首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The origin of thalamic inputs to distinct motor cortical areas was established in five monkeys to determine whether the motor areas receive inputs from a common thalamic nucleus and the extent to which the territories of origin overlap. To not rely on the rough definition of cytoarchitectonic boundaries in the thalamus, monkeys were subjected to multiple injections of tracers (four to seven) in the primary (M1), premotor (PM), and supplementary (SMA) motor cortical areas and in area 46. The cortical areas were distributed into five groups, each receiving inputs from a specific set of thalamic nuclei: 1) M1; 2) SMA-proper and the caudal part of the dorsal PM (PMdc); 3) the rostral and caudal parts of the ventral PM (PMvr and PMvc); 4) the rostral part of the dorsal PM (PMdr); and 5) the superior and inferior parts of area 46 (area 46sup and area 46inf). A major degree of overlap was obtained for the origins of the thalamocortical projections directed to areas 46inf and 46sup and for those terminating in SMA-proper and PMdc. PMvc and PMvr received inputs from adjacent and/or common thalamic regions. In contrast, the degree of overlap between M1 and SMA was smaller. The projection to M1 shared relatively limited zones of origin with the projections directed to PM. Thalamic inputs to the motor cortical areas (M1, SMA, PMd, and PMv), in general, were segregated from those directed to area 46, except in the mediodorsal nucleus, in which there was clear overlap of the territories sending projections to area 46, SMA-proper, and PMdc.  相似文献   

2.
Linkage between the prefrontal cortex and the primary motor cortex is mediated by nonprimary motor-related areas of the frontal lobe. In an attempt to analyse the organization of the prefrontal outflow from area 46 toward the frontal motor-related areas, we investigated the pattern of projections involving the higher-order motor-related areas, such as the presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA) and the rostral cingulate motor area (CMAr). Tracer injections were made into these motor-related areas (their forelimb representation) on the medial wall that had been identified electrophysiologically. The following data were obtained from a series of tract-tracing experiments in Japanese monkeys. (i) Only a few neurons in area 46 were retrogradely labelled from the pre-SMA and CMAr; (ii) terminal labelling from area 46 occurred sparsely in the pre-SMA and CMAr; (iii) a dual labelling technique revealed that the sites of overlap of anterograde labelling from area 46 and retrograde labelling from the pre-SMA and CMAr were evident in the rostral parts of the dorsal and ventral premotor cortices (PMdr and PMvr); (iv) and tracer injections into the PMdr produced neuronal cell labelling in area 46 and terminal labelling in the pre-SMA and CMAr. The present results indicate that a large portion of the prefrontal signals from area 46 is not directly conveyed to the pre-SMA and CMAr, but rather indirectly by way of the PMdr and PMvr. This suggests that area 46 exerts its major influence on the cortical motor system via these premotor areas.  相似文献   

3.
In order to assess the effect of learning a conditional motor task on set-related cells of the precentral cortex we recorded a total of 228 task-related cells in awake, behaving primate. A first sample of 54 set-related cells was recorded while the monkey was performing at 54.7±7.8% and a second sample of 119 set-related cells was recorded at the same stereotaxic coordinates while he was performing at 77.2±8.7%. After the monkey had learned the association between an auditory signal and a motor response we found a significant increase in the proportion and activity of directional set-related cells in the premotor cortex. Furthermore the proportion of short-latency set-related cells was found to increase in this area. None of those changes were observed in the motor cortex.  相似文献   

