首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
We studied the direction of the three-dimensional fingertip force vector in young and old adults during a simple pressing task with the index finger. Ten young and ten old subjects pressed against a force plate with their index finger and maintained target forces that ranged from 2.5 to 15 N. Subjects viewed a display of the force normal to the force plate; forces tangential to the force plate were not displayed. Young adults produced fingertip forces that were nearly perpendicular to the plate at all target forces while old adults produced fingertip forces that deviated in the proximal direction of the horizontal plane, and ulnar direction of the vertical plane. The fingertip force deviated from the onset of pressing in both groups, but in young subjects the force aligned to perpendicular within 200 ms. Additional study is required to determine if these biased force vector directions contribute to clumsiness and slowing that are characteristic of fine manipulation in old age.  相似文献   

2.
Electrical stimulation of the digital nerves can cause short- and long-latency increases in electromyographic activity (EMG) of the hand muscles, but mechanical stimulation of primarily tactile afferents in the digits generally evokes only a long-latency increase in EMG. To examine whether such stimuli can elicit short-latency reflex responses, we recorded EMG over the first dorsal interosseous muscle when subjects (n=13) used the tip of the right index finger to restrain a horizontally oriented plate from moving when very brisk tangential forces were applied in the distal direction. The plate was subjected to ramp-and-hold pulling loads at two intensities (a 1-N load applied at 32 N/s or a 2-N load applied at 64 N/s) at times unpredictable to the subjects (mean interval 2 s; trial duration 500 ms). The contact surface of the manipulandum was covered with rayon—a slippery material. For each load, EMG was averaged for 128 consecutive trials with reference to the ramp onset. In all subjects, an automatic increase in grip force was triggered by the loads applied at 32 N/s; the mean onset latency of the EMG response was 59.8±0.9 (mean ± SE) ms. In seven subjects (54%) this long-latency response was preceded by a weak short-latency excitation at 34.6±2.9 ms. With the loads applied at 64 N/s, the long-latency response occurred slightly earlier (58.9±1.7 ms) and, with one exception, all subjects generated a short-latency EMG response (34.9±1.3 ms). Despite the higher background grip force that subjects adopted during the stronger loads (4.9±0.3 N vs 2.5±0.2 N), the incidence of slips was higher—the manipulandum escaped from the grasp in 37±5% of trials with the 64 N/s ramps, but in only 18±4% with the 32-N/s ramps. The deformation of the fingertip caused by the tangential load, rather than incipient or overt slips, triggered the short-latency responses because such responses occurred even when the finger pad was fixed to the manipulandum with double-sided adhesive tape so that no slips occurred.  相似文献   

3.
Old age impairs the ability to form new associations for declarative memory, but the ability to acquire and retain procedural memories remains relatively intact. Thus, it is unclear whether old age affects the ability to learn the visuomotor associations needed to set efficient fingertip forces for handling familiar objects. We studied the ability for human subjects to use visual cues (color) about the mechanical properties (texture or weight) of a grasped object to control fingertip forces during prehension. Old and young adults (mean age 77 years and 22 years, respectively) grasped and lifted an object that varied in texture at the gripped surfaces (experiment 1: sandpaper versus acetate surface materials) or weight (experiment 2: 200 g versus 400 g). The object was color-coded according to the mechanical property in the "visual cue" condition, and the mechanical property varied unpredictably across lifts in the "no visual cue" condition. In experiment 1 (texture), the young adults' grip force (force normal to the gripped surface) when the object lifted from the support surface was 24% smaller when the surfaces were color-coded. The old adults' grip force did not vary between the visual conditions despite their accurate reports of the grip surface colors prior to each lift. Comparable findings were obtained in experiment 2, when object weight was varied and peak grip force rate prior to object lift-off was measured. Furthermore old and young subjects alike used about 2 N of grip force when lifting the 200 g object in experiment 2. Therefore, the old adults' failure to adjust grip force when the color cue was present cannot be attributed to a general inability or unwillingness to use low grip force when handling objects. We conclude that old age affects the associative learning that links visual identification of an object with the fingertip forces for efficiently handling the object. In contrast, old and young subjects' grip force was influenced by the preceding lift, regardless of visual cues, which supports existing theories that multiple internal representations govern predictive control of fingertip forces during prehension.  相似文献   

