首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 62 毫秒
1.
Visual tracking of moving targets requires the combination of smooth pursuit eye movements with catch-up saccades. In primates, catch-up saccades usually take place only during pursuit initiation because pursuit gain is close to unity. This contrasts with the lower and more variable gain of smooth pursuit in cats, where smooth eye movements are intermingled with catch-up saccades during steady-state pursuit. In this paper, we studied in detail the role of retinal slip in the prediction of target motion during smooth and saccadic pursuit in the cat. We found that the typical pattern of pursuit in the cat was a combination of smooth eye movements with saccades. During smooth pursuit initiation, there was a correlation between peak eye acceleration and target velocity. During pursuit maintenance, eye velocity oscillated at approximately 3 Hz around a steady-state value. The average gain of smooth pursuit was approximately 0.5. Trained cats were able to continue pursuing in the absence of a visible target, suggesting a role of the prediction of future target motion in this species. The analysis of catch-up saccades showed that the smooth-pursuit motor command is added to the saccadic command during catch-up saccades and that both position error and retinal slip are taken into account in their programming. The influence of retinal slip on catch-up saccades showed that prediction about future target motion is used in the programming of catch-up saccades. Altogether, these results suggest that pursuit systems in primates and cats are qualitatively similar, with a lower average gain in the cat and that prediction affects both saccades and smooth eye movements during pursuit.  相似文献   

2.
Operant conditioning was used to train cats with their heads immobilized to localize sound by directing their eyes to the location of the sources. The kinematics of those eye movements were studied and compared with eye movements to visual targets at the same locations. The main finding of this study is that eye movements to broadband long-duration acoustic targets have two components: an initial slow phase of variable duration and a fast, normal saccade. The slow component is characterized by a persistent, shallow velocity ramp, while the saccadic component of the response falls on the main sequence computed from eye movements to visual targets. The slow component was shorter before saccades to long-duration stimuli performed under the delayed-saccade task and practically absent before saccades to transient acoustic stimuli. The results suggest that the initial slow component is used by cats to deal with uncertainty associated with the location of long-duration broadband targets and that the input to the saccade integrator(s) is similar under both visual and acoustic conditions.  相似文献   

3.
Summary Single units in the regions of the III, IV and VI nuclei were recorded together with EOG's for horizontal and vertical eye positions in alert macaques. The sequential analysis of several dynamic parameters of the activity patterns in correlation to the saccade velocity for saccades in the on-direction leads to the results that: 1. eye position coded neurons can clearly be separated into two main classes [early peak (EP) and late peak (LP)] by means of their activity patterns during saccades in the on-direction; 2. the maximum impulse rate of EP neurons shows a better correlation with saccade velocity than the difference between maximum and initial impulse rate while the opposite is valid for LP neurons. EP neurons are likely to be motoneurons which initiate saccadic eye movements whereas LP neurons are too slow for this task because they reach their maximum impulse rate after half the saccadic time. The dynamic properties of LP neurons have several features similar to those of primary stretch receptors during ramp-like stretches. The possible influence of fusimotor activity on the oculomotor system is discussed. The fact that the relationship between dynamic index and saccade velocity shows subgroups of data supports the assumption that the state of alertness changes instantaneously in untrained monkeys.Supported in part by the National Eye Institute, U.S. Public Health Service under grant EY-00592 to Dr. G. Westheimer and by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft.  相似文献   