4.
OBJECTIVE: This study employed EEG source localisation procedures to study the contribution of motor preparatory and attentional processing to foreperiod activity in an S1-S2 motor priming task. METHODS: Behavioural and high-density event-related potential (ERP) data were recorded in an S1-S2 priming task where participants responded to S2 with a left or right-hand button press. S1 either provided information about response hand (informative) or ambiguous information (uninformative). RESULTS: Responses were significantly faster in informative trials compared with uninformative trials. Dipole source analysis of foreperiod lateralized ERPs revealed sources of motor preparatory activity in the dorsolateral premotor cortex (PMd) in line with previous work. In addition, two spatial attention components (ADAN, LDAP) were identified with generators in the PMd and occipitotemporal visual areas in the middle temporal (MT) region, respectively. Separation of motor-related and attentional PMd source locations was reliable along the rostral-caudal axis. CONCLUSIONS: The presence of attentional components in a motor priming paradigm supports the premotor theory of attention which suggests a close link between attention and motor preparatory processes. Separation of components in the premotor cortex is in accord with a functional division of PMd into rostral (higher-order processing) and caudal (motor-related processing) areas as suggested by imaging work. SIGNIFICANCE: A prime for response preparation is a trigger for separate, but closely linked, attention-related activity in premotor areas.  相似文献   

5.
Positron emission tomography (PET) was performed in 10 normal volunteers to investigate regional cortical and subcortical activation induced by the lifting of an object repetitively using a precision grip between the index finger and thumb. Data were obtained for three object weights (4, 200 and 600 g) and a resting condition. Grip and lift forces on a similar object and the activity of selected muscles in the hand, arm and shoulder were also recorded in separate lifting trials. A comparison between all movement conditions and the resting condition revealed significant activation of the primary motor (M1), primary sensory (S1), dorso-caudal premotor (PM), caudal supplementary motor (SMA) and cingulate motor (CMA) cortices contralateral to the hand used. On the ipsilateral side, activation of the M1, caudal SMA and inferior parietal cortex (BA 40) was also found. In the subcortical areas, the bilateral hemispheres and right vermis of the cerebellum, left basal ganglia and thalamus were activated. Behavioral adaptation to a heavier object weight was revealed in a nearly proportional increase of both grip and lift forces, prolonged force application period and a higher level of hand and arm muscle activities. An increase in the rCBF associated with these changes was noted in several cortical and subcortical areas. However, consistent object weight-dependent activation was observed only in the M1/S1 contralateral to the hand used.  相似文献   

6.
《Clinical neurophysiology》2009,120(5):980-986
ObjectiveWe sought to investigate the activity of bilateral parietal and premotor areas during a Go/No Go paradigm involving praxis movements of the dominant hand.MethodsA sentence was presented which instructed subjects on what movement to make (S1; for example, “Show me how to use a hammer.”). After an 8-s delay, “Go” or “No Go” (S2) was presented. If Go, they were instructed to make the movement described in the S1 instruction sentence as quickly as possible, and continuously until the “Rest” cue was presented 3 s later. If No Go, subjects were to simply relax until the next instruction sentence. Event-related potentials (ERP) and event-related desynchronization (ERD) in the beta band (18–22 Hz) were evaluated for three time bins: after S1, after S2, and from −2.5 to −1.5 s before the S2 period.ResultsBilateral premotor ERP was greater than bilateral parietal ERP after the S2 Go compared with the No Go. Additionally, left premotor ERP was greater than that from the right premotor area. There was predominant left parietal ERD immediately after S1 for both Go and No Go, which was sustained for the duration of the interval between S1 and S2. For both S2 stimuli, predominant left parietal ERD was again seen when compared to that from the left premotor or right parietal area. However, the left parietal ERD was greater for Go than No Go.ConclusionThe results suggest a dominant role in the left parietal cortex for planning, executing, and suppressing praxis movements. The ERP and ERD show different patterns of activation and may reflect distinct neural movement-related activities.SignificanceThe data can guide further studies to determine the neurophysiological changes occurring in apraxia patients and help explain the unique error profiles seen in patients with left parietal damage.  相似文献   

