首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 218 毫秒
1.
Abadi RV  Gowen E 《Vision research》2004,44(23):2675-2690
Primary fixation is never perfectly stable, but is frequently interrupted by slow drifts, microsaccades and saccadic intrusions (SI). SI are involuntary, conjugate movements which take the form of an initial fast movement away from the desired eye position and followed after a short duration, by either a return secondary saccade or a drift. The purpose of this study was to examine the prevalence and metrics of SI in a population of 50 healthy subjects. Using both one and two dimensional recordings we find that all 50 members of the subject group exhibited SI. The SI were bilateral, conjugate and horizontal. No purely vertical SI were detected when examined in three subjects. SI amplitude mean and range was 0.6° ± 0.5°, 0.1°-4.1°; SI frequency mean and range was 18.0 ± 14.3 per min, 1.0-54.8 per min; SI duration mean and range was 225 ± 150, 20-870 ms. The mean SI amplitude and frequency when SI < 0.5° were removed was 0.97° ± 0.56° and 7.0 ± 11.4 per min respectively. Age was positively correlated with SI amplitude (p < 0.01), but there was no correlation between age and SI frequency. Three of four types of SI monophasic square wave intrusions (MSWI), biphasic square wave intrusions (BSWI) and double saccadic pulses (DSP) were found to be exclusively saccadic, whilst the fourth type, the single saccadic pulses (SSP), were confirmed to exhibit a slow secondary component. MSWI were the most frequently observed SI occurring in 47 out of 50 (94%) of the subjects with a mean amplitude, frequency and duration of 0.7° ± 0.5°, 11.5 ± 11.6 per min, and 255 ± 147 ms respectively. Mean amplitudes and frequencies for BSWI (n = 20), SSP (n = 11) and DSP (n = 34) were found to be 0.50° ± 0.2°, 1.2 ± 2.5 per min; 0.40° ± 0.20°, 0.4 ± 1.0 per min and 0.3° ± 0.4°, 5.0 ± 8.7 per min respectively. No differences in MSWI characteristics were found between binocular and monocular viewing. Possible explanations for SI occurrence include experimental viewing conditions, subject fatigue and covert shifts in attention.  相似文献   

2.
The UVS cone mechanism is known to light adapt at low intensities in comparison to other cones. We were interested in whether this property was related to higher sensitivity in UVS cones or to network adjustments in sensitivity. We investigated spectral sensitivity of 107 individual cone photoreceptors in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) using a whole-cell voltage clamp technique. Mean time-to-peak response was 339 ± 90 ms and flash sensitivity for a 100 ms flash was 4.37 × 10−3 ± 2.50 × 10−3 pA photons−1 μm2, with no significant differences between the UVS, SWS, MWS and LWS cone classes. The spectral sensitivity of the UVS, SWS and LWS cones conformed to the expression of SWS1, SWS2 and LWS opsin genes. The spectral sensitivity of MWS cones, however, showed clear evidence of co-expression of RH2a and RH2b opsin pigments. The fish used in this study bridged the ontogenetic stage where the MWS cones shift their expression from RH2b to RH2a.  相似文献   

3.

Purpose

It is well established that spatial adaptation can improve visual acuity over time in the presence of spherical defocus. It is less well known how far adaptation to astigmatic defocus can enhance visual acuity. We adapted subjects to “simulated” and optically-induced “real” astigmatic defocus, and studied how much they adapt and how selective adaptation was for the axis of astigmatism.

Methods

Ten subjects with a mean age of 26.7 ± 2.4 years (range 23-30) were enrolled in the study, three of them myopic (average spherical equivalent (SE) ± SD: −3.08 ± 1.42D) and seven emmetropic (average SE ± SD: −0.11 ± 0.18D). All had a corrected minimum visual acuity (VA) of log VA 0.0. For adaptation, subjects watched a movie at 4 m distance for 10 min that was convolved frame-by-frame with an astigmatic point spread function, equivalent to +3D defocus, or they watched an unfiltered movie but with spectacle frames with a 0/+3D astigmatic trial lenses. Subsequently, visual acuity was determined at the same distance, using high contrast letter acuity charts. Four experiments were performed. In experiment (1), simulated astigmatic defocus was presented both for adaptation and testing, in experiment (2) optically-induced astigmatic defocus was presented both for adaptation and testing of visual acuity. In all these cases, the +3D power meridian was at 0°. In experiments (3) and (4), the +3D power meridian was at 0° during adaptation but rotated to 90° during testing. Astigmatic defocus was simulated in experiment (3) but optically-induced in experiment (4).