4.
Saccades made between targets at optical infinity require both eyes to rotate by the same angle. Nevertheless, these saccades are consistently accompanied by transient vergence eye movements. Here we have investigated whether the dynamics of these vergence movements depend on the trajectory of the coincident conjugate movement, and whether moving the head during eye-head gaze shifts modifies vergence dynamics. In agreement with previous reports, saccades with more symmetric (i.e., "bell-shaped") conjugate velocity profiles were accompanied by stereotyped biphasic vergence transients (i.e., a divergence phase immediately followed by a convergence phase). However, we found that saccades with more asymmetric, oscillatory-like dynamics (characterized by a typical conjugate reacceleration of the eyes following the initial peak velocity) were systematically accompanied by more complex vergence movements that also exhibited oscillatory-like dynamics. These findings could be extended to conditions where the head was free to move: comparable conjugate and vergence oscillations were observed during head-restrained saccades and combined eye-head gaze shifts. The duration of the vergence oscillation increased with gaze shift amplitude, such that as many as four vergence phases (divergence-convergence-divergence-convergence) were recorded during 55 degrees gaze shifts (approximately 240 ms). To quantify these observations, we first determined whether conjugate and vergence peak velocities were systematically correlated. Conjugate peak velocity was linearly related to the peak velocity of the initial divergence phase for saccades and gaze shifts of all amplitudes, regardless of their dynamics. However, for more asymmetric saccades and gaze shifts, the subsequent convergence and divergence peak velocities were not correlated with either the initial peak conjugate velocity or the peak velocity of the conjugate reacceleration. Next, we determined that the duration of the different conjugate and vergence oscillation phases remained relatively constant across all saccades and gaze shifts, and that the conjugate and vergence profiles oscillated together at approximately 7.5-10 Hz. Using computer simulations, we show that a classic feed-forward model is unable to reproduce vergence oscillations based solely on peripheral mechanisms. Furthermore, we demonstrate that small modifications to the gain and delay of a simple feedback model for saccade generation can generate conjugate oscillations, and propose that such changes reflect the influence of lowered alertness on the tecto-reticular pathways. We conclude that peripheral mechanisms can only account for the initial divergence that accompanies all saccades, and that the conjugate and vergence oscillations observed during asymmetric movements arise centrally from an integrative binocular controller.  相似文献   

5.
The activity of 53 antidromically identified abducens motoneurons was analyzed in alert cats during spontaneous and vestibular induced eye movements. Conduction velocities ranged from 13 to 70 m/s and all motoneurons increased their discharge rates with successive eye positions in the abducting direction. Motoneurons were recruited from -19 degrees to +7 degrees. Within the oculomotor range frequency saturation was never observed for any cell. The slope of rate-position (k) relationships ranged from 2 to 17.7 spikes/s/deg (n = 40, mean 8.7 +/- 2.5). Regression analysis showed that the rate-position plots could be fit by straight lines but in most cases exponential curves produced slightly better statistical fits. Steeper slopes suggest that successively larger increases in k are required for the lateral rectus muscle to maintain more eccentric fixations in the on direction. Interspike intervals for a constant eye position exhibited low variability (less than 3.5%) for fixations shorter than 1 s. Over longer periods, variability increased in proportion to the duration of the fixation in exponential-like fashion up to 14%. Abducens motoneurons showed considerable variability in frequency during repeated fixations of the same eye position. Discharge rates were found to depend upon both the direction of the previous eye movement and, more importantly, the animal's level of alertness. The rate-position regression lines for fixation periods after saccades in the on direction significantly differed in slopes (100%) and thresholds (20%) from those in the off direction. The observed static hysteresis in abducens motoneuron behavior was in opposite direction to that previously described for the mechanical properties of the lateral rectus. This suggests both neural and mechanical factors are significantly involved in determining final eye position. The animal's level of alertness was evaluated in this study by counting the number of saccadic movements/s occurring in "alert" (1 +/- 0.2 saccades/s), and "drowsy" (0.5 +/- 0.2 saccades/s) circumstances. Comparison of the rate-position regression lines between the two conditions showed a significant decrease in slopes (100%) and elevation of thresholds (70%). Discharge rate of abducens motoneurons increased abruptly 8.9 +/- 2.8 ms prior to saccades in the horizontal on direction, and decreased 14.8 +/- 4.05 m before saccades in the off direction. During purely vertical saccades the firing frequency of abducens motoneurons did not change. Burst frequency did not saturate during saccades, but increased with saccadic velocity in a linear fashion.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