4.
Summary In the previous paper regarding the somatosensory control of the human precision grip, we concluded that the elicited automatic grip force adjustments are graded by the amplitude of the imposed loads when restraining an active object subjected to unpredictable pulling forces (Johansson et al. 1992a). Using the same subjects and apparatus, the present study examines the capacity to respond to imposed load forces applied at various rates. Grip and load forces (forces normal and tangential to the grip surfaces) and the position of the object in the pulling direction (distal) were recorded. Trapezoidal load force profiles with plateau amplitudes of 2 N were delivered at the following rates of loading and unloading in an unpredictable sequence: 2 N/s, 4 N/s or 8 N/s. In addition, trials with higher load rate (32 N/s) at a low amplitude (0.7 N) were intermingled. The latencies between the start of the loading and the onset of the grip force response increased with decreasing load force rate. They were 80±9ms, 108 ±13ms, 138 ± 27 ms and 174 ± 39 ms for the 32, 8, 4 and 2 N/s rates, respectively. These data suggested that the grip response was elicited after a given minimum latency once a load amplitude threshold was exceeded. The amplitude of the initial rapid increase of grip force (i.e., the catch-up response) was scaled by the rate of the load force, whereas its time course was similar for all load rates. This response was thus elicited as a unit, but its amplitude was graded by afferent information about the load rate arising very early during the loading. The scaling of the catch-up response was purposeful since it facilitated a rapid reconciliation of the ratio between the grip and load force to prevent slips. In that sense it apparently also compensated for the varying delays between the loading phase and the resultant grip force responses. However, modification of the catch-up response may occur during its course when the loading rate is altered prior to the grip force response or very early during the catch-up response itself. Hence, afferent information may be utilized continuously in updating the response although its motor expression may be confined to certain time contingencies. Moreover, this updating may take place after an extremely short latency (45–50 ms). Our findings support the idea that the initiation as well as ongoing regulation of the motor responses is dependent on supraspinal control, but afferent signals directly processed through fast segmental networks may also contribute in the regulation. The grip force responses to the unloading phases, they were also graded by the load force rate and the response latencies increased with decreasing load force rate. However, the latencies were longer and more variable, and no catch-up responses were observed. Rather, the grip force decline was programmed for the inter-trial grip force level.  相似文献   

5.
The purpose of this study was to determine the effect of age on the ability to exert steady forces and to perform steady flexion movements with the muscles that cross the elbow joint. An isometric task required subjects to exert a steady force to match a target force that was displayed on a monitor. An anisometric task required subjects to raise and lower inertial loads so that the angular displacement around the elbow joint matched a template displayed on a monitor. Steadiness was measured as the coefficient of variation of force and as the normalized standard deviation of wrist acceleration. For the isometric task, steadiness as a function of target force decreased similarly for old adults and young adults. For the anisometric task, steadiness increased as a function of the inertial load and there were significant differences caused by age. Old adults were less steady than young adults during both shortening and lengthening contractions with the lightest loads. Furthermore, old adults were least steady when performing lengthening contractions. These behaviors appear to be associated with the patterns of muscle activation. These results suggest that different neural strategies are used to control isometric and anisometric contractions performed with the elbow flexor muscles and that these strategies do not change in parallel with advancing age.  相似文献   

6.
When an object held by a precision grip is subjected to an abrupt vertical load perturbation, somatosensory input from the digits triggers an increase in grip force to restore an adequate safety margin, preventing frictional slips. In adults the response occurs after a latency of 60–80 ms. In the present study, children from 2 years old upward and adults grasped and lifted an object using a precision grip. Sudden, unpredicted increases in load force (tangential to the grip surfaces) were induced by the experimenter by dropping a small disc on to a receptacle attached to the object. The impact elicited a grip force response which in young children had a longer latency and a smaller amplitude than was seen in adults. The grip response latency gradually become shorter and its amplitude increased with increasing age, reaching adult values at 6–10 years. The muscle activity underlying the response could have several bursts. The adults showed one brisk response, appearing 40–50 ms after impact, in extrinsic and intrinsic hand muscles, while younger children also exhibited a short-latency burst, appearing about 20 ms after impact. It is suggested that the short-latency response was mediated via spinal pathways, and that these pathways are disengaged by supraspinal centers during development. In a predictable loading situation, when subjects dropped the disc themselves into the receptacle using the contralateral hand, they changed strategy. Adults induced a well-timed anticipatory grip force increase prior to the impact that was scaled to the weight of the object. The youngest children did not time the force increase properly in relation to the impact. Yet, they could scale their anticipatory grip force increase with respect to the weight of the dropped disc. This suggests a well-developed capacity to use information about the weight of objects held by one hand to parameterize a programmed force output to the other hand.  相似文献   

