共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Yuncai Chen Christopher S. Rex Courtney J. Rice Céline M. Dubé Christine M. Gall Gary Lynch Tallie Z. Baram 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2010,107(29):13123-13128
Stress affects the hippocampus, a brain region crucial for memory. In rodents, acute stress may reduce density of dendritic spines, the location of postsynaptic elements of excitatory synapses, and impair long-term potentiation and memory. Steroid stress hormones and neurotransmitters have been implicated in the underlying mechanisms, but the role of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH), a hypothalamic hormone also released during stress within hippocampus, has not been elucidated. In addition, the causal relationship of spine loss and memory defects after acute stress is unclear. We used transgenic mice that expressed YFP in hippocampal neurons and found that a 5-h stress resulted in profound loss of learning and memory. This deficit was associated with selective disruption of long-term potentiation and of dendritic spine integrity in commissural/associational pathways of hippocampal area CA3. The degree of memory deficit in individual mice correlated significantly with the reduced density of area CA3 apical dendritic spines in the same mice. Moreover, administration of the CRH receptor type 1 (CRFR1) blocker NBI 30775 directly into the brain prevented the stress-induced spine loss and restored the stress-impaired cognitive functions. We conclude that acute, hours-long stress impairs learning and memory via mechanisms that disrupt the integrity of hippocampal dendritic spines. In addition, establishing the contribution of hippocampal CRH–CRFR1 signaling to these processes highlights the complexity of the orchestrated mechanisms by which stress impacts hippocampal structure and function. 相似文献
2.
Radwanska K Medvedev NI Pereira GS Engmann O Thiede N Moraes MF Villers A Irvine EE Maunganidze NS Pyza EM Ris L Szymańska M Lipiński M Kaczmarek L Stewart MG Giese KP 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2011,108(45):18471-18475
Long-term memory (LTM) formation has been linked with functional strengthening of existing synapses and other processes including de novo synaptogenesis. However, it is unclear whether synaptogenesis can contribute to LTM formation. Here, using α-calcium/calmodulin kinase II autophosphorylation-deficient (T286A) mutants, we demonstrate that when functional strengthening is severely impaired, contextual LTM formation is linked with training-induced PSD95 up-regulation followed by persistent generation of multiinnervated spines, a type of synapse that is characterized by several presynaptic terminals contacting the same postsynaptic spine. Both PSD95 up-regulation and contextual LTM formation in T286A mutants required signaling by the mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR). Furthermore, we show that contextual LTM resists destabilization in T286A mutants, indicating that LTM is less flexible when synaptic strengthening is impaired. Taken together, we suggest that activation of mTOR signaling, followed by overexpression of PSD95 protein and synaptogenesis, contributes to formation of invariant LTM when functional strengthening is impaired. 相似文献
3.
Eun Joo Kim Omer Horovitz Blake A. Pellman Lancy Mimi Tan Qiuling Li Gal Richter-Levin Jeansok J. Kim 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2013,110(36):14795-14800
The periaqueductal gray (PAG) and amygdala are known to be important for defensive responses, and many contemporary fear-conditioning models present the PAG as downstream of the amygdala, directing the appropriate behavior (i.e., freezing or fleeing). However, empirical studies of this circuitry are inconsistent and warrant further examination. Hence, the present study investigated the functional relationship between the PAG and amygdala in two different settings, fear conditioning and naturalistic foraging, in rats. In fear conditioning, electrical stimulation of the dorsal PAG (dPAG) produced unconditional responses (URs) composed of brief activity bursts followed by freezing and 22-kHz ultrasonic vocalization. In contrast, stimulation of ventral PAG and the basolateral amygdalar complex (BLA) evoked freezing and/or ultrasonic vocalization. Whereas dPAG stimulation served as an effective unconditional stimulus for fear conditioning to tone and context conditional stimuli, neither ventral PAG nor BLA stimulation supported fear conditioning. The conditioning effect of dPAG, however, was abolished by inactivation of the BLA. In a foraging task, dPAG and BLA stimulation evoked only fleeing toward the nest. Amygdalar lesion/inactivation blocked the UR of dPAG stimulation, but dPAG lesions did not block the UR of BLA stimulation. Furthermore, in vivo recordings demonstrated that electrical priming of the dPAG can modulate plasticity of subiculum–BLA synapses, providing additional evidence that the amygdala is downstream of the dPAG. These results suggest that the dPAG conveys unconditional stimulus information to the BLA, which directs both innate and learned fear responses, and that brain stimulation-evoked behaviors are modulated by context. 相似文献
4.
