首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 109 毫秒
1.
The purpose of this study was to examine the role of the external lateral parabrachial subnucleus (PBNLe) in two different taste aversion learning (TAL) procedures. For the first, short-term (concurrent) TAL, two different-flavored stimuli were presented at the same time, one associated with simultaneous intragastric administration of an aversive product, hypertonic NaCl, and the other with saline. In the second, long-term (sequential/delayed) TAL, each gustatory stimulus was presented every other day and the intragastric products LiCl and saline were administered after a 15-min delay. Electrolytic lesions in the PBNLe blocked acquisition of concurrent TAL, in which the vagal visceral information is critical. But the same lesions failed to interrupt sequential TAL. This result was independent of the order in which the two tasks (concurrent and sequential) were presented. However, as found by other authors, the latter type of learning was impaired in the presence of larger lesions in this same area. This supports the existence of sensory information needed to establish sequential TAL in other subnuclei of the parabrachial complex. The results of these experiments suggest that the different modalities of TAL are anatomically specific.  相似文献   

2.
Summary An identifiable histamine-containing neuron is located on the anterior dorsal surface of the visceral ganglion ofLymnaea stagnalis L. After [3H]histamine was injected into the perikaryon of this neuron, labelled axonal ramifications were seen in the neuropils of all ganglia except the pedals and right cerebral, and labelled axons occurred in seven nerve trunks. Electron microscopical examination of the perikaryon of the histamine neuron revealed the presence of large aggregations of granulated vesicles (80–200 m diameter), elaborate endoplasmic reticulum, and numerous mitochondria, Golgi complexes and lysosome-like organelles.  相似文献   

3.
The internal defence system (immune system) of the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis is reviewed. Humoral defence activities are agglutination, opsonisation and inhibition of bacterial growth. Cellular defence is exerted by antigen trapping endothelial cells, foreign protein engulfing pore cells, phagocytic reticulum cells and mobile haemocytes. The haemocytes contribute most to defence and are therefore treated in more detail. There is one type of haemocyte; morphological heterogeneity of the haemocyte population is due to varying states of differentiation of the cells. Haemocytopoiesis is through mitosis of haemocytes, both in the pool of tissue-dwelling cells and in circulation. Haemocytes resemble monocytes/macrophages, they are typical phagocytes equipped with recognition factors (lectins), lysosomal enzymes and a cytotoxicity mechanism using reative oxygen intermediates. A comparison is made of the internal defence systems of the three main metazoan taxa, insects, molluscs and vertebrates.Originally presented at ECCP 93.  相似文献   

4.
The unconditioned stimulus pre‐exposure effect (US‐PE) refers to the interference paradigm in which acquisition of the conditioned response is retarded due to prior experience with the US. Most studies analyzing the psychological mechanisms underlying this effect have been conducted with adult rats. The most widely accepted hypothesis explains this effect as a contextual blocking effect. Contextual cues associated with the US block the conditioned stimulus (CS)‐US association during conditioning. The modulatory role of a context devoid of distinctive olfactory attributes is not observable until approximately PD23 in rats, including modulation of interference paradigms such as latent inhibition or extinction. In this study, we analyzed US‐PE in preweanling rats along with the role of the training context in this effect in terms of conditioned taste aversion preparation. Pre‐exposure to LiCl before conditioning retarded the acquisition of taste aversion. The US‐PE was observed in preweanling rats when, during pre‐exposure, subjects were exposed to the conditioning context, and this effect was not attenuated either by the administration of the US in a familiar environment (Experiment 1a), or by the presence of an alternative, more salient context during pre‐exposure (Experiment 1b). Additionally, the US‐PE was still observed when the route by which the US was administered was changed between the pre‐exposure and conditioning phases (Experiment 2a) as well as when the injection cues were removed during conditioning (Experiment 2b). These experiments show a strong US‐PE in preweanling rats and fail to support the contextual blocking hypothesis, at least in this stage of ontogeny. © 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Dev Psychobiol 55: 193–204, 2013  相似文献   