6.
We presented a mechanical target moving at constant velocity to awake, nontrained, head-restrained cats, in order to study how naive animals pursue objects moving at a high speed with their gaze. Eye movements were recorded while the target was moving in different directions at a constant velocity (20–80°/s) through the center of the visual field. We observed two oculomotor strategies: cats either made an interception saccade (IS) toward the target but opposite to its motion, or tracked it in the direction of motion. They used the interception strategy more frequently when the gaze position error at the onset of target motion was large, and the tracking strategy when it was small. Interception was always achieved by single saccades, which were faster than tracking saccades (TS). During tracking, cats generated sequences of two to six saccades separated by "smooth" eye movements. Tracking quality varied considerably from trial to trial. When the level of motivation was high, cats would track the target at 80°/s over up to 75% of the oculomotor range, with relatively small position errors. We compared ISs and TSs with respect to their metric properties and timing. The amplitudes of ISs positively correlated with position error existing 100 ms before saccade onset, but saccade vectors were directed to a point ahead of the target along the target's track. We conclude that, in programming the ISs, target motion is used to predict the future target position so as to assure a spatial lead of the gaze at the saccade end, instead of attempting a precise capture of the target. The amplitude of TSs did not depend on preceding position errors. TSs were usually small at the onset of the first saccade, as if cats would wait till the target arrived near the line of sight. A majority of primary TSs were initiated before the target arrived near the direction of gaze. Thus they had a direction, opposite to the position error sampled 100 ms before the saccade, but the same as the direction of target motion. Prediction of the future target position from its velocity vector should therefore contribute to the programming of TSs. In addition, we observed that TSs were faster when they were initiated with a spatial lag relative to the target and they were slower if there was a spatial lead or target velocity was reduced. Such a modulation appears to be analogous to the predictive correction of the saccade amplitude during smooth pursuit in primates. Considering strong visual motion sensitivity and motor properties of output neurons of the superior colliculus, it is likely that, in cats, the colliculus makes a major contribution to the integration of eye movement-related and target motion-related signals. Electronic Publication  相似文献   

7.
Changes in the amplitude and dynamic parameters of purposive saccades were studied in monkeys with MPTP-induced Parkinson-like syndrome. Lengthening of saccade latency, decreased maximum velocity of eye movements, and impaired saccade accuracy were observed at the early stages MPTP- syndrome. Different disturbances of large- and small-scale saccades were found.  相似文献   

8.
The activity of 43 antidromically identified abducens internuclear neurons with conduction velocities ranging from 14 to 54 m/s was analyzed in alert cats during spontaneous and vestibular induced eye movements. The discharge rate of internuclear neurons significantly increased with successive adducting positions of the contralateral eye. Slopes of rate-position (k) relationships ranged from 3.1 to 17.9 spikes/deg (mean 12.01 +/- 3.1). Threshold ranged from -19 degrees to +3 degrees. Frequency saturation was never observed for any internuclear neuron within the oculomotor range. Although straight lines were selected to illustrate the rate-position relationships, exponential curves always provided the best statistical fit demonstrating that an enhancement in frequency potentiation (k) must accompany more eccentric fixations in the on direction. Internuclear neurons showed a low variability in firing rate (less than 3.0%) for fixations less than 1 s. Variability increased with both longer and repeated fixations of the same eye position. Discharge rates were found to depend upon both the direction of the preceding eye movement and the animal's level of alertness. Separate regression lines of rate-position relations following saccades in the on and off directions differed significantly in slope (100%), but not threshold. The observed static hysteresis in an identified non-motoneuron shows this property to be in a central neural circuit prior to the extraocular motoneuron. The slopes (k) of rate-position plots for all internuclear neurons decreased significantly (100%) when level of alertness changed from "alert" (1 +/- 0.2 saccades/s) to "drowsy" (0.5 +/- 0.2 saccades/s). Thresholds, however, were not significantly altered. Discharge rate of abducens internuclear neurons increased abruptly 10.4 +/- 2.5 ms preceding saccades in the on direction, and decreased 20.5 +/- 7.8 ms before saccades in the off direction. Internuclear neuronal activity was not affected by pure vertical saccades. During on direction saccades, firing frequency did not saturate, but increased with velocity in a linear fashion. Exponential functions often fit the data better due to the difference in slopes of rate-velocity plots for on vs off direction saccades. Slopes (rs) of rate-velocity regression lines during spontaneous saccades ranged from 0.99 to 4.10 spikes/s/deg/s (mean 2.16 +/- 0.93). During saccades in the off direction activity always decreased, but it seldom ceased. Rate-velocity regression lines measured during the fast phase of vestibular nystagmus (rsv = 2.09 +/- 0.88) showed no significant differences from rs slopes in 82% of the cases.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