Results

Experiments 1 and 2: adaptation to either simulated or real astigmatic defocus increased visual acuity in both test paradigms, simulated (change in VA 0.086 ± 0.069 log units; p < 0.01) and lens-induced astigmatic defocus (change in VA 0.068 ± 0.031 log units; p < 0.001). Experiments 3 and 4: when the axis was rotated, the improvement in visual acuity failed to reach significance, both for simulated (change in VA 0.042 ± 0.079 log units; p = 0.13) and lens-induced astigmatic defocus (change in VA 0.038 ± 0.086 log units; p = 0.19).

Conclusions

Adaptation to astigmatic defocus occurs for both simulated and real defocus, and the effects of adaptation seem to be selective for the axis of astigmatism. These observations suggest that adaptation involves a re-adjustment of the spatial filters selectively for astigmatic meridians, although the underlying mechanism must be more complicated than just changes in shapes of the receptive fields of retinal or cortical neurons.  相似文献   

4.
In adult albino (SD) and pigmented (PVG) rats the entire population of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) was quantified and their spatial distribution analyzed using a computerized technique. RGCs were back-labelled from the optic nerves (ON) or the superior colliculi (SCi) with Fluorogold (FG). Numbers of RGCs labelled from the ON [SD: 82,818 ± 3,949, n = 27; PVG: 89,241 ± 3,576, n = 6) were comparable to those labelled from the SCi [SD: 81,486 ± 4,340, n = 37; PVG: 87,229 ± 3,199; n = 59]. Detailed methodology to provide cell density information at small scales demonstrated the presence of a horizontal region in the dorsal retina with highest densities, resembling a visual streak.  相似文献   

5.
Purpose To investigate the characteristics of dynamic processing in the visual field of patients with age-related maculopathy (ARM) by measuring motion sensitivity, double-pulse resolution (DPR), and critical flicker fusion. Methods Fourteen subjects with ARM (18 eyes), 14 age-matched controls (19 eyes), and 7 young controls (8 eyes) served as subjects. Motion contrast thresholds were determined by a four-alternative forced-choice (4 afc) staircase procedure with a modification by Kernbach for presenting a plaid (size = 3.8°) moving within a stationary spatial and temporal Gaussian envelope in one of four directions. Measurements were performed on the horizontal meridian at 10°, 20°, 30°, 40°, and 60° eccentricity. DPR was defined as the minimal temporal gap detectable by the subject using a 9-fold interleaved adaptive procedure, with stimuli positioned on concentric rings at 5°, 10°, and 20° eccentricity on the principal and oblique meridians. Critical flicker fusion thresholds (CFF) and the Lanthony D-15 color vision test were applied foveally, and the subjects were free to use their fovea or whatever retinal area they needed to use instead, due to their retinal lesions caused by ARM. All measurements were performed under photopic conditions. Results Motion contrast sensitivity in subjects with ARM was pronouncedly reduced (0.23–0.66 log units, p < 0.01), not only in the macula but in a region up to 20° eccentricity. In the two control groups, motion contrast sensitivity systematically declined with retinal eccentricity (0.009–0.032 log units/degree) and with age (0.01 log units/year). Double-pulse thresholds in healthy subjects were approximately constant in the central visual field and increased outside a radius of 10° (1.73 ms/degree). DPR thresholds were elevated in subjects with ARM (by 23–32 ms, p < 0.01) up to 20° eccentricity, and their foveal CFFs were increased by 5.5 Hz or 14% (p < 0.01) as compared with age-matched controls. Conclusions Dynamic processing properties in subjects with ARM are severely impaired in the central visual field up to 20° eccentricity, which is clearly beyond the borders of the macula. Presented at ARVO 2004 (#3084): Reduced motion sensitivity inside and outside the macula in age-related maculopathy.  相似文献   

6.
Shen M  Fan F  Xue A  Wang J  Zhou X  Lu F 《Vision research》2008,48(21):2167-2171

Purpose

To determine corneal biomechanical properties in patients with high myopia.

Design

Observational study.