7.
OBJECTIVE: The present study used behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) indices of task-switching to examine whether schizophrenia patients have a specific deficit in anticipatory task-set reconfiguration. METHODS: Participants switched between univalent tasks in an alternating runs paradigms with blocked response-stimulus interval (RSI) manipulation (150, 300, 600, and 1200ms). Nineteen high functioning people with schizophrenia were compared to controls that were matched for age, gender, education and premorbid IQ estimate. RESULTS: Schizophrenia patients had overall increased RT, but no increase in corrected RT switch cost. In the schizophrenia group, ERPs showed reduced activation of the differential positivity in anticipation of switch trial at the optimal 600ms RSI and reduced activation of the frontal post-stimulus switch negativity at both 600 and 1200ms RSI compared to the control group. CONCLUSIONS: Despite no behavioral differences in task switching performance, anticipatory and stimulus-triggered ERP indices of task-switching suggest group differences in processing of switch and repeat trials, especially at longer RSI conditions that for control participants provide opportunity for anticipatory activation of task-set reconfiguration processes. SIGNIFICANCE: These results are compatible with impaired implementation of endogenously driven processes in schizophrenia and greater reliance on external task cues, especially at long preparation intervals.  相似文献   

8.
Functional connections between dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) and primary motor cortex (M1) have been revealed by paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). We tested if such connections would be modulated during a cognitive process (response selection) known to rely on those circuits. PMd-M1 TMS applied 75 ms after a cue to select a manual response facilitated motor-evoked potentials (MEPs). MEPs were facilitated at 50 ms in a control task of response execution, suggesting that PMd-M1 interactions at 75 ms are functionally specific to the process of response selection. At 100 ms, PMd-M1 TMS delayed choice reaction time (RT). Importantly, the MEP (at 75 ms) and the RT (at 100 ms) effects were correlated in a way that was hand-specific. When the response was made with the M1-contralateral hand, MEPs correlated with slower RTs. When the response was made with the M1-ipsilateral hand, MEPs correlated with faster RTs. Paired-pulse TMS confined to M1 did not produce these effects, confirming the causal influence of PMd inputs. This study shows that a response selection signal evolves in PMd early during the reaction period (75-100 ms), impacts on M1 and affects behaviour. Such interactions are temporally, anatomically and functionally specific, and have a causal role in choosing which movement to make.  相似文献   

9.
We recorded event-related brain potentials (ERPs) from 13 patients with chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) and 13 matched normal controls. To assess attentional and memory deficits in CFS patients, we used a short-term memory task in which events occurred in different spatial locations and the patients made a rapid-response (RT) when a letter in a relevant location matched a letter in the prememorized set (Attention paradigm). Time-on-task effects on the ERP and behavioral measures were assessed over the 2 1/4-hour duration of this task. Both groups also performed a visual Oddball paradigm, with an RT, before and after the Attention paradigm. The patients' RTs were much more variable and, in nine of 13 cases, slower than the mean RT of the controls in both paradigms. The patients' memory performance was not significantly different from that of the controls and there were no group differences in the overall amplitude, latency, or scalp distribution of the N1, P2, N2, or P300 components of the ERP in either paradigm. The ERP and performance data from both paradigms suggest that perceptual, attentional, and short-term memory processes were unaffected in CFS patients and that the differences were limited to response-related processes.  相似文献   

10.
In 19 healthy volunteers, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to probe the excitability in pathways linking the left dorsal premotor cortex and right primary motor cortex and those linking the left and right motor cortex during the response delay and the reaction time period while subjects performed a delayed response [symbol 1 (S1) - symbol 2 (S2)] Go-NoGo reaction time task with visual cues. Conditioning TMS pulses were applied to the left premotor or left motor cortex 8 ms before a test pulse was given to the right motor cortex at 300 or 1800 ms after S1 or 150 ms after S2. S1 coded for right-hand or left-hand movement, and S2 for release or stopping the prepared movement. Conditioning of the left premotor cortex led to interhemispheric inhibition at 300 ms post-S1, interhemispheric facilitation at 150 ms post-S2, and shorter reaction times in the move-left condition. Conditioning of the left motor cortex led to inhibition at 1800 ms post-S1 and 150 ms post-S2, and slower reaction times for move-right conditions, and inhibition at 300 and 1800 ms post-S1 for move-left conditions. Relative motor evoked potential amplitudes following premotor conditioning at 150 ms post-S2 were significantly smaller in 'NoGo' than in 'Go' trials for move-left instructions. We conclude that the excitability in left premotor/motor right motor pathways is context-dependent and affects motor behaviour. Thus, the left premotor cortex is engaged not only in action selection but also in withholding and releasing a preselected movement generated by the right motor cortex.  相似文献   

