首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 765 毫秒
1.
The ability to estimate a filled interval of time is affected by numerous non-temporal factors, such as the sensory modality, duration, and the intensity of the stimulus. Here we explore the role of modality (auditory or visual), stimulus intensity (low vs. high), and motor response speed on the ability to reproduce the duration of short (<1 s) filled intervals. In accordance with the literature, the reproduced duration was affected by both the modality and the intensity of the stimulus; longer reproduction times were generally observed for visual as compared to auditory stimuli, and for low as compared to high-intensity stimuli. We used general estimating equations in order to determine whether these factors independently affected participants’ ability to reproduce a given duration, after eliminating the variability associated with reaction time, since it covaried with the reproduced durations. This analysis revealed that stimulus duration, modality, and intensity were all significant independent predictors of the reproduced durations. Additionally, duration interacted with intensity when reproducing auditory intervals. That is, after taking into account the general speeding-up effect that high-intensity stimuli have on responses, they seem to have an additional effect on the rate of the internal clock. These results support previous evidence suggesting that auditory and visual clocks run at different speeds.  相似文献   

2.
Whereas sensory memory in humans has been found to store auditory information for a longer time than visual information, it is unclear whether this is the case also in other species. We recorded hippocampal event-related potentials (ERPs) in awake rabbits exposed to occasional changes in a repeated 50-ms acoustic (1000 versus 2000 Hz) and visual (vertical versus horizontal orientation) stimulus. Three intervals (500, 1500, or 3000 ms) between stimulus repetitions were applied. Whereas acoustic changes significantly affected ERPs with the repetition intervals of 500 and 1500 ms, visual changes did so only with the repetition interval of 500 ms. Our finding, thus, suggests a similarity in sensory processing abilities between human and non-human mammals.  相似文献   

3.
The objective of this study was to verify hypothesised changes in event related potentials (visual mismatch negativity, vMMN) in 17 subjects dependent on methamphetamine (MAMP) compared to age and gender matched 17 healthy volunteers. We found a significant correlation between vMMN and duration of methamphetamine abuse (Spearman correlation coefficient r=0.54-0.78; P<.05). The positive correlation indicates drop of originally more negative response to deviant stimulus, what may indicate a pre-attentive processing enhancement in the first years of MAMP abuse with its decease later on. Accordingly, post-hoc analysis revealed significantly stronger vMMN in patients with length of MAMP abuse shorter than 5 years than in paired controls. There were no such differences in abusers with the length of abuse longer than 5 years. The results show that the visual processing on the pre-attentional level can be influenced by long-term MAMP abuse, what can be specifically assessed by vMMN.  相似文献   

4.
The effects of a prestimulus on the electrically elicited blink reflex components were investigated in 20 healthy subjects. In the first group of 10 subjects (warned group), electric shocks were delivered in isolation or preceded, at an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 0.1 s, 1 s, or 10 s, by a visual or acoustic warning stimulus. In the second group of 10 subjects (unwarned group), the electric shocks were delivered either in isolation or preceded, at the same ISI, by visual or acoustic stimuli having no warning value. The modulation of the three blink reflex components was then analysed. Compared to the baseline condition, the R1 oligosynaptic component was enhanced at 0.1 s and 1 s ISI, in the warned group with the visual prestimulus, but only at 0.1 s after a visual and acoustic prestimulus in the unwarned group. On the contrary, the polysynaptic responses showed a different course: R2 was significantly reduced at the 0.1 s interval in the warned group with both the prestimuli, and only with the visual prestimulus in the unwarned group. The R3 was inhibited at all three intervals with the visual prestimulus, and at the 0.1 s and 1 s with the acoustic one in the warned group, and only at 0.1 s in the unwarned group, both after visual and acoustic prestimuli. The decrement in R2 and R3 observed with the shortest interval was probably related to the prepulse inhibition of startle reflex. Furthermore, only R3 was still inhibited at longer intervals, when the sustained processes of attention may have influenced this component. Perhaps this combination of events represents, in the warned group, the best preparation for voluntary reflex reaction.  相似文献   