Alexandra Karlén Tobias E. Karlsson Anna Mattsson Karin Lundstr?mer Simone Codeluppi Therese M. Pham Cristina M. B?ckman Sven Ove ?gren Elin ?berg Alexander F. Hoffman Michael A. Sherling Carl R. Lupica Barry J. Hoffer Christian Spenger Anna Josephson Stefan Brené Lars Olson 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2009,106(48):20476-20481
Formation of lasting memories is believed to rely on structural alterations at the synaptic level. We had found that increased neuronal activity down-regulates Nogo receptor-1 (NgR1) in brain regions linked to memory formation and storage, and postulated this to be required for formation of lasting memories. We now show that mice with inducible overexpression of NgR1 in forebrain neurons have normal long-term potentiation and normal 24-h memory, but severely impaired month-long memory in both passive avoidance and swim maze tests. Blocking transgene expression normalizes these memory impairments. Nogo, Lingo-1, Troy, endogenous NgR1, and BDNF mRNA expression levels were not altered by transgene expression, suggesting that the impaired ability to form lasting memories is directly coupled to inability to down-regulate NgR1. Regulation of NgR1 may therefore serve as a key regulator of memory consolidation. Understanding the molecular underpinnings of synaptic rearrangements that carry lasting memories may facilitate development of treatments for memory dysfunction. 相似文献
5.
Kim JJ Lee HJ Welday AC Song E Cho J Sharp PE Jung MW Blair HT 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2007,104(46):18297-18302
Acute, inescapable, and unpredictable stress can profoundly modify brain and cognition in humans and animals. The present study investigated the ensuing effects of 2-h variable "audiogenic" stress on three related levels of hippocampal functions in rats: long-term potentiation, place cell activity, and spatial memory. In agreement with prior findings, we observed that stress reduced the magnitude of Schaffer collateral/commissural-Cornu Ammonis field 1 long-term potentiation in vitro, and selectively impaired spatial memory on a hidden platform version of the Morris water maze task. We also observed that stress impaired the stability of firing rates (but not firing locations) of place cells recorded from dorsal Cornu Ammonis field 1 in rats foraging freely on a novel open-field platform located in a familiar surrounding room. These findings suggest that stress-induced modifications in synaptic plasticity may prevent the storage of stable "rate maps" by hippocampal place cells, which in turn may contribute to spatial memory impairments associated with stress. 相似文献
6.
AO Ardiles CC Tapia-Rojas M Mandal F Alexandre A Kirkwood NC Inestrosa AG Palacios 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2012,109(34):13835-13840
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is an age-related neurodegenerative disorder associated with progressive memory loss, severe dementia, and hallmark neuropathological markers, such as deposition of amyloid-β (Aβ) peptides in senile plaques and accumulation of hyperphosphorylated tau proteins in neurofibrillary tangles. Recent evidence obtained from transgenic mouse models suggests that soluble, nonfibrillar Aβ oligomers may induce synaptic failure early in AD. Despite their undoubted value, these transgenic models rely on genetic manipulations that represent the inherited and familial, but not the most abundant, sporadic form of AD. A nontransgenic animal model that still develops hallmarks of AD would be an important step toward understanding how sporadic AD is initiated. Here we show that starting between 12 and 36 mo of age, the rodent Octodon degus naturally develops neuropathological signs of AD, such as accumulation of Aβ oligomers and phosphorylated tau proteins. Moreover, age-related changes in Aβ oligomers and tau phosphorylation levels are correlated with decreases in spatial and object recognition memory, postsynaptic function, and synaptic plasticity. These findings validate O. degus as a suitable natural model for studying how sporadic AD may be initiated. 相似文献
7.
Stephen M. Taubenfeld Elizaveta V. Muravieva Ana Garcia-Osta Cristina M. Alberini 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2010,107(27):12345-12350
Addicts repeatedly relapse to drug seeking even after years of abstinence, and this behavior is frequently induced by the recall of memories of the rewarding effects of the drug. Established memories, including those induced by drugs of abuse, can become transiently fragile if reactivated, and during this labile phase, known as reconsolidation, can be persistently disrupted. Here we show that, in rats, a morphine-induced place preference (mCPP) memory is linked to context-dependent withdrawal as disrupting the reconsolidation of the memory leads to a significant reduction of withdrawal evoked in the same context. Moreover, the hippocampus plays a critical role in linking the place preference memory with the context-conditioned withdrawal, as disrupting hippocampal protein synthesis and cAMP-dependent-protein kinase A after the reactivation of mCPP significantly weakens the withdrawal. Hence, targeting memories induced by drugs may represent an important strategy for attenuating context-conditioned withdrawal and therefore subsequent relapse in opiate addicts. 相似文献
8.