Methods

High myopia patients (n = 45, age: 37.0 ± 12.6 years) with refractive errors of spherical equivalent (SE) greater than −9.00 D were recruited in this study along with healthy subjects (n = 90, age: 33.7 ± 12.4 years) with refractive errors of SE ranging from 0 D to −3.00 D. Only the right eye was studied. Central corneal thickness (CCT) was measured by optical coherence tomography (OCT). Metrics of corneal biomechanical properties, including corneal hysteresis (CH) and corneal resistance factor (CRF), were measured with the Ocular Response Analyzer (ORA). The ORA also determined the values of intraocular pressure (IOPg) and corneal compensated IOP (IOPcc).

Results

No significant differences of CCT and CRF were present between the two groups (P = .15 and 0.35 for CCT and CRF, respectively); however, CH in the high myope group was lower than that in the controls (P < .01). IOPg and IOPcc were both significantly higher in the high myopes compared to the controls. In both groups, there were significant correlations between CH and CCT and between CRF and CCT. CH was not significantly correlated with age in either the control group or the high myope group (P > .05). There was a significant correlation between CH and SE when the two groups were combined for analysis.

Conclusion

CH, but not CRF, was significantly lower in high myopia patients compared to that in normal subjects. The results indicate that some compromised aspects of the biomechanical properties of cornea may exist in people with high myopia.  相似文献   

7.
We examined in tree shrews the effect of age on the development of, and recovery from, myopia induced with a negative lens. Starting at 11, 16, 24, 35 or 48 days after natural eye-opening (days of visual experience [VE]), juvenile tree shrews (n = 5 per group) wore a monocular −5 D lens for 11 days. A long-term lens-wear group (n = 6) began treatment at 16 days of VE and wore the lens for 30 days. A young adult group (n = 5) began to wear a −5 D lens between 93 and 107 days of VE (mean ± SD, 100 ± 6 days of VE) and wore the lens for 29-54 days (mean ± SD, 41.8 ± 9.8 days). The recovery phase in all groups was started by discontinuing −5 D lens wear. Contralateral control eyes in the three youngest groups were compared with a group of age-matched normal eyes and showed a small (<1 D), transient myopic shift. The amount of myopia that developed during lens wear was measured as the difference between the treated and control eye refractions. After 11 days of lens wear, the induced myopia was similar for the four younger groups (near full compensation: 11 days, −5.1 ± 0.4 D; 16 days, −4.7 ± 0.3 D; 24 days, −4.9 ± 0.4 D; 35 days, −4.0 ± 0.02) and slightly less in the oldest juvenile group (48 days, −3.3 ± 0.5 D). The young adult animals developed −4.8 ± 0.3 D of myopia after a longer lens-wear period. The rate of compensation (D/day) was high in the 4 youngest groups and decreased in the 48-day and young adult groups. The refractions of the long-term lens-wear juvenile group remained stable after compensating for the −5 D lens. During recovery, all animals in the youngest group recovered fully (<1 D residual myopia) within 7 days. Examples of both rapid (<10 days) and slow recovery (>12 days) occurred in all age groups except the youngest. Every animal showed more rapid recovery (higher recovery slope) in the first 4 days than afterward. One animal showed extremely slow recovery. Based on the time-course of myopia development observed in the youngest age groups, the start of the susceptible period for negative-lens wear is around 11-15 days after eye opening; the rate of compensation remains high until approximately 35 days of VE and then gradually declines. Compensation is stable with continued lens wear. The emmetropization mechanism, both for lens compensation and recovery, remains active into young adulthood. The time-course of recovery is more variable than that of compensation and seems to vary with age, with the amount of myopia (weakly) and with the individual animal.  相似文献   