5.
The vestibular system detects the velocity of the head even in complete darkness, and thus contributes to spatial orientation. However, during vestibular estimation of linear passive self-motion distance in darkness, healthy human subjects mainly rely on time, and they replicate also stimulus duration when required to reproduce previous self-rotation. We then made the hypothesis that the perception of vestibular-sensed motion duration is embedded within encoding of motion kinetics. The ability to estimate time during passive self-motion in darkness was examined with a self-rotation reproduction paradigm. Subjects were required to replicate through self-driven transport the plateau velocity (30, 60 and 90 °/s) and duration (2, 3 and 4 s) of the previously imposed whole-body rotation (trapezoid velocity profile) in complete darkness; the rotating chair position was recorded (500 Hz) during the whole trials. The results showed that the peak velocity, but not duration, of the plateau phase of the imposed rotation was accurately reproduced. Suspecting that the velocity instruction had impaired the duration reproduction, we added a control experiment requiring subjects to reproduce two successive identical rotations separated by a momentary motion interruption (MMI). The MMI was of identical duration to the previous plateau phase. MMI duration was fidelitously reproduced whereas that of the plateau phase was hypometric (i.e. lesser reproduced duration than plateau) suggesting that subjective time is shorter during vestibular stimulation. Furthermore, the accurate reproduction of the whole motion duration, that was not required, indicates an automatic process and confirms that vestibular duration perception is embedded within motion kinetics.  相似文献   

6.
Pattern reversal visual evoked potentials in fencers.   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
Visual evoked potentials were recorded from occipital and temporal leads in the two cerebral hemispheres of eight fencers and eight control subjects. The stimulus was a checkerboard subtending a small (1 degree) or large (30 degrees) visual field. Significant differences in P60, N75 and P100 latency and amplitude were found between the two subject groups, especially during the processing of the large visual field. In fencers and left-handers shorter latencies were found for the large visual field condition, whereas right-handers showed an opposite trend. The results give further evidence of special patterns of visual processing in athletes, like fencers, in agreement with the literature.  相似文献   

7.
Summary Some of the properties of a single mammalian straited muscle fibre were studied by applying a local depolarizing or hyperpolarizing current.During a long lasting depolarizing current of more than 0.05 A or after termination of a hyperpolarizing current of about 0.5 A a burst of spikes was evoked.The time intervalls between the action potentials got progressively longer with the time after onset of the positive current pulse or after termination of the negative current.Not only the stimulus current-time interval relation appears to be logarithmic for both stimulus polarities but also the stimulus duration-time interval relation with negative stimulation.The change in the time intervals between successive action potentials during a positive current is much slower than after a hyperpolarizing negative current.It is suggested that the increase in the time intervals between the action potentials evoked during a depolarizing stimulus is related to accommodation of the fibre and that the change in the time intervals after a hyperpolarizing stimulus is due to the fibre accommodation and to the recovery of the fibre after a partial breakdown of the membrane.  相似文献   

8.
 The present study investigated the processing of durations on the order of seconds with slow cortical potential changes. The question is whether trial-to-trial fluctuations in temporal productions or judgments correspond to variations in the amplitude of surface Laplacians computed over particular scalp regions. Topographical analyses were done using the source derivation method. Subjects performed three successive tasks: (1) time production, in which they produced a 2.5-s interval separated by two brief trigger presses; (2) time discrimination, in which they detected small differences in intervals delimited by two brief clicks in comparison with a memorized standard interval; and (3) intensity discrimination (control task, devoid of time judgments), in which they detected small differences between the intensity of clicks, in comparison with standard clicks initially memorized. In order to focus on subjective differences, in the two discrimination tasks most comparison stimuli were identical to the standard, without the subjects being aware of it. At FCz, reflecting activity from the mesial frontocentral cortex that mainly includes the supplementary motor area (SMA), larger negativities were found during the longer target intervals, whether these were produced (task 1) or judged so (task 2). Those performance-dependent trends were restricted to the target intervals of the temporal tasks; they appeared neither during the 2 s preceding the target, nor during the control task. The data therefore suggest that the SMA subserves important functions in timing both sensory and motor tasks. We propose that the SMA either provides the ”pulse accumulation” process commonly postulated in models of time processing or that it receives output from this process through striatal efferent pathways. Received: 12 March 1998 / Accepted: 6 November 1998  相似文献   