5.
Pistell PJ  Zhu M  Ingram DK 《Neuroscience》2008,152(3):594-600
Research into the underlying mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease (AD) has relied traditionally on tasks such as the water maze which evaluate spatial learning and memory. Since non-spatial forms of memory are also disrupted by AD, it is critical to establish other paradigms capable of investigating these deficits. Utilizing a non-spatial learning task, acquisition of conditioned taste aversion (CTA) was evaluated in a mouse model of AD. This line of transgenic mice encode a mutated allele of the human amyloid precursor protein (APP) and presenilin 1 (PS1) genes and exhibit extensive amyloid plaque deposition in the brain by 6-7 mo of age. Compared with wild-type mice, 10-17 month old APP/PS1 mice failed to acquire CTA to saccharin. Mice that only possessed one of the two mutations were able to acquire CTA to the saccharin. In 2-5 month old APP/PS1 mice acquisition of CTA was disrupted despite the lack of extensive plaque deposition. However, further analysis indicated a potential gender difference in both the CTA deficit and onset of plaque deposition with females showing greater conditioned aversion.  相似文献   

6.
Kindling is a model in which an initially subconvulsive electrical stimulation of certain brain areas eventually develops a generalized seizure that produces behavioral and long term neuronal changes. In the present study we evaluated if kindling can modify conditioning taste aversion (CTA). In this paradigm animals acquire aversion to saccharin when it is presented as the conditioned stimulus (CS) followed by an injection of lithium chloride (LiCl) that induces a gastric irritation as the unconditioned stimulus (US). Male Wistar rats were implanted with bipolar electrodes aimed at the right amygdala (AMG) or at the right insular cortex (IC). The animals were stimulated daily until they reached stages 2-4 (intermediate) or until kindling was fully established (three consecutive stage 5 seizures). At least two weeks after kindling stimulation had ceased the animals were deprived of water for 24 h and given 10-min drinking sessions twice a day for 4 days. On day 5 (morning session) tap water was replaced by saccharin solution (0.1%), 20 min later the animals were injected with LiCl (7.5 ml/kg i.p., 0.2 M) to induce gastric malaise or taste aversion. After three more days of baseline consumption, water was substituted by a fresh 0.1% saccharin solution to test the aversion. AMG-kindling delayed the extinction of CTA. Animals with kindling in the IC had a higher retention than the sham kindling group; that is, they drank significantly less saccharin solution than the other groups. The results of the present experiment show that local modification of brain function induced by kindling stimulation can prolong the aversive effects of CTA.  相似文献   

7.
Adolescent-typical sensitivities to ethanol (EtOH) are characterized in part by reduced sensitivity to EtOH's aversive effects. Rodent studies have shown that adolescents are less sensitive than adults to aversive properties of EtOH in a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) paradigm. To the extent that EtOH exerts antagonist-like actions upon glutamate receptors and/or agonist-like actions upon γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptors, age differences in excitatory/inhibitory balance may regulate age-specific EtOH sensitivities, such as attenuated sensitivity of adolescents to EtOH aversion. In our experiments, adolescent and adult Sprague-Dawley rats were tested for CTA following challenge with one of the following pharmacological agents: glutamatergic AMPA1 receptor antagonist NBQX, glutamatergic N-methyl-d -aspartate NR2B receptor antagonist ifenprodil, and extrasynaptic GABAA receptor agonist THIP to determine whether these induced age-specific aversive sensitivities like those seen with EtOH. NBQX administration did not induce CTA. The highest dose of extrasynaptic GABAA agonist THIP induced CTA in adolescents but not adults, an opposite ontogenetic profile as seen following EtOH. Ifenprodil exerted an age-specific pattern of CTA similar to that seen with EtOH in males, with adolescents being insensitive to ifenprodil's aversive effects relative to adults. Thus, only antagonism of NR2B receptors in male rats mimicked age-specific sensitivities to the aversive effects of EtOH.  相似文献   

8.
Both corticotropin (ACTH) and testosterone prolong the extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in water-deprived intact male rats. An investigation was made to determine whether ACTH affects extinction in the absence of the testes and also to determine the effect of ACTH on serum testosterone levels. Water-deprived intact males showed prolonged extinction after ACTH injections; water-deprived gonadectomized males and intact females did not. All three of these groups showed elevated testosterone levels after ACTH administration, but testosterone levels were higher in the intact males than in the gonadectomized males or intact females. These results clearly show that in the absence of the testes ACTH is unable to prolong extinction. It is proposed that the increased level of testosterone following ACTH injection in water-deprived intact males is responsible for the prolonged extinction of a conditioned taste aversion. Although testosterone levels may increase in females and castrated males following ACTH injection, the increase is not sufficient to prolong extinction in these water-deprived animals.  相似文献   