7.
Cortical auditory evoked potentials (N1 wave) were studied in 24 adults (12 men, 12 women) and 20 children (12 boys, 8 girls; age: 4-8 years). In adults, this wave was recorded with maximal amplitude at frontocentral sites, peaking at about 100 ms poststimulation, whereas in children the auditory response displayed maximal amplitude at the midtemporal sites, with a positive wave at about 100 ms and a large negative wave at approximately 170 ms. Moreover, the modulatory effects of intensity on N1 amplitude were prominent at frontocentral sites in adults and at temporal sites in children. Frontocentral negative response was also recorded in children but was smaller in amplitude and longer in peak latency (around 140 ms) than in adults; responses were of greater amplitude at the frontal site than at the vertex before 6 years of age, whereas the reverse was more often found after this age. These data suggest great differences with age in the neural generators contributing to auditory evoked potentials recorded in the N1 latency range.  相似文献   

8.
The purpose of this study was to compare the discharge characteristics of motor units recruited during an isometric contraction that was sustained with the elbow flexor muscles by older adults at target forces that were less than the recruitment threshold force of each isolated motor unit. The discharge times of 27 single motor units were recorded from the biceps brachii in 11 old adults (78.8 ± 5.9 yr). The target force was set at either a relatively small (6.6 ± 3.7% maximum) or large (11.4 ± 4.5% maximum) difference below the recruitment threshold force and the contraction was sustained until the motor unit was recruited and discharged action potentials for about 60 s. The time to recruitment was longer for the large target-force difference (P = 0.001). At recruitment, the motor units discharged repetitively for both target-force differences, which contrasts with data from young adults when motor units discharged intermittently at recruitment for the large difference between recruitment threshold force and target force. The coefficient of variation (CV) for the first five interspike intervals (ISIs) increased from the small (18.7 ± 7.9) to large difference (35.0 ± 10.2%, P = 0.008) for the young adults, but did not differ for the two target force differences for the old adults (26.3 ± 14.7 to 24.0 ± 13.1%, P = 0.610). When analyzed across the discharge duration, the average CV for the ISI decreased similarly for the two target-force differences (P = 0.618) in old adults. These findings contrast with those of young adults and indicate that the integration of synaptic input during sustained contractions differs between young and old adults.  相似文献   

9.
The task of sliding a nut from a rod has been used to study manual slowing in old age (Smith et al. in Neurology 53:1458–1461, 1999; Neurobiol Aging 26:883–890, 2005). In this experiment, we sought to determine if the age-related slowing in this task occurs with losses of motor precision, as indicated by the forces exerted on the rod. The forces exerted by the nut on the rod were monitored along with the kinematics of the hand in old and young adults while they attempted to lift a nut from three vertically oriented rods of different shape (straight, single curve, double curve). Old adults performed the task 64% slower than young adults for the straight rod, 100% slower for the single-curve rod, and 80% slower for the double-curve rod. Old adults did not differ from the young adults in the amount of force exerted against the rods in the horizontal plane, or in the steadiness of these forces, but exerted greater force impulses in the vertical direction over the course of a trial (359% straight, 236% single curve, 214% double curve) and much more force in the vertical direction (255% straight, 267% single curve, 159% double curve). Old adults also performed the task with 35% greater average roll of the hand into pronation. We suspect that old adults tilted the nut, even for the straight rod, dragging it against the rod to create the elevated vertical forces. These observations support previous speculation that old adults do not control the external moments applied to grasped objects as well as young adults.  相似文献   

10.
The reliability of ERP measures was investigated in a sample covering the adult life span (n = 59, age 21-92). This sample was divided into a young and an old group. ERPs to an auditory two-stimuli oddball task were recorded in the sample at two occasions separated by 12-14 months (T1 and T2). The recordings of T1 were split in half, to assess within session reliability. Correlations were calculated for N1, P2 and P3 peak latency and amplitude, for average amplitude during 50 ms epochs within the defined P3-window 250-550, and for average amplitude in successive 15 ms epochs from 1 to 705 ms. The results show that amplitude measures were more reliable than the latency measures at all electrodes. Time window/epoch amplitude measures yielded reliabilities in the same range as peak amplitude. Reliabilities peaked around the conventionally studied N1, P2 and P3, and this is seen as a validation of the components. In general, the old group exhibited weaker P3 peak latency reliabilities than the young group. However, many of these differences did not reach statistical significance. Implications of the findings are discussed.  相似文献   