9.
Lid-eye coordination during vertical gaze changes in man and monkey   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
1. To investigate the coordination between the upper lid and the eye during vertical gaze changes, the movements of the lid and the eye were measured by the electromagnetic search-coil technique in three humans and two monkeys. 2. In both man and monkey, there was a close correspondence between the metrics of the lid movement and those of the concomitant eye movement during vertical fixation, smooth pursuit, and saccades. 3. During steady fixation, the eye and lid assumed essentially equal average positions; however, in man the lid would often undergo small idiosyncratic movements of up to 5 degrees when the eye was completely stationary. 4. During sinusoidal smooth pursuit between 0.2 and 1.0 Hz, the gain and phase shift of eye and lid movements were remarkably similar. The smaller gain and larger phase lag for downward smooth pursuit eye movements was mirrored in a similar reduced gain and increased phase lag for downward lid movements. 5. The time course of vertical lid movements associated with saccades was generally a faithful replica of the time course of the concomitant saccade; the similarity was especially impressive when the details of the velocity profiles were compared. Consequently, lid movements associated with vertical eye saccades are called lid saccades. 6. On average, lid saccades start some 5 ms later than the concomitant eye saccades but reach peak velocity at about the same time as the eye saccade. Concurrent lid and eye saccades in the downward direction have similar amplitudes and velocities. Lid saccades in the upward direction are often smaller and slower than the concomitant eye saccades. The relation of peak velocity versus amplitude and of duration versus amplitude are similar for lid and eye saccades. 7. To investigate the neural signal responsible for lid saccades, isometric tension and EMG activity were recorded from the lids of the two authors. 8. The isometric tensions during upward lid saccades exceeded the tensions required to hold the lid in its final position.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

10.
In this report, we provide the first characterization of abducens nucleus neuron (ABN) discharge dynamics during horizontal disjunctive saccades. These movements function to rapidly transfer the visual axes between targets located at different eccentricities and depths. Our primary objective was to determine whether the signals carried by ABNs during these movements are appropriate to drive the motion of the eye to which they project. We also asked whether ABNs encode eye movements similarly during disjunctive saccades and disjunctive fixation. To address the first objective we 1) assessed whether we could predict the discharge dynamics of individual neurons during disjunctive saccades based on their discharge properties during conjugate saccades and 2) directly estimated the sensitivity of individual neurons to either the ipsilateral/contralateral eye or the conjugate/vergence position and velocity using bootstrap statistics. Our main finding was that during disjunctive saccades in the direction ipsilateral to the recording site (ON-direction), the majority of ABNs preferentially encoded the velocity and the position of the ipsilateral eye. The remaining neurons predominantly encoded the conjugate motion of the eyes (i.e., were equally sensitive to the motion of both eyes). Generally, ipsilateral/contralateral eye based models better described neuronal discharges than conjugate/vergence based models, yet both model structures yielded similar conclusions. Moreover, the preferred eye of individual neurons based on their position and velocity sensitivities were generally well matched. We also found that for saccades in the OFF-direction, the pausing behavior of ABNs was similar during conjugate and disjunctive saccades, with the exception that for movements of small amplitudes, more ABNs paused during conjugate saccades. Finally, we found that putative motoneurons and internuclear neurons encoded ON- and OFF-direction disjunctive saccades in a similar manner. To address our second objective, we compared the discharge properties of individual ABNs during disjunctive saccades and disjunctive fixation. Good coherence was observed between the preferred eye of individual ABNs during the two behaviors. Taken together, our results indicate that although individual ABNs can encode the motion of both eyes to various degrees, the population drive of ABNs accounts for most of the movement of the ipsilateral eye during disjunctive saccades and disjunctive fixation.  相似文献   