9.
To study the role of lectins in the defence system of the snailLymnaea stagnalis, saccharides were tested for their capacity to inhibit plasma-mediated haemagglutination of trypsinised and fixed mouse red blood cells (M-rbc) and fixed rabbit red blood cells (R-rbc).d-Arabinose,N-acetylneuraminic acid, fucoidan andd-melezitose were good agglutination inhibitors using both types of rbc.d-Fructose, however, stimulated haemagglutination. Based on these results, in vitro phagocytosis assays were performed withd-arabinose (an agglutination inhibitor) andd-fructose (an agglutination stimulator). In co-incubation experiments both sugars inhibited adherence and phagocytosis of M-rbc in a concentration-dependent manner. Adherence and phagocytosis of R-rbc was stimulated at the lowest concentrations of D-arabinose and most concentrations ofd-fructose. Preincubation experiments showed decreased adherence and phagocytosis after preincubation of M-rbc with either sugar. Adherence and phagocytosis of R-rbc was reduced after preincubation of haemocytes withd-fructose. Preincubation of R-rbc had little effect. Our results support former studies indicating the presence of lectins both in the snail plasma and on the haemocytes.  相似文献   

10.
An enzyme cytochemical, light microscopy study was undertaken to evaluate the effect of haemolymph extraction on distribution of acid phosphatase, alkaline phosphatase, alpha-naphthyl acetate esterase, and peroxidase in haemocytes of Lymnaea stagnalis. The study revealed that discrete acid phosphatase, alkaline phosphatase, alpha-naphthyl acetate esterase, and peroxidase-staining subpopulations of haemocytes do exist in L. stagnalis, and that there are differences in distribution. A high percentage, about 96%, of haemocytes showed positive reaction for peroxidase, the percentage of haemocytes showing activity for alpha-naphthyl acetate esterase, acid phosphatase, and alkaline phosphatase, was about 54, 42, and 34, respectively. Haemolymph extraction by foot retraction, significantly affected enzyme distribution. Whereas distribution of acid phosphatase increased significantly from day 1 to 5 after extraction of haemolymph, that of all other enzymes showed significant decrease from day 1 to 7 depending upon the enzyme. The distribution of all enzymes stabilized by day 8 after extraction of haemolymph. Probable reasons for the observed fluctuations in the distribution of enzymes subsequent to haemolymph extraction are discussed.  相似文献   

11.
Central pattern generators (CPGs) are neuronal networks that control vitally important rhythmic behaviors including breathing, heartbeat, and digestion. Understanding how CPGs recover activity after their rhythmic activity is disrupted has important theoretical and practical implications. Previous experimental and modeling studies indicated that rhythm recovery after central neuromodulatory input loss (decentralization) could be based entirely on activity-dependent mechanisms, but recent evidence of long-term conductance regulation by neuromodulators suggest that neuromodulator-dependent mechanisms may also be involved. Here we examined the effects of altering activity and the neuromodulatory environment before decentralization of the pyloric CPG in Cancer borealis on the initial phase of rhythmic activity recovery after decentralization. We found that pretreatments altering the network activity through shifting the ionic balance or the membrane potential of pyloric pacemaker neurons reduced the delay of recovery initiation after decentralization, consistent with the recovery process being triggered already during the pretreatment period through an activity-dependent mechanism. However, we observed that pretreatment with neuromodulators GABA and proctolin, acting via metabotropic receptors, also affected the initial phase of the recovery of pyloric activity after decentralization. Their distinct effects appear to result from interactions of their metabotropic effects with their effects on neuronal activity. Thus we show that the initial phase of the recovery process can be accounted for by the existence of distinct activity-and neuromodulator-dependent pathways. We propose a computational model that includes activity- and neuromodulator-dependent mechanisms of the activity recovery process, which successfully explains the experimental observations and predicts the results of key biological experiments.  相似文献   

