discussed for future assessment and treatment of these disorders. 相似文献
18.
Verbal marking of affect by older children with Asperger Syndrome (AS) and high functioning autism (HFA) during spontaneous interactions is described. Discourse analysis of AS and HFA and typically developing children included frequency of affective utterances, affective initiations, affective labels and affective explanations, attribution of affective responses to self and others, and positive and negative markers of affect. Findings indicate that children with AS and HFA engaged in a higher proportion of affect marking and provided a higher proportion of affective explanations than typically developing children, yet were less likely to initiate affect marking sequences or talk about the affective responses of others. No significant differences were found between groups in terms of the marking of positive and negative affect. 相似文献
19.
Sanna Kuusikko Helena Haapsamo Eira Jansson-Verkasalo Tuula Hurtig Marja-Leena Mattila Hanna Ebeling Katja Jussila Sven Bölte Irma Moilanen 《Journal of autism and developmental disorders》2009,39(6):938-945
We examined upper facial basic emotion recognition in 57 subjects with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (M = 13.5 years) and 33 typically developing controls (M = 14.3 years) by using a standardized computer-aided measure (The Frankfurt Test and Training of Facial Affect Recognition,
FEFA). The ASD group scored lower than controls on the total scores of FEFA and perceived ambiguous stimuli more often as
a negative emotion. The older ASD group (≥12 years) performed better than the younger ASD group (<12 years) on the blended
emotions of FEFA. The results support the findings that individuals with ASD have difficulties in emotion recognition. However,
older subjects with ASD seem to have better skills than younger subjects with ASD. 相似文献