9.
Conditioned auditory discrimination and extinction of the skin potential response were attempted in 4-month-old infants using interstimulus intervals of 1500, 3500, 5500, and 7500 msec. Half of the infants in each of the interstimulus interval groups were defined as high magnitude orienters and half were low magnitude orienters. Conditioning was successful with the 5500- and 7500-msec interstimulus intervals, but not with the 1500- and 3500-msec intervals. Analysis of individual subject data indicated that individual differences in conditionability were related to interstimulus interval and orienting response magnitude. Also, those subjects discriminating at the longer intervals tended to be high magnitude orienters. In other words, longer interstimulus intervals interacted with a high magnitude of orienting to facilitate conditioning. The results were taken as evidence that individual differences in the magnitude of the orienting response reflect different individual needs in stimulus information processing time.  相似文献   

10.
P300, Probability, and Interstimulus Interval   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
John  Polich 《Psychophysiology》1990,27(4):396-403
The relationship between target stimulus probability and interstimulus interval on the P300 (P3) component of the event-related potential was assessed in three experiments. In each study an auditory discrimination paradigm was employed wherein subjects indicated with a finger tap response the occurrence of a randomly presented 2000 Hz target tone embedded in a series of 1,000 Hz tones. Target stimuli were presented with a probability of either .20 or .80 in different conditions which were combined factorially with different interstimulus intervals. Experiment 1 presented stimuli at 2.0, 2.5, or 3.0 second intervals; Experiment 2 presented stimuli at 2.0, 3.0, or 4.0 second intervals; Experiment 3 presented stimuli at 4.0, 6.0, or 10.0 second intervals. P3 amplitude was larger for the .20 relative to the .80 target probability conditions for the shorter interstimulus intervals but not for the longer intervals. P3 latency was consistently longer for the .20 relative to the .80 target probability conditions, with generally little effect observed for interstimulus interval changes. The results suggest that interstimulus interval affects component amplitude by determining the amount of processing resources available when the P3 is produced.  相似文献   

11.
In self-rotation reproduction tasks, subjects appear to estimate the displacement angle and then reproduce this angle without necessarily replicating the entire temporal velocity profile. In contrast, subjects appear to reproduce the entire temporal velocity profile during linear motion stimulating the otoliths. To investigate what happens during combined rotation and translation, we investigated in darkness the central processing of vestibular cues during eccentric rotation. Controlling a centrifuge with a joystick, nine healthy subjects were asked to reproduce the angle of the previously imposed rotation. Rotations were either ON-center, or 50 cm OFF-center with inter-aural centripetal acceleration. Rotation duration was either variable (proportional to the traveled angle), or constant. We examined whether the stimulation of the otoliths during OFF-center rotation changes self-rotation reproduction, and whether rotation duration is processed differently by the nervous system with and without otolith stimulation. As postulated, the subjects indeed reproduced more closely the stimulus velocity profile when OFF-center. But the primary result is that the additional supra-threshold linear acceleration cues, measured by the otoliths, did not improve performance. More specifically, to our surprise, the ability to reproduce rotation angle degraded slightly in the presence of additional information from the otolith organs, with the linear acceleration cues appearing to interfere with the reproduction of movement duration.  相似文献   

12.
Conditioned taste aversion can be acquired when rats experience an unconditioned stimulus (US) while anesthetized. In contrast to anesthetics, a hypothermia-induced comatose state immediately after presentation of a taste conditioned stimulus (CS) prevented a taste-illness association at relatively short CS-US intervals and potentiated the aversion at longer intervals. Results at shorter CS-US intervals were explained on the basis of hypothermia's temporally graded amnesitc properties. Evidence for conditioning at the longer intervals was discussed in relation to slowing down metabolism allowing for associations to be formed at CS-US intervals that normally do not result in evidence of conditioning. Manipulating body temperature during the CS-US interval was demonstrated to alter rats' ability to bridge temporal gaps in associative learning.  相似文献   