12.
Retrieving the memory of a conditioned taste aversion involves multiple forebrain areas. Although the amygdala clearly plays a role in the expression of a conditioned taste aversion, critical regions, downstream from the amygdala remain to be defined. To this end, Fos immunoreactivity was used in the rat to explore forebrain structures associated with retrieval that have an anatomical relationship with the amygdala. The results showed that expression of a conditioned taste aversion to 0.5 M sucrose elicited neuronal activation in the nucleus accumbens and in a complex of structures collectively referred to as the extended amygdala. The posterior hypothalamus and parasubthalamic nucleus, which receive inputs from the extended amygdala, were also activated upon re-exposure to the sucrose conditioned stimulus. Fos immunoreactivity did not increase in these regions in response to an innately aversive tastant, quinine hydrochloride (conditioned stimulus control), nor to LiCl-induced visceral stimulation in unconditioned animals (unconditioned stimulus control). In addition, these regions did not respond to the sucrose conditioned stimulus in sham-conditioned animals. These results suggest that conditioned and innately aversive tastes are differentially processed in the forebrain circuitry that includes the nucleus accumbens and extended amygdala.  相似文献   

13.
Subdiaphragmatic vagotomy suppresses food intake and water intake in normal rats. Since human patients report some nausea and discomfort following vagotomy, the present study assessed the aversive consequences of vagotomy in rats using a conditioned taste aversion paradigm. Rats were given a total subdiaphragmatic vagotomy or sham vagotomy, and were then maintained on either plain water (Vag-Water and Sham-Water groups) or a novel cherry solution (Vag-Cherry and Sham-Cherry groups). When subsequently tested for their water vs. cherry preferences on postoperative days 6, 16, and 26, the Vag-Cherry group displayed a greater aversion to the cherry solution than did the remaining three groups. This result suggests that vagotomy produces visceral malaise in rats which may contribute to the feeding and drinking suppressive effects of the surgery.  相似文献   

14.
Variations in the intertrial interval (ITI) between two taste-aversion conditioning trials indicated that more saccharin-aversion learning occurs with longer ITIs (Experiments 1, 2 and 4). Two conditioning trials separated by 35 min did not produce stronger taste aversions than a single saccharin-lithium pairing; however, stronger conditioning was evident when the two trials were administered with a 3-day intertrial interval. Independent evaluation of saccharin and lithium exposure as possible sources of proactive and retroactive interference suggested that poor conditioning at short ITIs occurs because lithium treatment during Trial 1 interferes with conditioning during Trial 2 (Experiment 2). Assessments of the unconditioned suppression of activity (Experiments 3) and novel-fluid intake (Experiment 4) indicated that drug treatment was effective during both conditioning trials regardless of the intertrial interval. Furthermore, with a 35-min ITI (but not with a 3-day ITI) drug treatment in Trial 1 persisted long enough to summate with the unconditioned responses to drug treatment in Trial 2. These findings are consistent with the idea that with short ITIs the lingering effects of the US from Trial 1 overshadow or mask the CS flavor in Trial 2, and this interferes with the conditioning that Trial 2 would otherwise produce.  相似文献   

15.
Independent groups of rats were compared drinking in response to either water deprivation or osmotic thirst induced by intraperitoneal injections of hypertonic saline. When water or a palatable saccharin solution served as the drinking fluid, deprivation and osmotic thirst produced comparable fluid intakes. In contrast, when a saccharin solution previously associated with the aversive effects of lithium served as the drinking fluid, animals injected with hypertonic saline drank substantially less than water deprived animals. Experiment 2 indicated that this hyperreactivity to a conditioned aversive flavor in animals suffering from osmotic thirst was due to the reduced palatability of the saccharin flavor rather than the previous experience with lithium. Experiment 3 showed that the effect also could not be attributed to differential taste-aversion learning, handling, food deprivation or weight loss before the test sessions. The phenomenon is discussed in terms of various differences between thirst induced by water deprivation and thirst induced by acute cellular dehydration.  相似文献   