8.
The purpose of the present study was to investigate the effects of granulocyte colony-stimulating factor (G-CSF) on neurodegeneration of optic nerve (ON) and retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) in a rat model of ON crush. The ONs of adult male Wistar rats (150-180 g) were crushed by a standardized method. The control eyes received a sham operation. G-CSF (100 μg/kg/day in 0.2 ml phosphate-buffered saline) or phosphate-buffered saline (PBS control) was immediately administered after ON crush for 5 days by subcutaneous injection. Rats were euthanized at 1 or 2 weeks after the crush injury. RGC density was counted by retrograde labeling with FluoroGold application to the superior colliculus, and visual function was assessed by flash visual evoked potentials (FVEP). TUNEL assay, Western blot analysis and immunohistochemistry of p-AKT in the retina and ED1 (marker of macrophage/microglia) in the ON were conducted. 2 weeks after the insult, the RGC densities in the central and mid-peripheral retinas in ON-crushed, G-CSF-treated rats were significantly higher than that of the corresponding ON-crushed, PBS-treated rats (survival rate was 60% vs. 19.6% in the central retina; 46.5% vs. 23.9% in mid-peripheral retina, respectively; p < 0.001). FVEP measurements showed a significantly better preserved latency of the p1 wave in the ON-crushed, G-CSF-treated rats than the ON-crushed, PBS-treated rats (78 ± 9 ms in the sham operation group, 98 ± 16 ms in the G-CSF-treated group, and 174 ± 16 ms in the PBS-treated group; p < 0.001). TUNEL assays showed fewer apoptotic cells in the retinal sections in the ON-crushed, G-CSF-treated rats. p-AKT immunoreactivity was up-regulated in the retinas of the ON-crushed, G-CSF-treated rats at 1 and 2 weeks. In addition, the number of ED1-positive cells was attenuated at the lesion site of the optic nerve in the ON-crushed, G-CSF-treated group. From these results, we gather that administration of G-CSF is neuroprotective in the rat model of optic nerve crush, as demonstrated both structurally by RGC density and functionally by FVEP. G-CSF may work by being anti-apoptotic involving the p-AKT signaling pathway as well as by attenuation of the inflammatory responses at the injury site, as evidenced by less ED1-positive cell infiltration in the optic nerve.  相似文献   

9.
Spatial cueing of transient attention has recently been shown to reduce temporal sensitivity. We investigated how the size of the sustained attentional focus influences double-pulse resolution (DPR) thresholds mapped across the visual field in a sample of 95 healthy subjects using a 9-fold interleaved adaptive algorithm (YAAP). Peripheral DPR thresholds increased for measurements between 2.5 degrees and 20 degrees eccentricity. Additionally, central DPR thresholds increased at a similar rate when measured with increasingly larger stimulus displays for peripheral measurements. This latter effect suggests that temporal resolution decreases with a larger sustained attention focus and cannot be explained by retinal characteristics only.  相似文献   

10.
The purpose of this study was to test the hypothesis that ischemia/reperfusion injury in the rat retina may be ameliorated by reducing retinal metabolism with either hypothermia or inhibitory GABA agonists. The intraocular pressure of each right eye in rats was raised to 130 mmHg for 60 min with the left eye serving as normal control. The rats were divided into four groups in terms of drug and hypothermia treatment: (1) Untreated ischemia, (2) Hypothermia, (3) Baclofen/midazolam and (4) Baclofen/muscimol. Electroretinogram was recorded before ischemia and again after 10-day reperfusion. Histological analysis with H&E staining and cell counts was performed. Untreated ischemia/reperfusion resulted in severely reduced ERG responses. The ERG b-wave was reduced from 423 ± 144 μV to 130 ± 91 μV (mean ± SD, n = 5). With hypothermia the ERG b-wave was reduced from 499 ± 80 μV to 237 ± 111 μV (n = 4). With combinations of baclofen and midazolam the ERG b-wave was reduced from 432 ± 96 μV to 104 ± 67 μV (n = 7). In baclofen/muscimol treated eyes the ERG b-wave went from 426 ± 101 μV to 148 ± 118 μV (n = 6). The histological tissue damage was severe in untreated ischemia and the baclofen/midazolam and baclofen/muscimol groups, but less severe in the hypothermia group. The GABA agonists do not provide any protection in our ischemia/reperfusion model. Our results are consistent with earlier reports that hypothermia may be helpful in ischemic conditions in the retina.  相似文献   

11.
Zhenlan Jin 《Vision research》2009,49(16):2045-2055
Can a release of attention from fixation help explain the saccadic ‘gap effect’, the shortening of saccadic latency (SL) when the fixation spot is extinguished just before saccade target onset? Practiced observers generated SLs and button-presses to one of four 10° eccentric targets in overlap (fixation spot stays on), gap0 (fixation offsets at target onset), and gap200 conditions; in gap200, the fixation spot was removed, dimmed, expanded, or brightened 200 ms before target onset. Our data excluded speed-accuracy trade-offs, express saccades, stimulus salience, and oculomotor readiness, while fixation offset and general warning had minor effects, leaving attention release as the default explanation. Supporting this notion, finger-press reactions to foveal probe dots presented after the fixation spot was brightened (to hold attention) were faster than those made after the spot was removed (to release attention). Varying the time from gap onset to the probe dot mapped out the time-course of the putative attentional release, which takes ∼140 ms.  相似文献   