11.
Qiu X  Yang X  Qiao Z  Wang L  Ning N  Shi J  Zhao L  Yang Y 《Neuroscience letters》2011,487(1):53-57
Age-related declines in central processing may delay the facilitation of corticospinal (CS) tracts that underlie emergence of voluntary responses to external stimuli. To explore this effect, single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was applied to the left motor cortex at different latencies from the go-signal (auditory tone) during a simple reaction time (SRT) task with the right or left thumb [i.e. right (RHM) or left hand move (LHM)]. Motor evoked potentials (MEPs) in the right abductor pollicis brevis (APB) were recorded from eleven healthy right-handed participants (aged 22-65; six young adults and five old adults). Both age groups showed significant facilitation of CS excitability approximately 100-120 ms from the onset of the go-signal in the RHM SRT that occurred before the onset of EMG voluntary burst, with no evidence for motor slowing in old adults. Old adults demonstrated a significant facilitation of MEPs in the time that preceded the go-signal for RHM SRT and a marked depression of CS excitability in preparation for the LHM SRT that was sustained up to 80 ms after the onset of the go-signal. Both effects were not seen in young adults. While the small number of participants may hinder the generality of the present observations, this pilot study suggests for the first time that old adults implemented selective tuning of CS excitability prior to the onset of the go command to speed up their response generation.  相似文献   

12.
The purpose of this study was to investigate the change in the fingertip forces and balance control of young adults and older adults. The subjects lifted an object of constant weight (i.e., 1500 g) using their right hand, first in a seated position and then in a standing position. We quantified the ability of the participants to adjust their fingertip forces across trials by comparing the percentage of change in the peak grip force, peak load force and the ratio between peak grip force and peak load force. Moreover, we quantified their ability to stabilize their balance following the lifting of the object in the standing condition. The results showed that in both conditions young adults reduced their peak grip force much more than older adults across trials. In the seated condition, young adults increased slightly their peak load force, across trials, while older adults reduced it. In the standing condition, both groups showed similar change in peak load force across trials. Remarkably, older adults improved their balance stability similarly to young adults in the standing condition. This observation suggests that the ability of the older adults to modulate grip force applied to an object while standing is diminished probably to dedicate more attention to the balance control task rather than fine-tuning the grip force. Reducing balance instability following repetitive lifting is certainly more beneficial as the consequences of a fall could be more dramatic than dropping a cup of coffee.  相似文献   

13.
Maturation of Startle Modulation   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
This study of the maturation of prestimulation-induced modulation of startle in 3 to 8 year old children and adults demonstrated significant effects of age on both startle magnitude and onset latency. Startle was evoked by 104dB(SPL) 50-ms bursts of white noise, and the amplitude and onset latency of the blink reflex were measured after integration of the obicularis oculi EMG. Prestimulation with 75dB 1000 Hz tones resulted in severe inhibition of both amplitude and latency in adults when 20-ms tones preceded the startling stimuli by 120 ms or 250 ms. Following sustained prestimulation for 2000 ms, the adults showed modest nonsignificant response facilitation. Eight-year-old children showed mature inhibitory and facilitatory startle amplitude modulation, but significantly less inhibition and more facilitation of onset latency compared to adults. Preschool children showed significantly less amplitude and latency inhibition and more facilitation than 8-year-olds and adults. In response to prestimulation 120 ms before startling stimuli, the preschool children actually showed latency facilitation. Modulation of startle by prestimulation is mediated by brainstem neuronal networks. These findings suggest that brainstem mechanisms which mediate startle response modulation undergo development during early childhood and do not mature until about 8 years of age.  相似文献   

14.
The effects of responding hand and stimulus probability were investigated in young and old subjects in an RT task in which both rare and frequent stimuli required a response. It was found that the effect of stimulus probability was less pronounced in old subjects than in young, and that the latency of P3 was longer in the elderly, although their RTs were not different from young subjects. ERPs for right hand responses were larger than for left hand responses; this difference was discernible already in the P2 and N2 peaks of the ERP. A tentative explanation is offered for these large and unexpected hand differences. An interpretation in terms of an age-related decrease in resources is proposed for the increased P3 latency and the decreased probability effect on P3 amplitude in old subjects.  相似文献   