11.
OBJECTIVES: Valid markers of psychobiological processes, including changes over the lifespan, must be reliable. This study investigated the reliability of the auditory event-related potential (ERP) over a 3 year period. METHODS: Predictable and unpredictable rare tones were embedded in common-to-rare sequences at 3 different ratios (2:3, 2:5 and 2:8). Forty-six older (mean age 72.3 years) volunteers pressed a key to the rare tones, and ERPs (Fz, Cz and Pz) and reaction time (RT) were measured. Reliability across years was assessed using 3 methods: (1) determination of the stability of waveform components (P1, N1, P2, N2 and P3); (2) cross-correlation of successive 15 ms epochs of within-subject ERPs; and (3) cross-correlation of 15 ms epochs of between-subject ERPs. RESULTS: With all analyses, the ERP was stable. Analysis of the scored components indicated that P3 was especially stable in the unpredictable rare (2:8) condition. Earlier components were equally stable across all conditions. Analysis of 15 ms ERP epochs indicated significant ERP stability 60 ms after stimulation, lasting over 640 ms. CONCLUSIONS: Robust within-subject reliability of the ERP strengthens its potential use for detecting preclinical changes in at-risk elderly populations.  相似文献   

12.
OBJECTIVE: The somatosensory evoked potentials of the median nerve (SEP) were registered intracerebrally in 12 subjects to elucidate the origin of N30 component and its behavior in the motor 'gating' tasks. METHODS: The recordings were done from the electrodes which were inserted within the cortex of frontal lobe in the pre-surgical phase of epilepsy surgery. The registrations focused on the precentral N30 SEP component and its behaviour under the 'gating' paradigms. Two different 'gating' paradigms, motor and mental, were used and the SEP then were recorded in 3 conditions: (1) normal (N) paradigm, during which the subjects were instructed not to perform any movement by the stimulated hand, or to mentally simulate the movement; (2) active movement (AM) paradigm, during which the subjects were instructed to perform the active movement as the internal motor sequence test by the fingers of the hand of the stimulated limb; (3) mental movement simulation (MMS), during which the subjects were instructed to only mentally simulate the movements performed in the previous paradigm, and this 'virtual' movement also involved the hand of the stimulated limb. The recordings were done at least twice in each paradigm and averaged runs of 2000 artefact-free sweeps were used for the analysis. RESULTS: The results demonstrated that the precentral N30 component of SEP is generated only in the pre-motor area, either dorsolaterally or mesially, which consists of Brodmann's areas 6 and 8, and their borders. Only the N30 potentials recorded there in 7 subjects had a shape and character of 'near-field' potential. The behaviour of the N30 component when recorded in the AM and MMS paradigms was different depending on the fact of whether they were recorded dorsolaterally or mesially. When there was a clear 'near-field' N30 potential recorded mesially, there was a certain gating present during the AM paradigm, i.e. during the performance of movement. However, the gating caused by the mental movement simulation in the MMS paradigm was substantially more expressed, and the N30 wave practically disappeared in some cases. On the contrary, the gating of the N30 wave, recorded in the frontal dorsolateral premotor cortex (DLPC), was almost complete when the AM (active movement) paradigm was employed, and it was only partial when the MMS paradigm (mental movement simulation) was employed. CONCLUSIONS: The results of N30 registrations in our group of patients strongly support the theory of separate generator (or generators) of the N30 wave within the premotor cortex. They also brought forward evidence that the dorsolateral premotor cortex (Brodmann's areas 6 and 8) serves as the substrate of the 'motor execution' process, and the mesial frontal cortex (Brodmann's area 6) serves as the substrate of the 'motor planning' process. Further research should focus on the mutual registration of neurophysiological phenomena and imaging phenomena to obtain new data, which will be able to more precisely elucidate the workings of the premotor cortex during the whole process of motor performance.  相似文献   