12.
In adult Swiss albino and C57 pigmented mice, RGCs were identified with a retrogradely transported neuronal tracer applied to both optic nerves (ON) or superior colliculi (SCi). After histological processing, the retinas were prepared as whole-mounts, examined and photographed under a fluorescence microscope equipped with a motorized stage controlled by a commercial computer image analysis system: Image-Pro Plus® (IPP). Retinas were imaged as a stack of 24-bit color images (140 frames per retina) using IPP with the Scope-Pro plug-in 5.0 and the images montaged to create a high-resolution composite of the retinal whole-mount when required. Single images were also processed by specific macros written in IPP that apply a sequence of filters and transformations in order to separate individual cells for automatic counting. Cell counts were later transferred to a spreadsheet for statistical analysis and used to generate a RGC density map for each retina. Results: The mean total numbers of RGCs labeled from the ON, in Swiss (49,493 ± 3936; n = 18) or C57 mice (42,658 ± 1540; n = 10) were slightly higher than the mean numbers of RGCs labeled from the SCi, in Swiss (48,733 ± 3954; n = 43) or C57 mice (41,192 ± 2821; n = 42), respectively. RGCs were distributed throughout the retina and density maps revealed a horizontal region in the superior retina near the optic disk with highest RGC densities. In conclusion, the population of mice RGCs may be counted automatically with a level of confidence comparable to manual counts. The distribution of RGCs adopts a form of regional specialization that resembles a horizontal visual streak.  相似文献   

13.
Normal observers perceive less motion smear if a target moves in the opposite direction of a smooth eye movement than if the target moves to produce the same retinal image speed in the same direction as the eye movement. This study investigated whether a similar asymmetrical attenuation of perceived motion smear occurs in observers with infantile nystagmus (IN). Observers (N = 3) viewed a laser spot that moved for 100 or 125 ms to the right or left at a speed between 5 and 60°/s during the slow phase of jerk IN. After each trial, the observer adjusted the length of a bright line to match the extent of the perceived smear. Across observers, the average duration of perceived smear was 39 and 106 ms, respectively, for relative motion of the laser spot in the opposite vs. the same direction as the IN slow phase. In one observer with periodic alternating nystagmus, the direction of spot motion that produced less perceived smear reversed with an alternation in the direction of the IN slow phase. The reduction of perceived motion smear for relative target motion in the opposite direction of IN slow phases is attributed to extra-retinal signals that accompany IN. As during normal eye movements, the reduction of perceived smear for this direction of relative motion should foster the perception of clarity in the stationary visual world.  相似文献   

14.
To interact with its visual environment, an organism needs to perceive objects in both space and time. High temporal resolution is hence important to the fitness of diurnally active animals, not least highly active aerial species such as birds. However, temporal resolution, as assessed by flicker fusion frequency (FFF; the stimulus frequency at which a flickering light stimulus can no longer be resolved and appears continuous) or critical flicker fusion frequency (CFF; the highest flicker fusion frequency at any light intensity) has rarely been assessed in birds. In order to further our understanding of temporal resolution as a function of light intensity in birds we used behavioural experiments with domestic chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus) from an old game breed ‘Gammalsvensk dvärghöna’ (which is morphologically and behaviourally similar to the wildtype ancestor, the red jungle fowl, G. gallus), to generate an ‘Intensity/FFF curve’ (I/FFF curve) across full spectrum light intensities ranging from 0.2 to 2812 cd m−2. The I/FFF curve is double-branched, resembling that of other chordates with a duplex retina of both rods and cones. Assuming that the branches represent rod and cone mediated responses respectively, the break point between them places the transition between scotopic and photopic vision at between 0.8 and 1.9 cd m−2. Average FFF ranged from 19.8 Hz at the lowest light intensity to a CFF 87.0 Hz at 1375 cd m−2. FFF dropped slightly at the highest light intensity. There was some individual variation with certain birds displaying CFFs of 90-100 Hz. The FFF values demonstrated by this non-selected breed appear to be considerably higher than other behaviourally derived FFF values for similar stimuli reported for white and brown commercial laying hens, indicating that the domestication process might have influenced temporal resolution in chicken.  相似文献   