15.
Motor-unit synchronization, which is a measure of the near simultaneous discharge of action potentials by motor units, has the potential to influence spike-triggered average force and the steadiness of a low-force isometric contraction. The purpose of the study was to estimate the contribution of motor-unit synchronization to the larger spike-triggered average forces and the decreased steadiness exhibited by old adults. Eleven young (age 19-30 yr) and 14 old (age 63-81 yr) adults participated in the study. Motor-unit activity was recorded with two fine-wire intramuscular electrodes in the first dorsal interosseus muscle during isometric contractions that caused the index finger to exert an abduction force. In a separate session, steadiness measurements were obtained during constant-force isometric contractions at target forces of 2.5, 5, 7. 5, and 10% of the maximum voluntary contraction (MVC) force. Mean (+/-SD) motor-unit forces measured by spike-triggered averaging were larger in old (15.5 +/- 12.1 mN) compared with young (7.3 +/- 5.7 mN) adults, and the differences were more pronounced between young (8.7 +/- 6.4 mN) and old (19.9 +/- 12.2 mN) men. Furthermore, the old adults had a reduced ability to maintain a steady force during an isometric contraction, particularly at low target forces (2.5 and 5% MVC). Mean (+/-SD) motor-unit synchronization, expressed as the frequency of extra synchronous discharges above chance in the cross-correlogram, was similar in young [0.66 +/- 0.4 impulses/s (imp/s); range, 0.35-1.51 imp/s; 53 pairs) and old adults (0.72 +/- 0.5 imp/s; range, 0.27-1.38 imp/s; 56 pairs). The duration of synchronous peaks in the cross-correlogram was similar for each group (approximately 16 ms). These data suggest that motor-unit synchronization is not responsible for larger spike-triggered average forces in old adults and that motor-unit synchronization does not contribute to the decreased steadiness of low-force isometric contractions observed in old adults.  相似文献   

16.
1. Brief increases or decreases in vertical load force were applied to an object held between the thumb and finger. Grip force increases occurred consistently from 60 to 90 ms after onset of the load force increase. These responses did not adapt and were typically from 100 to 200 ms in duration. Reductions in object load force yielded rapid reductions in grip force at latencies comparable to those for load increases. 2. Response magnitude was proportional to the size or velocity of the load force increment, but did not vary with the level of the preexisting grip force. Thus these responses did not maintain the grip force at a specified level above the object's slip point. 3. Grip force responses were abolished or substantially reduced when loads were delivered directly to the hand rather than to the object. In contrast, force responses were not always abolished upon anesthetization of the thumb and finger. These results are discussed in relation to the role of cutaneous mechano-receptors of the digital pulps and proprioceptors of the arm and hand for providing necessary afferent information utilized in load-related grip force modulation. 4. Rapid and automatic grip force adjustments to load force variations may contribute importantly to grasp tasks in which the load forces vary dynamically and without complete predictability, such as in the manipulation of tools or objects that contact the environment.  相似文献   

17.
In order to explore the neural correlates of age-related changes in visual perception of faces, positron emission tomographic scans were obtained on young and old adults while they were engaged in tasks of nondegraded and degraded face matching. Old adults were less accurate than were young adults across all face matching conditions, although the age difference was greatly reduced when degraded performance was adjusted for nondegraded performance. The interaction of age and degree of degradation on performance measures was not significant. Brain activity patterns during nondegraded face matching were similar in the two groups with some differences in parietal and prestriate cortices (greater activity in young adults) and in prefrontal cortex, thalamus, and hippocampus (greater activity in old adults). Increases in activity related to increasing degradation of the faces were seen mainly in prefrontal cortices in both age groups. Despite this similarity in the brain response to face degradation, there were striking differences between groups in the correlations between brain activity and degraded task performance. Different regions of extrastriate cortex were positively correlated with behavioural measures in the two groups (fusiform gyrus in the young adults and posterior occipital regions in old adults). In addition two areas where older adults showed greater activity during nondegraded face matching, thalamus and hippocampus, also showed positive correlations with behaviour during the degraded tasks in this group, but not in the young group. Thus, although the elderly are not more vulnerable to the effects of increasing face degradation, the brain systems involved in carrying out these visual discriminations in young and old adults are not the same. These results are consistent with the idea of functional plasticity in face processing over the life span.  相似文献   