13.
汉字单字词识别的N400   总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7  
目的 探讨单词识别过程中事件相关电位N40 0成分的心理学意义。方法 在长 ( 60 0ms)与短 ( 2 0 0ms)两种刺激呈现间隔 (SOA)、3种语境 (相关、中性和无关 )条件下 ,进行词汇抉择作业 ,通过对反应时间 (RT)和事件相关电位 (ERP)的检测 ,观察目标词词汇抉择的语义启动效应 (RT效应和N40 0波幅效应 )。结果 长SOA时 ,RT实验有语义促进效应 (相关语境RT较中性语境短 )和抑制效应 (无关语境RT较中性语境长 ) ,而ERP实验只有N40 0波幅抑制效应 ;短SOA时 ,RT实验只有语义促进效应 ,而ERP实验无任何N40 0波幅效应。结论 结果表明 ,N40 0反映了词汇后水平的整合加工 ,而不反映词汇水平的词汇提取加工。  相似文献   

14.
The ipsilateral association connections of the cortex of the dorsal part of the rostral bank of the parieto-occipital sulcus and of the adjoining posterior part of the superior parietal lobule were studied by using different retrograde fluorescent tracers. Fluoro-Ruby, Fast blue and Diamidino yellow were injected into visual area V6A, and dorso-caudal (PMdc, F2) and dorso-rostral (PMdr, F7) premotor cortex, respectively. The parietal area of injection had been previously characterized physiologically in behaving monkeys, through a variety of oculomotor and visuomanual tasks. Area V6A is mainly linked by reciprocal projections to parietal areas 7m, MIP (medial intraparietal) and PEa, and, to a lesser extent, to frontal areas PMdr (rostral dorsal premotor cortex, F7) and PMdc (F2). All these areas project to that part of the dorsocaudal premotor cortex that has a direct access to primary motor cortex. V6A is also connected to area F5 and, to a lesser extent, to 7a, ventral (VIP) and lateral (LIP) intraparietal areas. This pattern of association connections may explain the presence of visually-related and eye-position signals in premotor cortex, as well as the influence of information concerning arm position and movement direction on V6A neural activity. Area V6A emerges as a potential 'early' node of the distributed network underlying visually-guided reaching. In this network, reciprocal association connections probably impose, through re-entrant signalling, a recursive property to the operations leading to the composition of eye and hand motor commands.  相似文献   

15.
ObjectiveWe investigated the characteristics of cortical rhythmic activity in No-go processing during somatosensory Go/No-go paradigms, by using magnetoencephalography (MEG).MethodsTwelve normal subjects performed a warning stimulus (S1) – imperative stimulus (S2) task with Go/No-go paradigms. The recordings were conducted in three conditions. In Condition 1, the Go stimulus was delivered to the second digit, and the No-go stimulus to the fifth digit. The participants responded by pushing a button with their right thumb for the Go stimulus. In Condition 2, the Go and No-go stimuli were reversed. Condition 3 was the resting control.ResultsA rebound in amplitude was recorded in the No-go trials for theta, alpha, and beta activity, peaking at 600–900 ms. A suppression of amplitude was recorded in Go and No-go trials for alpha activity, peaking at 300–600 ms, and in Go and No-go trials for beta activity, peaking at 200–300 ms.ConclusionThe cortical rhythmic activity clearly has several dissociated components relating to different motor functions, including response inhibition, execution, and decision-making.SignificanceThe present study revealed the characteristics of cortical rhythmic activity in No-go processing.  相似文献   