13.
The temporal reproduction of standard durations ranging from 1 to 9 seconds was investigated in monochannel cochlear implant (CI) users and in normally hearing subjects for the auditory and visual modality. The results showed that the pattern of performance in patients depended on their level of auditory comprehension. Results for CI users, who displayed relatively good auditory comprehension, did not differ from that of normally hearing subjects for both modalities. Patients with poor auditory comprehension significantly overestimated shorter auditory standards (1, 1.5 and 2.5 s), compared to both patients with good comprehension and controls. For the visual modality the between-group comparisons were not significant. These deficits in the reproduction of auditory standards were explained in accordance with both the attentional-gate model and the role of working memory in prospective time judgment. The impairments described above can influence the functioning of the temporal integration mechanism that is crucial for auditory speech comprehension on the level of words and phrases. We postulate that the deficits in time reproduction of short standards may be one of the possible reasons for poor speech understanding in monochannel CI users.  相似文献   

14.
Yuan J  He Y  Qinglin Z  Chen A  Li H 《Psychophysiology》2008,45(6):986-993
The inhibition of inappropriate behaviors is important for adaptive living in changing environments. The present study investigated gender-related behavioral inhibitory control by recording event-related potentials for standard and deviant stimuli while subjects performed a standard/deviant distinction task by accurately pressing different keys within 1000 ms. The results showed faster reaction times (RTs) for deviant stimuli in women than in men, although RTs for standard stimuli were similar across genders. There were significant gender and stimulus interaction effects on mean amplitudes during each of the 170–230-ms, 250–330-ms, and 350–600-ms intervals, and women exhibited shorter latencies and larger amplitudes than men at deviant-related P2, N2, and P3 components. As an accurate, fast response to the rare deviant stimuli involves behavioral inhibitory control on the prepotent response whereas the response to the standard stimuli does not, it is clear that there is a general gender difference in behavioral control for human adults. This may relate to differential inhibitory demands by each gender during evolution.  相似文献   

15.
Event-Related Potentials to Different Feedback Stimuli   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded in a time estimation task under different feedback conditions, in which the informative value of the feedback signals (true versus false) was manipulated. A control condition was added in which no signal was presented. Fifteen subjects pressed a button 3 seconds after presentation of a warning signal. Two seconds after the response, a visual feedback signal was presented, indicating whether the preceding interval was estimated correctly. Two different slow waves were observed: the response was preceded by a readiness potential and the feedback stimulus was preceded by a negative slow wave called the stimulus-preceding negativity. The readiness potential was not influenced by the different feedback conditions. The stimulus-preceding negativity was larger in the true feedback condition compared to the false feedback and no feedback conditions. The P300 to the feedback signal was also larger following a true as compared to a false feedback signal. The conclusion is that the stimulus-preceding negativity is an anticipatory component contingent upon the presentation of an informative feedback signal.  相似文献   

16.
Sensory stimulation triggers spindles during sleep stage 2   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Sato Y  Fukuoka Y  Minamitani H  Honda K 《Sleep》2007,30(4):511-518
STUDY OBJECTIVES: Toward understanding the function of sleep spindle, we examined whether sensory stimulation triggers sleep spindles. PARTICIPANTS: Eleven normal subjects participated in the experiments. INTERVENTION: The subjects had a nap in the afternoon, and sensory stimulation was applied during sleep stage 2. MEASUREMENTS: 21-channel EEG was recorded during the 2-3 hour experiment carried out between 13:00 and 16:00. Somatosensory, auditory, or visual stimulation was performed over a 5-minute period during stage 2. The frequency and duration of spindles were compared in 2 different segments of 5 minutes, with and without sensory stimulation. The latency from the onset of a sensory stimulus to the succeeding spindle was also analyzed. To estimate the active brain regions during a spindle, the EEG recordings were modeled with a single equivalent moving dipole (SEMD) model. RESULTS: In the period with stimulation, spindle frequency and duration increased compared with the period without stimulation. Statistical tests revealed that with stimulation, the interval between 2 consecutive spindles was significantly shorter (p < 0.05, regardless of the modality) and that the duration of the spindles was significantly longer with stimulation (p < 0.05, regardless of the modality). The latency was approximately 2 s. During a spindle after somatosensory stimulation brain activities were observed near the somatosensory area, while with auditory stimulation active regions were observed near the auditory cortex. CONCLUSIONS: A sensory stimulus appeared to trigger a sleep spindle during sleep stage 2. SEMD trajectories suggest that active brain regions during spindle are different according to the modality of the preceding stimulus.  相似文献   