Dai JX Han HL Tian M Cao J Xiu JB Song NN Huang Y Xu TL Ding YQ Xu L 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2008,105(33):11981-11986
Central serotonin (5-HT) dysregulation contributes to the susceptibility for mental disorders, including depression, anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder, and learning and memory deficits. We report that the formation of hippocampus-dependent spatial memory is compromised, but the acquisition and retrieval of contextual fear memory are enhanced, in central 5-HT-deficient mice. Genetic deletion of serotonin in the brain was achieved by inactivating Lmx1b selectively in the raphe nuclei of the brainstem, resulting in a near-complete loss of 5-HT throughout the brain. These 5-HT-deficient mice exhibited no gross abnormality in brain structures and had normal locomotor activity. Spatial learning in the Morris water maze was unaffected, but the retrieval of spatial memory was impaired. In contrast, contextual fear learning and memory induced by foot-shock conditioning was markedly enhanced, but this enhancement could be prevented by intracerebroventricular administration of 5-HT. Foot shock impaired long-term potentiation and facilitated long-term depression in hippocampal slices in WT mice but had no effect in 5-HT-deficient mice. Furthermore, bath application of 5-HT in 5-HT-deficient mice restored foot shock-induced alterations of hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Thus, central 5-HT regulates hippocampus-dependent contextual fear memory, and 5-HT modulation of hippocampal synaptic plasticity may be the underlying mechanism. The enhanced fear memory in 5-HT-deficient mice supports the notion that 5-HT deficiency confers susceptibility to posttraumatic stress disorder in humans. 相似文献
9.
Yuan Ge Zhifang Dong Rosemary C. Bagot John G. Howland Anthony G. Phillips Tak Pan Wong Yu Tian Wang 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2010,107(38):16697-16702
Although NMDA receptor (NMDAR)-dependent long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) of glutamatergic transmission are candidate mechanisms for long-term spatial memory, the precise contributions of LTP and LTD remain poorly understood. Here, we report that LTP and LTD in the hippocampal CA1 region of freely moving adult rats were prevented by NMDAR 2A (GluN2A) and 2B subunit (GluN2B) preferential antagonists, respectively. These results strongly suggest that NMDAR subtype preferential antagonists are appropriate tools to probe the roles of LTP and LTD in spatial memory. Using a Morris water maze task, the LTP-blocking GluN2A antagonist had no significant effect on any aspect of performance, whereas the LTD-blocking GluN2B antagonist impaired spatial memory consolidation. Moreover, similar spatial memory deficits were induced by inhibiting the expression of LTD with intrahippocampal infusion of a short peptide that specifically interferes with AMPA receptor endocytosis. Taken together, our findings support a functional requirement of hippocampal CA1 LTD in the consolidation of long-term spatial memory. 相似文献
10.
Jociane de Carvalho Myskiw Cristiane Regina Guerino Furini Fernando Benetti Ivan Izquierdo 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2014,111(12):4572-4577
Exposure to a novel environment enhances the extinction of contextual fear. This has been explained by tagging of the hippocampal synapses used in extinction, followed by capture of proteins from the synapses that process novelty. The effect is blocked by the inhibition of hippocampal protein synthesis following the novelty or the extinction. Here, we show that it can also be blocked by the postextinction or postnovelty intrahippocampal infusion of the NMDA receptor antagonist 2-amino-5-phosphono pentanoic acid; the inhibitor of calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII), autocamtide-2–related inhibitory peptide; or the blocker of L-voltage–dependent calcium channels (L-VDCCs), nifedipine. Inhibition of proteasomal protein degradation by β-lactacystin has no effect of its own on extinction or on the influence of novelty thereon