18.
In order to explore the neural correlates of age-related changes in visual perception of faces, positron emission tomographic scans were obtained on young and old adults while they were engaged in tasks of nondegraded and degraded face matching. Old adults were less accurate than were young adults across all face matching conditions, although the age difference was greatly reduced when degraded performance was adjusted for nondegraded performance. The interaction of age and degree of degradation on performance measures was not significant. Brain activity patterns during nondegraded face matching were similar in the two groups with some differences in parietal and prestriate cortices (greater activity in young adults) and in prefrontal cortex, thalamus, and hippocampus (greater activity in old adults). Increases in activity related to increasing degradation of the faces were seen mainly in prefrontal cortices in both age groups. Despite this similarity in the brain response to face degradation, there were striking differences between groups in the correlations between brain activity and degraded task performance. Different regions of extrastriate cortex were positively correlated with behavioural measures in the two groups (fusiform gyrus in the young adults and posterior occipital regions in old adults). In addition two areas where older adults showed greater activity during nondegraded face matching, thalamus and hippocampus, also showed positive correlations with behaviour during the degraded tasks in this group, but not in the young group. Thus, although the elderly are not more vulnerable to the effects of increasing face degradation, the brain systems involved in carrying out these visual discriminations in young and old adults are not the same. These results are consistent with the idea of functional plasticity in face processing over the life span.  相似文献   

19.
The purpose of this study was to quantify correlated motor unit activity during isometric, shortening and lengthening contractions of a hand muscle in older adults. Thirteen old subjects (69.6±5.9 years, six women) lifted and lowered a light load with abduction–adduction movements of the index finger over 10° using 6-s shortening and lengthening contractions of the first dorsal interosseus muscle. The task was repeated 10–20 times while activity in 23 pairs of motor units was recorded with intramuscular electrodes. The data were compared with 23 motor-unit pairs in 15 young (25.9±4.6 years, five women) subjects obtained using a similar protocol in a previous study. Correlated motor unit activity was quantified using time-domain (synchronization index; Common Input Strength) and frequency-domain (coherence) analyses for the same motor-unit pairs. For all contractions, there was no difference with age for the strength of motor-unit synchronization, although age-related differences were observed for synchronous peak widths (young, 17.6±7.4 ms; old, 13.7±4.9 ms) and motor-unit coherence at 6–9 Hz (z score for young, 3.0±1.8; old, 2.2±1.5). Despite increased synchrony during lengthening contractions and narrower peak widths for shortening contractions in young subjects, there was no difference in the strength of motor unit synchronization (CIS ~0.8 imp/s), or the width of the synchronous peak (~14 ms) during the three tasks in old subjects. Furthermore, no significant differences in motor-unit coherence were observed between tasks at any frequency for old adults. These data suggest that the strategy used by the central nervous system to control isometric, shortening, and lengthening contractions varies in young adults, but not old adults. The diminished task-related adjustments of common inputs to motor neurons are a likely consequence of the neural adaptations that occur with advancing age.  相似文献   

20.
The purpose of the study was to determine the practice-induced adjustments in the motor-output variability and the agonist-antagonist activity that accompanied improvements in endpoint accuracy of goal-directed isometric contractions in young and old adults. Young and old adults performed 100 trials that involved accurately matching the peak of a force trajectory (25% maximum) to a target force in 150 ms. Endpoint accuracy was quantified as the absolute difference between the target and the peak force and time-to-peak force. Motor-output variability was expressed as the SDs of the force trajectory, peak force, and time-to-peak force. The force and time errors differed between the two groups initially, but after 35 practice trials the errors were similar for the two groups. Reductions in force endpoint error were predicted by decreases in the variability of the force trajectory for both groups, adaptations in the agonist (first dorsal interosseus) and antagonist (second palmar interosseus) EMG for young adults, and adaptations only for the agonist EMG for old adults. Reductions in time endpoint error were predicted by increases in the SD of time-to-peak force and a longer delay to the peak EMG of the antagonist muscle for young adults, but by decreases in the SDs of time-to-peak force and force trajectory and a shorter delay to the peak EMG of the antagonist muscle for the old adults. The findings indicate that the neural adjustments underlying the improvement in endpoint accuracy with practice differed for young and old adults.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号