17.
Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded from 26 young adults, with equal numbers of male and female subjects, using attended and ignored, olfactory and trigeminal stimuli. The amplitudes and latencies of the N1, P2, and P3 components were recorded using a single-stimulus paradigm, with an inter-stimulus interval of 60 s, employing amyl acetate as the olfactory stimulus and ammonia as the trigeminal stimulus. Subjects estimated stimulus intensity in the attend condition or continued with a visual tracking task in the ignore condition. Results indicate that olfactory information is processed 30-70 ms faster than trigeminal information for the N1 and P2 potential and 100 ms faster for the P3 ERP component. N1/P2 interpeak amplitude was greater for the trigeminal than the olfactory stimuli, and greater in the attended than ignored condition. P3 amplitude was greater in the attend than ignore condition for olfactory information processing and equivalent for trigeminal information processing. These findings suggest that neuronal resource allocation is greatest for attended stimuli and that a painful stimulus demands neuronal resources even when ignored.  相似文献   

18.
The hypothesis that time perception is determined by an internal clock's rate, which is sensitive to physiological circadian changes was examined. This hypothesis suggests that when body temperature and activity increase, clock rate increases causing overestimations of time and a decrease in time interval production. The following experiments examine this hypothesis with rats. Since rats are nocturnal, and body temperature is highest at night, time estimation should be longer and time production shorter than during the day. All experiments were run 24 hr a day under a 12:12 light:dark cycle. In the first experiment, rats discriminated among stimulus durations. Consistent with the hypothesis, both rats reliably judged standard durations as "longer" during the night than during the day. In Experiment 2, fixed-interval (FI) schedules of 60, 120 and 180 sec were used to examine circadian variations in time production. All seven rats produced longer postreinforcement pause (PRP) durations as FI length increased, and shorter PRP durations at night than during the day. However, absolute differences in PRP durations between day and night did not increase as FI duration increased, suggesting the PRP durations alone do not directly characterize proportional changes in internal clock rate. To more directly examine circadian variation in clock rate and the production of shorter intervals (i.e., between 1 and 16 sec) Experiment 3 used a lever holding procedure. Since this procedure requires the measured lever hold duration to meet or exceed the required hold duration, it more accurately characterizes the timing process.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

19.
探讨大脑对时间信息的处理过程,考察了大脑在由声音标记的时间间隔长短感知过程中的事件相关电位变化,从电生理角度揭示出大脑对时间这一信息的处理过程。实验包括无任务和间隔长短辨识任务,间隔长短辨识任务要求受试者在两种时间间隔(四个声音信号)结束后进行长短判断。结果发现无任务状态下各声音信号诱发的听觉诱发电位与正常的听觉诱发电位无显著性差异。而间隔长短辨识任务的结果表明,时间间隔判别任务中待判别间隔(第二个间隔)较给定间隔(第一个间隔)短时,在两间隔结束时即第四个声音信号的诱发电位与无任务实验的结果出现显著变化,在200ms-300ms左右诱发出一个明显的正峰,称其为P250(因峰值出现在250ms左右),同时,此声音诱发的P2显著被抑制,甚至消失;但是待判别间隔较给定间隔长时,则无显著性差异,原因在于在较长的第四个声音信号结束之前大脑已经判断完毕并作出了决策,从而无法诱发出P250。提示大脑对时间信息的比较判断引发了一个新的电位P250。  相似文献   

20.
An experiment was performed to test the effect of changing the CS-US interval on the pattern of conditioned salivation. Each of 6 subjects (Ss) received 60 reinforced trials on each of 3 CS-US intervals: 10, 20, and 40 sec. For each CS-US interval 10 test trials were given to determine the change in pattern of response with change in the CS-US interval. An additional session included 2 test trials in which an extraneous stimulus was given simultaneously with the CS. The results showed little evidence of inhibition of delay in the longer intervals or of disinhibition when the extraneous stimulus was applied. The most significant result was a response which followed CS offset in the 20- and 40-sec intervals. The question of the nature of this response remains to be answered by use of a different experimental design. Significance of the level of conditioning remained high (p <. 00001) over the manipulation of CS-US interval.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号