but blocks the inhibitory effects of all the other substances except that of rapamycin on extinction, suggesting that their action depends on concomitant synaptic protein turnover. Thus, the tagging-and-capture mechanism through which novelty enhances fear extinction involves more molecular processes than hitherto thought: NMDA receptors, L-VDCCs, CaMKII, and synaptic protein turnover.Frey and Morris (1, 2) and their collaborators (3–7) proposed a mechanism whereby relatively “weak” hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) or long-term depression (LTD) lasting only a few minutes can nevertheless “tag” the synapses involved with proteins synthesized ad hoc, so that other plasticity-related proteins (PRPs) produced at other sets of synapses by other LTPs or LTDs can be captured by the tagged synapses and strengthen their activity to “long” LTPs or LTDs lasting hours or days (8). LTDs and LTPs can “cross”-tag each other; that is, LTDs can enhance both LTDs and LTPs, and vice versa (6, 8). Because many learned behaviors rely on hippocampal LTP or LTD (7–9), among them the processing of novelty (9, 10) and the making of extinction (11–13), interactions between consecutive learnings can also be explained by the “tagging-and-capture” hypothesis (9, 10, 13), whose application to behavior became known as “behavioral tagging and capture” (5, 7, 9, 13). Typically, exposure to a novel environment [e.g., a nonanxiogenic 50 × 50 × 40-cm open field (OF) (5, 7, 9, 10, 14)] is interpolated before testing for another task, which becomes enhanced (4–10, 13). The usual reaction to novelty is orienting and exploration (14), followed by habituation of this response (14–16). Habituation is perhaps the simplest form of learning, and it consists of inhibition of the orienting/exploratory response (14, 16).We recently showed that the brief exposure of rats to a novel environment (the OF) within a limited time window enhances the extinction of contextual fear conditioning (CFC) through a mechanism of synaptic tagging and capture (13), which is a previously unidentified example of behavioral tagging of inhibitory learning. Fear extinction is most probably due to LTD in the hippocampus (11, 12), although the possibility that it may also involve LTP is not discarded (13). The enhancement of extinction by novelty probably relies on the habituation to the novel environment, which is also probably due to LTD (15, 16). The enhancement of extinction by the exposure to novelty depends on hippocampal gene expression and ribosomal protein synthesis following extinction training and on both ribosomal and nonribosomal protein synthesis caused by the novel experience (13). Nonribosomal protein synthesis that can be blocked by rapamycin is believed to be dendritic (13, 17), so it would be strategically located for tagging-and-capture processes, but it has not been studied in synaptic tagging to date (3–8) or in other forms of behavioral tagging (7–10). As occurs with the interactions between LTPs and/or LTDs (4), the enhancement of extinction by novelty relies on hippocampal but not amygdalar processes (13).Recent findings indicate that several hippocampal processes related to learning and memory, such as the reconsolidation of spatial learning, are highly dependent on NMDA glutamate receptors, calcium/calmodulin protein kinase II (CaMKII), and long-term voltage channel blockers (L-VDCCs), which, in turn, rely on the proteasomal degradation of proteins (18). Here, we study the effects of an NMDA blocker, 2-amino-5-phosphono pentanoic acid (AP5); the L-VDCC blocker nifedipine (Nife); a CaMKII inhibitor, the autocamtide-2–related inhibitory peptide (AIP); and the irreversible proteasome blocker β-lactacystin (12, 13) on the interaction between novelty and extinction (11). As will be seen, we found that both the setting up of tags by extinction and the presumable production of PRPs by the processing of novelty are dependent on NMDA receptors, CaMKII, and L-VDCCs. This endorses and expands the hypothesis that the novelty–extinction interaction relies on synaptic tagging and capture (13). 相似文献
11.