11.
We studied responses of pulvinar neurons in awake cats that were allowed to execute spontaneous eye movements. Extracellular cell activity during saccades, saccade-like image shifts, and various stationary visual stimuli was recorded together with the animals' eye positions. All neurons analyzed had receptive fields that covered most of the central 80x80 degrees of the animals' visual field and did only respond to large (>20 degrees) visual stimuli. According to their response properties, recorded neurons were divided into three populations. The first group, termed "S neurons" (16%), responded when the animals performed saccades but were unresponsive to any of the visual stimuli tested. These neurons do not seem to receive a visual input that is strong enough to drive them. The second group, termed "V neurons" (51%), responded to various visual stimuli including saccade-like image motion when the eyes were stationary, but not when the animals executed saccades. V neurons therefore distinguish retinal image movements that are generated externally from internally generated image motion. Finally, "SV neurons" (31%) responded when the animals made saccades as well as to saccade-like image motion or to stationary stimuli. Although these neurons do not distinguish self-induced retinal image motion from motion generated by external stimulus movements, they must receive non-retinal motion-related input, because responses elicited by saccades had shorter latencies than responses to saccade-like stimulus movements. Only SV neurons resemble response properties of pretectal neurons that project to the pulvinar and that comprise the major subcortical visual input. The functional significance of pulvinar neuronal populations for visual and visuomotor information processing is discussed.  相似文献   

12.
The present results demonstrate that anticompensatory neurones could be recorded within the prepositus hypoglossi nucleus of the alert cat. These neurones, like burster-driving neurones described in paralysed cats, were characterized by a tonic increase of firing for head turning in the contraversive direction (type II) associated to bursts for each vestibular quick phase in the same direction. They were also involved in the generation of visually triggered saccades since they displayed a burst preceding contraversive saccades and a pause in the opposite direction. Thus, these neurones code eye velocity for both vestibular quick phases and visually triggered saccades in addition to head velocity. Such a firing behaviour suggests that, in addition to the classical crossed tecto-reticulo-spinal network, an ipsilateral pathway could also be involved in the generation of visually triggered rapid eye movements.  相似文献   

13.
Rapid eyelid closure, or a blink, often accompanies head-restrained and head-unrestrained gaze shifts. This study examines the interactions between such gaze-evoked blinks and gaze shifts in monkeys. Blink probability increases with gaze amplitude and at a faster rate for head-unrestrained movements. Across animals, blink likelihood is inversely correlated with the average gaze velocity of large-amplitude control movements. Gaze-evoked blinks induce robust perturbations in eye velocity. Peak and average velocities are reduced, duration is increased, but accuracy is preserved. The temporal features of the perturbation depend on factors such as the time of blink relative to gaze onset, inherent velocity kinematics of control movements, and perhaps initial eye-in-head position. Although variable across animals, the initial effect is a reduction in eye velocity, followed by a reacceleration that yields two or more peaks in its waveform. Interestingly, head velocity is not attenuated; instead, it peaks slightly later and with a larger magnitude. Gaze latency is slightly reduced on trials with gaze-evoked blinks, although the effect was more variable during head-unrestrained movements; no reduction in head latency is observed. Preliminary data also demonstrate a similar perturbation of gaze-evoked blinks during vertical saccades. The results are compared with previously reported effects of reflexive blinks (evoked by air-puff delivered to one eye or supraorbital nerve stimulation) and discussed in terms of effects of blinks on saccadic suppression, neural correlates of the altered eye velocity signals, and implications on the hypothesis that the attenuation in eye velocity is produced by a head movement command.  相似文献   