16.
A conditioned taste aversion was produced by pairing a novel sucrose solution with either 3 mEq lithium chloride or with 100 rad gamma radiation in rats with the area postrema intact. Lesions of the area postrema were then made in half of the rats exposed to each treatment and in rats that were not treated with the unconditioned stimulus. When tested for a conditioned taste aversion, all treated rats showed a significant aversion to the sucrose solution compared to the untreated control rats. There were no significant differences between rats with area postrema lesions and those with the area postrema intact, indicating that the lesions had no effect on the recall of the previously acquired aversion. The results are interpreted as being consistent with the hypothesis that the role of the area postrema in taste aversion learning is to monitor blood and cerebrospinal fluid for potential toxins and to transmit that information to the central nervous system.  相似文献   

17.
Jejunoileal bypass (JIB) surgery reduces food intake and body weight in obese humans and rats. Human bypass patients report visceral discomfort following surgery, and the present study assessed the aversive consequences of JIB in rats using a conditioned taste aversion paradigm. In Experiment 1 rats given a cherry-flavored solution immediately after JIB surgery subsequently displayed a strong aversion to the cherry flavor compared to Bypass and Sham-Bypass control groups. Rats in Experiment 2 were familiarized with cherry solution prior to surgery and they did not display an aversion to the solution after receiving a JIB. In Experiment 3, Bypass rats who developed a cherry flavor aversion after JIB subsequently lost this aversion following reconnection of their intestinal tract. The rats in Experiment 4 displayed an aversion to a saccharin-flavored chow that was eaten shortly after JIB surgery, although the aversion was not as pronounced as that obtained with the cherry solution. The results suggest that JIB produces a persisting malaise in rats that may contribute to the feeding and weight inhibitory effects of the operation.  相似文献   

18.
Studies were carried out on male and female rats in which the effects of isolated presentation of a conditioned stimulus (a saccharine solution) to which the animals had previously developed conditioned reflex taste aversion (RTA) on the level of urinary catecholamine secretion were determined. The studies showed that presentation of an aversive taste stimulus without reinforcement by a negative stimulus increased the levels of urinary adrenaline, noradrenaline, and dopamine secretion; this repeated, albeit more weakly, the effects of the negative reinforcement (angular acceleration) used for development of RTA. After presentation of the isolated aversive taste stimulus, the greatest increase in catecholamine excretion affected adrenaline, which indicates an anxiety state (fear). There was also a significant increase in noradrenaline excretion in these conditions. The accompanying increase in dopamine excretion in experimental and control animals showed this change to be largely nonspecific in nature, and to result from the experimental procedures. Isolated presentation of the conditioned taste stimulus elicited significantly greater increases in urinary catecholamine excretion in males than in females. It is suggested that the time for which the RTA is retained could be increased by activation of the sympathetico-adrenal system resulting from presentation of the nonreinforced taste stimulus which had acquired aversive properties. Laboratory for the Ontogenesis of Higher Nervous Activity, I. P. Pavlov Institute of Physiology, Russian Academy of Sciences, 199034 St. Petersburg. Translated from Fiziologicheskii Zhurnal imeni I. M. Sechenova, Vol. 82, No. 3, pp. 57–65, March, 1996.  相似文献   

19.
The unconditioned stimulus preexposure effect (US‐PE) is defined as an attenuation of the conditioned response after preexposure to the US prior to conditioning. Evidence exists that this effect can be weakened or eliminated by the presence of a signal predicting the US during the preexposure phase. This evidence has been found consistently across a variety of procedures in adult rats. The aim of the present study was to evaluate whether, in infant rats, signaling the US (LiCl) during preexposure with a salient cue (almond odor) attenuates the US‐PE. During the preexposure phase, preweanling rats received three (Experiment 1) or one (Experiment 2) preexposures to LiCl, preceded by exposure to almond odor. Appropriate control groups were also included in these experiments. After preexposure, two conditioning trials were carried out in which subjects were given LiCl after saccharin consumption. During preexposure, three (Experiment 1a), although not one (Experiment 2a), contingent exposures to almond odor and LiCl resulted in a strong odor aversion. Extinction of the learned taste aversion was facilitated by prior experience with LiCl (Experiments 1b and 2b). This effect was observed regardless of whether or not LiCl was signaled by the almond odor. These results do not coincide with the associative hypotheses proposed to explain the US‐PE, nor are they concurrent with alternative explanations based on the learned helplessness phenomenon. © 2011 Wiley Periodicals,Inc. Dev Psychobiol 54: 808–817, 2012  相似文献   

20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号