16.
Single unit recording studies in non-human premotor cortex have revealed neurons with motor-related activity. Other neurons, however, seem to be involved in prior movement selection and preparation processes, and have activity related to visual instruction signals or movement preparation ('set'). We have used single pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to identify similar processes in human subjects. In Experiment 1 subjects performed a cued movement task while being stimulated with TMS over three sites: sensorimotor cortex, posterior premotor cortex and anterior premotor cortex. TMS slowed movements when applied at 140 ms after the visual cue over the anterior premotor site, at 180 ms after the visual cue over the posterior premotor site, and at 220 ms and later after the visual cue over the sensorimotor cortex. The results are consistent with a change from signal to movement-related processing when moving from premotor to motor cortex. In Experiment 2 there was a preparatory set period between the instruction signal that informed subjects which movement to make and the 'go' signal that informed them when to actually make the movement. TMS was applied over the anterior premotor site and the sensorimotor site during the set period. At both sites TMS had similar effects on slowing subsequent movements. The results suggest set activity in both premotor and motor cortices in human subjects.  相似文献   

17.
This study concerns the question of how task modification affects the frequency occurrence of event-related potentials (ERP) inside the active cortical areas. In 13 candidates for epilepsy surgery, 156 sites in the temporal (74), frontal (73), and parietal (9) cortices were recorded by means of depth and subdural electrodes. Four modifications of the somatosensory evoked P3-like potentials were performed; (i) an oddball paradigm with silent counting of target stimuli (P3c); (ii) an oddball paradigm with a hand movement in response to target stimuli (P3m); (iii) an S1-S2 paradigm, ERP in the P300 time window after the S2 stimulus, with silent counting of target stimuli (S2c), and (iv) an S1-S2 paradigm with a hand movement in response to target stimuli (S2m). In comparing the oddball paradigms with the S1-S2 (contingent negative variation, CNV) paradigms, four regions emerge that are significantly linked with the oddball P3; the prefrontal cortex, the cingulate, the amygdalo-hippocampal complex, and the lateral temporal cortex. A prominent role of the cingulate and the fronto-orbital cortex in the cognitive processing of movement was supported when tasks with identical cognitive loads but different required responses were compared. Even relatively simple cognitive tasks activate many cortical regions. The investigated areas were activated in all tests; however, small regions in each field were active or inactive in relation to the nature of the task. The study indicates a variable and task-dependent internal organization of a highly complex and widely distributed system of active cortical areas.  相似文献   

18.
《Clinical neurophysiology》2014,125(9):1912-1922
ObjectiveThe aim of this study was to identify neurophysiologic markers generated by primary motor and premotor cortex for laryngeal muscles, recorded from laryngeal muscle.MethodsTen right-handed healthy subjects underwent navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation (nTMS) and 18 patients underwent direct cortical stimulation (DCS) over the left hemisphere, while recording neurophysiologic markers, short latency response (SLR) and long latency response (LLR) from cricothyroid muscle. Both healthy subjects and patients were engaged in the visual object-naming task. In healthy subjects, the stimulation was time-locked at 10–300 ms after picture presentation while in the patients it was at zero time.ResultsThe latency of SLR in healthy subjects was 12.66 ± 1.09 ms and in patients 12.67 ± 1.23 ms. The latency of LLR in healthy subjects was 58.5 ± 5.9 ms, while in patients 54.25 ± 3.69 ms. SLR elicited by the stimulation of M1 for laryngeal muscles corresponded to induced dysarthria, while LLR elicited by stimulation of the premotor cortex in the caudal opercular part of inferior frontal gyrus, recorded from laryngeal muscle, corresponded to speech arrest in patients and speech arrest and/or language disturbances in healthy subjects.ConclusionIn both groups, SLR indicated location of M1 for laryngeal muscles, and LLR location of premotor cortex in the caudal opercular part of inferior frontal gyrus, recorded from laryngeal muscle, while stimulation of these areas in the dominant hemisphere induced transient speech disruptions.SignificanceDescribed methodology can be used in preoperative mapping, and it is expected to facilitate surgical planning and intraoperative mapping, preserving these areas from injuries.  相似文献   