14.
It has been reported that the visuomotor processing underlying the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movement is modulated in relation to the recent experience of eye movements: the initial pursuit eye velocity is larger after experiencing repeated pursuits than saccades. To assess which parameters of the previously executed pursuits play an essential role in modulating the gain of visuomotor transmission, we recorded the ocular responses of monkeys to a brief perturbing motion of the tracking target injected before the start of the eye movements. First, we compared the perturbation responses among the blocks in which the duration of executing pursuit was varied. We found that the response amplitude increased with the increase of the pursuit duration and it reached a plateau level at 100-200 ms of the duration. Second, a comparison of the perturbation responses in the blocks in which target velocity was different showed a gradual increase of the response as a function of the required pursuit velocity. Third, when the animals repeatedly performed pursuits, the response amplitude gradually increased with increasing interval between the appearance of the target and the onset of perturbation. On the other hand, such an increase was not observed when the animals repeatedly performed saccades. These results suggest that before initiating eye movements, the pursuit system modulates the gain of visuomotor transmission so as to be closely related to the properties of the repeatedly experienced eye movements and this gain modulation is triggered by the target's appearance.  相似文献   

15.
Saccade-related discharge in the superior colliculus is greater for saccades made to a spot of light than for saccades in complete darkness. However, it is unclear whether this enhancement is due to the discontinuity of the spot or due to its being a new object of fixation. In these experiments, we examined the saccade-related activity of intermediate-layer neurons in the primate superior colliculus during delayed saccades to the center or corner of a large, bright square, as well as for visual and memory-guided movements to small spots in isolation. The saccade-related discharge for movements made to a local visual landmark present at the time of the saccade, be it a corner of a square or a small spot, was higher than that for saccades made to the center of a square that contained no local visual landmarks within. Moreover, discharge for movements to the center of a square were very similar to that for saccades to blank, dark space. Saccade velocity was similarly dependent on the presence of such a landmark, though less dramatically. The endpoints of saccades directed toward a square's corner were slightly displaced toward the center of the square. Across all neurons, discharge and velocity for saccades to the center of a square increased as the square size was decreased, but were never greater than those for saccades to a small spot of light. These results suggest that both saccade-related discharge in the superior colliculus and saccade metrics are enhanced for movements directed to parts of the visual scene with high contrast, while shifting fixation to a new object is not itself sufficient to elevate discharge and metrics above those of saccades to blank space.  相似文献   

16.
Eye-head coordination in cats   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Gaze is the position of the visual axis in space and is the sum of the eye movement relative to the head plus head movement relative to space. In monkeys, a gaze shift is programmed with a single saccade that will, by itself, take the eye to a target, irrespective of whether the head moves. If the head turns simultaneously, the saccade is correctly reduced in size (to prevent gaze overshoot) by the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR). Cats have an oculomotor range (OMR) of only about +/- 25 degrees, but their field of view extends to about +/- 70 degrees. The use of the monkey's motor strategy to acquire targets lying beyond +/- 25 degrees requires the programming of saccades that cannot be physically made. We have studied, in cats, rapid horizontal gaze shifts to visual targets within and beyond the OMR. Heads were either totally unrestrained or attached to an apparatus that permitted short unexpected perturbations of the head trajectory. Qualitatively, similar rapid gaze shifts of all sizes up to at least 70 degrees could be accomplished with the classic single-eye saccade and a saccade-like head movement. For gaze shifts greater than 30 degrees, this classic pattern frequently was not observed, and gaze shifts were accomplished with a series of rapid eye movements whose time separation decreased, frequently until they blended into each other, as head velocity increased. Between discrete rapid eye movements, gaze continued in constant velocity ramps, controlled by signals added to the VOR-induced compensatory phase that followed a saccade. When the head was braked just prior to its onset in a 10 degrees gaze shift, the eye attained the target. This motor strategy is the same as that reported for monkeys. However, for larger target eccentricities (e.g., 50 degrees), the gaze shift was interrupted by the brake and the average saccade amplitude was 12-15 degrees, well short of the target and the OMR. Gaze shifts were completed by vestibularly driven eye movements when the head was released. Braking the head during either quick phases driven by passive head displacements or visually triggered saccades resulted in an acceleration of the eye, thereby implying interaction between the VOR and these rapid-eye-movement signals. Head movements possessed a characteristic but task-dependent relationship between maximum velocity and amplitude. Head movements terminated with the head on target. The eye saccade usually lagged the head displacement.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