15.
We examine the time course of visual grouping and figure-ground processing. Figure (contour) and ground (random-texture) elements were flickered with different phases (i.e., contour and background are alternated), requiring the observer to group information within a pre-specified time window. It was found this grouping has a high temporal resolution: less than 20 ms for smooth contours, and less than 50 ms for line conjunctions with sharp angles. Furthermore, the grouping process takes place without an explicit knowledge of the phase of the elements, and it requires a cumulative build-up of information. The results are discussed in relation to the neural mechanism for visual grouping and figure-ground segregation.  相似文献   

16.
This study investigated the effects of visual status, driver age and the presence of secondary distracter tasks on driving performance. Twenty young (M = 26.8 years) and 19 old (M = 70.2 years) participants drove around a closed-road circuit under three visual (normal, simulated cataracts, blur) and three distracter conditions (none, visual, auditory). Simulated visual impairment, increased driver age and the presence of a distracter task detrimentally affected all measures of driving performance except gap judgments and lane keeping. Significant interaction effects were evident between visual status, age and distracters; simulated cataracts had the most negative impact on performance in the presence of visual distracters and a more negative impact for older drivers. The implications of these findings for driving behaviour and acquisition of driving-related information for people with common visual impairments are discussed.  相似文献   

17.
This study determined retinal thinning in a mammalian model of high myopia using optical coherence tomography (OCT) and histological sections from the same retinal tissue. High myopia was induced in three tree shrews (Tupaia belangeri) by deprivation of form vision via lid suture of one eye, with the other eye a control. Ocular biometry data was obtained by Ascan ultrasonography, keratometry and retinoscopy. The Zeiss StratusOCT was used to obtain Bscans in vivo across the retina. Subsequently, eyes were enucleated and retinas fixed, dehydrated, embedded and sectioned. Treated eyes developed a high degree of axial myopia (−15.9 ± 2.3 D; n = 3). The OCT analysis showed that in myopic eyes the nasal retina thinned more than the temporal retina relative to the disc (p = 0.005). Histology showed that the retinas in the myopic eyes comprise all layers but were thinner than the retinas in normal and control eyes. Detailed thickness measurements in corresponding locations of myopic and control eyes in superior nasal retina using longitudinal reflectivity profiles from OCT and semithin vertical histological sections showed the percentage of retinal thinning in the myopic eyes was similar between methods (OCT 15.34 ± 5.69%; histology 17.61 ± 3.02%; p = 0.10). Analysis of retinal layers revealed that the inner plexiform, inner nuclear and outer plexiform layers thin the most. Cell density measurements showed all neuronal cell types are involved in retinal thinning. The results indicate that in vivo OCT measurements can accurately detect retinal thinning in high myopia.  相似文献   

18.
PurposeTo quantify astigmatism-related meridional anisotropy in visual resolution at central, nasal, and inferior visual fields.MethodsThree groups of young adults (range, 18–30 years) with corrected-to-normal visual acuity (logMAR 0) were recruited: (1) myopic astigmats (MA): spherical-equivalent error (SE) < −0.75D, with-the-rule astigmatism ≥ 2.00D, n = 19; (2) simple myopes (SM): SE < −0.75D, astigmatism ≤ 0.50D, n = 20; and (3) emmetropes (EM): SE ± 0.50D, astigmatism ≤ 0.50D, n = 14. Resolution acuity was measured for the horizontal and vertical gratings at central and peripheral visual fields (eccentricity: 15°) using a 3-down 1-up staircase paradigm. On- and off-axis refractive errors were corrected by ophthalmic lenses.ResultsThe MA group exhibited meridional anisotropy preferring vertical gratings. At the central field, the MA group had better resolution acuity for vertical than horizontal gratings, and their resolution acuity for horizontal gratings was significantly worse than the SM and EM groups. At peripheral visual fields, both the SM and EM groups showed better resolution acuity for the radial (i.e., nasal field: horizontal gratings; inferior field: vertical gratings) than tangential orientation. However, the MA group tended to have better resolution acuity for the tangential orientation (i.e., vertical gratings), and their resolution acuity for horizontal gratings was significantly lower than the SM and EM groups at the nasal field. No significant differences were found in the inferior field among the three groups.ConclusionsThis study provided evidence of astigmatism-related meridional anisotropy at the fovea and nasal visual fields, underscoring the significant impact of astigmatism on orientation-dependent visual functions.  相似文献   

19.