19.
OBJECTIVE: This study concerned sensory processing (post-stimulus late evoked potential components) in different parts of the human brain as related to a motor task (hand movement) in a cognitive paradigm (Contingent Negative Variation). The focus of the study was on the time and space distribution of middle and late post-stimulus evoked potential (EP) components, and on the processing of sensory information in the subcortical-cortical networks. METHODS: Stereoelectroencephalography (SEEG) recordings of the contingent negative variation (CNV) in an audio-visual paradigm with a motor task were taken from 30 patients (27 patients with drug-resistant epilepsy; 3 patients with chronic thalamic pain). The intracerebral recordings were taken from 337 cortical sites (primary sensorimotor area (SM1); supplementary motor area (SMA); the cingulate gyrus; the orbitofrontal, premotor and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices; the temporal cortex, including the amygdalohippocampal complex; the parietooccipital lobes; and the insula) and from subcortical structures (the basal ganglia and the posterior thalamus). The concurrent scalp recordings were obtained from 3 patients in the thalamic group. In 4 patients in the epilepsy group, scalp recordings were taken separately from the SEEG procedure. The middle and long latency evoked potentials following an auditory warning (S1) and a visual imperative (S2) stimuli were analyzed. The occurrences of EPs were studied in two time windows (200-300 ms; and over 300 ms) following S1 and S2. RESULTS: Following S1, a high frequency of EP with latencies over 200 ms was observed in the primary sensorimotor area, the supplementary motor area, the premotor cortex, the orbitofrontal cortex, the cingulate gyrus, some parts of the temporal lobe, the basal ganglia, the insula, and the posterior thalamus. Following S2, a high frequency of EP in both of the time windows over 200 ms was observed in the SM1, the SMA, the premotor and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the orbitofrontal cortex, the cingulate gyrus, the basal ganglia, the posterior thalamus, and in some parts of the temporal cortex. The concurrent scalp recordings in the thalamic group of patients twice revealed potentials peaking approximately at 215 ms following S1. Following S2, EP occurred with latencies of 215 and 310 ms, respectively. Following S1, separate scalp recordings in 4 patients in the epilepsy group displayed EP 3 times in the 'over 300 ms' time window. Following S2, EP were presented once in the '200-300 ms' time window and 3 times in the 'over 300 ms' time window. CONCLUSIONS: The SM1, the SMA, multiple sites of the frontal lobe, some parts of the temporal lobe, the cingulate gyrus, the basal ganglia, the insula, and the posterior thalamus all participate in a cortico-subcortical network that is important for the parallel cognitive processing of sensory information in a movement related task.  相似文献   

20.
OBJECTIVES: To examine how the differences in the sequences of brain activation during the go/no-go delayed response choice reaction time (RT) task are reflected into two concurrent methods of measuring brain electrical activity, the alpha band event-related desynchronization (alpha ERD) and the contingent negative variation (CNV). METHODS: alpha ERD and CNV were calculated using appropriate techniques from the same samples of electroencephalographic activity recorded during performance of a cued choice go/no-go delayed response RT task (i.e. S1 (go/no-go)--S2 paradigm) in 8 healthy subjects. RESULTS: All segments of the CNV traces were different in the go and the no-go conditions. The go CNV traces displayed a typical pattern of slow rising negativity reflecting the build-up of attentional resources necessary for adequate performance of the task. On the other hand, the no-go traces remained close to zero reflecting the 'withdrawal' of further preparation after evaluation of S1. During the same period, both go and no-go ERD traces showed a gradual decrease in alpha band power (desynchronization) that was evident until shortly before the presentation of S2. It was only in the 500 ms before S2 presentation that there was any indication that the go and no-go ERD traces were different, but this did not reach statistical significance. CONCLUSIONS: Our data show that the pattern of the go/no-go difference in alpha ERD traces does not correspond to the pattern that can be seen in the CNV traces. This suggests that there is no direct coupling of CNV and alpha ERD, most probably because they measure different aspects of cortical electrical activity. In addition, the extent of the no-go alpha ERD reveals that refraining from performance of a pre-programmed movement is by no means a passive/inactive process.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号