17.
Tonic and phasic phenomena underlying eye movements during sleep in the cat   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Mammalian sleep is not a homogenous state, and different variables have traditionally been used to distinguish different periods during sleep. Of these variables, eye movement is one of the most paradigmatic, and has been used to differentiate between the so-called rapid eye movement (REM) and non-REM (NREM) sleep periods. Despite this, eye movements during sleep are poorly understood, and the behaviour of the oculomotor system remains almost unknown. In the present work, we recorded binocular eye movements during the sleep–wake cycle of adult cats by the scleral search-coil technique. During alertness, eye movements consisted of conjugated saccades and eye fixations. During NREM sleep, eye movements were slow and mostly unconjugated. The two eyes moved upwardly and in the abducting direction, producing a tonic divergence and elevation of the visual axis. During the transition period between NREM and REM sleep, rapid monocular eye movements of low amplitude in the abducting direction occurred in coincidence with ponto-geniculo-occipital waves. Along REM sleep, the eyes tended to maintain a tonic convergence and depression, broken by high-frequency bursts of complex rapid eye movements. In the horizontal plane, each eye movement in the burst comprised two consecutive movements in opposite directions, which were more evident in the eye that performed the abducting movements. In the vertical plane, rapid eye movements were always upward. Comparisons of the characteristics of eye movements during the sleep–wake cycle reveal the uniqueness of eye movements during sleep, and the noteworthy existence of tonic and phasic phenomena in the oculomotor system, not observed until now.  相似文献   

18.
Inspection of eye saccades made by head-fixed, trained cats revealed the existence of many eye shifts at an approximately constant velocity during the deceleratory phase of the saccade or at the end of it. Slow eye movements occurring at the end of a saccade are usually referred to as “postsaccadic drifts”. It is shown that the duration and mean velocity of these “drifts” are related to the amplitude of the movement. The kinematics of these slow eye movements are nevertheless different from those of saccades. Slow movements at the end of the gaze shift have longer durations than those occurring during the intersaccadic interval between a saccade and a reacceleration of the eye. A closer study of the drifts of three trained cats showed that they play an important corrective role in reducing the residual error at the end of a saccade or during an intersaccadic interval. This functional corrective role was demonstrated by relating the amplitude of the slow movement to the amplitude of the residual error when the slow velocity eye shift began. It is therefore proposed to name these eye shifts “slow correcting movements”.  相似文献   

19.
Responses of relay cells in the A-laminae of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (LGNd) during spontaneous saccades and saccade-like visual stimulation were extracellularly recorded in awake cats. Ninety-six out of 137 cells recorded (42 X and 54 Y cells) were responsive during spontaneous saccadic eye movements. All Y cells and 67% of the X cells responded with burst activity, i.e. with either one or two activity peaks during and after saccades. Thirty-three percent of the X cells were inhibited during saccades. Excitatory peaks occurred at mean latencies of 33 ms and 31 ms for X and Y cells, respectively. Comparable burst responses were obtained when retinal image shifts similar to those during saccades were induced by external saccade-like stimulus movements. However, the latencies of excitatory peak activity were significantly longer to external stimuli than to the onsets of saccades. This indicates the existence of an eye movement-related input which activates LGNd relay cells in addition to the visual input. We propose that the pretectogeniculate projection may contribute to the responses of LGNd relay cells following saccadic eye movements via a disinhibitory input and that this input could be involved in intra- and postsaccadic modulations of the transfer of visual signals to visual cortex.  相似文献   

20.
Ketamine induces failure of the oculomotor neural integrator in the cat   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
We studied the effect of intramuscular injection of low dose of ketamine (1 mg/kg) on the spontaneous ocular movements of the cat. Ketamine is a non-competitive antagonist of the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors, which is used as an anesthetic agent in human surgery. We found that ketamine administration caused a failure of gaze holding: each saccade was followed by a centripetal post-saccadic drift. This defect was selective: the dynamics of the saccades was not altered (the amplitude/maximum velocity relationship was unaffected by ketamine at the dose of 1 mg/kg). We postulated that the observed effect was due to the fact that NMDA receptors were implicated in the network of the oculomotor neural integrator that converted activity related to the saccade (pulse signal) into activity responsible for gaze holding (step signal).  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号