Purpose

To evaluate the impact of intraocular (IOP) reduction on retinal ganglion cell (RGC) function measured using pattern electroretinogram optimized for glaucoma (PERGLA) in glaucoma suspect and glaucomatous eyes receiving latanoprost 0.005% versus placebo.

Methods

This was a prospective, placebo-controlled, double masked, cross-over clinical trial. One randomly selected eye of each subject meeting eligibility criteria was enrolled. At each visit, subjects underwent five diurnal measurements between 8:00 am and 4:00 pm consisting of Goldmann IOP, and PERGLA measurements. A baseline examination was performed following a 4-week washout period, and repeat examination after randomly receiving latanoprost or placebo for 4-weeks. Subjects were then crossed over to receive the alternative therapy for 4 weeks following a second washout period, and underwent repeat examination. Linear mixed-effect models were used for the analysis.

Results

Sixty-eight eyes (35 glaucoma, 33 glaucoma suspect) of 68 patients (mean age 67.4 ± 10.6 years) were enrolled. The mean IOP (mm Hg) after latanoprost 0.005% therapy (14.9 ± 3.8) was significantly lower than baseline (18.8 ± 4.7, < 0.001) or placebo (18.0 ± 4.3), with a mean reduction of −20 ± 13%. Mean PERGLA amplitude (μV) and phase (π-radian) using latanoprost (0.49 ± 0.22 and 1.71 ± 0.22, respectively) were similar (> 0.05) to baseline (0.49 ± 0.24 and 1.69 ± 0.19) and placebo (0.50 ± 0.24 and 1.72 ± 0.23). No significant (> 0.05) diurnal variation in PERGLA amplitude was observed at baseline, or using latanoprost or placebo. Treatment with latanoprost, time of day, and IOP were not significantly (> 0.05) associated with PERGLA amplitude or phase.

Conclusion

Twenty percent IOP reduction using latanoprost monotherapy is not associated with improvement in RGC function measured with PERGLA.  相似文献   

20.
Studies using conventional electron microscopy describe the cytoplasm of lens fiber cells as having essentially an amorphous structure. We hypothesized that significant structural detail might have been lost as a result of projecting the entire thickness of the section (50-100 nm) onto a single plane (the “projection artifact”). To test this hypothesis, we studied the 3D-structure of rat lens cortical fibers before and after extracting the “soluble” crystallins with low ionic strength buffers to make “ghosts.” Tomographic series in conical geometry were collected at 55° tilts and by 5° rotations until completing a 360° turn by low dose methods. They were aligned using fiduciary points, reconstructed with the weighted back projection algorithm and refined by projection matching. Analysis of the 3D-maps included semiautomatic density segmentation using a computer program based on the watershed algorithm. We found that the cytoplasm of cortical fibers, though appearing amorphous in regions of the highest density, was in fact comprised of an ordered structure resembling a “clustered matrix.” The matrix was comprised of thin (∼6 nm diameter) filaments bent sharply at 110-120° angles and studded with cube-shaped particles (the “beaded” filaments). In cortical fibers, the particles measured a = 14 ± 2, b = 13 ± 2 and c = 10 ± 2.4 nm (n = 30, mean ± SD) and were spaced at distances measuring 27.5 ± 2.4 nm apart (n = 8, mean ± SD), center-to-center. The matrix was formed as “beaded” filaments, bound to clusters of “soluble” proteins, crossed each other at nearly perpendicular angles. The matrix also made contact with the plasma membrane at a large number of distinct regions. We thus concluded that the cytoplasm of cortical lens fibers is comprised of a cytoskeletal matrix of “beaded” filaments that organize the “soluble” crystallins in separate regions. The association of this matrix with the plasma membrane allows the lens to maintain its structural integrity, while its association with crystallins yields its long-term transparency. Loss of either function likely would play a significant role in cataract formation.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号