Jae Hoon Jung Lyndsey M. Kirk Jennifer N. Bourne Kristen M. Harris 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2021,118(17)
Long-term potentiation (LTP) is a cellular mechanism of learning and memory that results in a sustained increase in the probability of vesicular release of neurotransmitter. However, previous work in hippocampal area CA1 of the adult rat revealed that the total number of vesicles per synapse decreases following LTP, seemingly inconsistent with the elevated release probability. Here, electron-microscopic tomography (EMT) was used to assess whether changes in vesicle density or structure of vesicle tethering filaments at the active zone might explain the enhanced release probability following LTP. The spatial relationship of vesicles to the active zone varies with functional status. Tightly docked vesicles contact the presynaptic membrane, have partially formed SNARE complexes, and are primed for release of neurotransmitter upon the next action potential. Loosely docked vesicles are located within 8 nm of the presynaptic membrane where SNARE complexes begin to form. Nondocked vesicles comprise recycling and reserve pools. Vesicles are tethered to the active zone via filaments composed of molecules engaged in docking and release processes. The density of tightly docked vesicles was increased 2 h following LTP compared to control stimulation, whereas the densities of loosely docked or nondocked vesicles congregating within 45 nm above the active zones were unchanged. The tethering filaments on all vesicles were shorter and their attachment sites shifted closer to the active zone. These findings suggest that tethering filaments stabilize more vesicles in the primed state. Such changes would facilitate the long-lasting increase in release probability following LTP.Long-term potentiation (LTP) is the persistent strengthening of synapses after a brief high-frequency stimulation and is widely accepted as a cellular correlate of learning and memory (1, 2). Within minutes after the induction of LTP, new receptors are inserted into the postsynaptic membrane. The resulting increase in the excitatory postsynaptic potential is immediate and can persist for hours in vitro or days to months in vivo (1, 3–8). Quantal content is also increased soon after LTP induction and reflects an increase in the number of presynaptic vesicles that release neurotransmitter (9–13). This increase in release probability is sustained several hours following LTP (14), concurrent with postsynaptic growth and spine enlargement (15). One might expect that the enhanced probability of release would involve increasing the number of vesicles docked and primed for neurotransmitter release. However, 2 h after induction of LTP, the total number of both docked and nondocked vesicles per presynaptic bouton are markedly decreased relative to control stimulation (16). These findings raise the question of whether an altered structure of docking and priming molecules leads to local clustering of vesicles that would elevate the probability of release following LTP.The proteins that connect synaptic vesicles to the plasma membrane can be visualized as filaments with electron-microscopic tomography (EMT) connecting vesicles to the presynaptic active zone (17–24). Studies suggest that the SNARE complex begins to form when a vesicle and presynaptic membrane are within 8 nm of each other rendering them loosely docked (25–27). Vesicles are then drawn toward the active zone, and the SNARE complex bundle is fully formed when the vesicle is within 2 nm of the presynaptic membrane (26, 28). Tightly docked vesicles are defined as being in contact with the presynaptic membrane and correspond to primed vesicles that comprise the readily releasable pool (27). Recent studies have suggested that docked vesicles can oscillate between loosely and tightly docked states (27, 29), providing a target mechanism for synaptic plasticity.To address the question of how changes in vesicle proximity and tethering might enhance the probability of release, we used EMT, which enabled us to acquire high-resolution structural data from small volumes of presynaptic boutons that were enriched in synaptic vesicles. We targeted active zones of hippocampal synapses, comparing their structure 2 h after LTP induction to control stimulation. The vesicle density and tethering filament dimensions were unchanged for the loosely docked and nondocked vesicles. In contrast, the density of tightly docked vesicles was increased, their tethering filaments were shorter, and the filament attachment sites on the vesicles were positioned closer to the side of the vesicle membrane facing the presynaptic membrane. Such alterations could contribute to the sustained increase in the probability of neurotransmitter release following LTP. 相似文献
12.
Wang SH Redondo RL Morris RG 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2010,107(45):19537-19542
Memory for inconsequential events fades, unless these happen before or after other novel or surprising events. However, our understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms of novelty-enhanced memory persistence is mainly restricted to aversive or fear-associated memories. We now outline an "everyday appetitive" behavioral model to examine whether and how unrelated novelty facilitates the persistence of spatial memory coupled to parallel electrophysiological studies of the persistence of long-term potentiation (LTP). Across successive days, rats were given one trial per day to find food in different places and later had to recall that day's location. This task is both hippocampus and NMDA receptor dependent. First, encoding with low reward induced place memory that decayed over 24 h; in parallel, weak tetanization of CA1 synapses in brain slices induced early-LTP fading to baseline. Second, novelty exploration scheduled 30 min after this weak encoding resulted in persistent place memory; similarly, strong tetanization--analogous to novelty--both induced late-LTP and rescued early- into late-LTP on an independent but convergent pathway. Third, hippocampal dopamine D1/D5 receptor blockade or protein synthesis inhibition within 15 min of exploration prevented persistent place memory and blocked late-LTP. Fourth, symmetrically, when spatial memory was encoded using strong reward, this memory persisted for 24 h unless encoding occurred under hippocampal D1/D5 receptor blockade. Novelty exploration before this encoding rescued the drug-induced memory impairment. Parallel effects were observed in LTP. These findings can be explained by the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. 相似文献
13.
14.
Christine N. Smith Annette Jeneson Jennifer C. Frascino C. Brock Kirwan Ramona O. Hopkins Larry R. Squire 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2014,111(27):9935-9940
Hippocampal damage has been thought to result in broad memory impairment. Recent studies in humans, however, have raised the possibility that recognition memory for faces might be spared. In five experiments we investigated face recognition in patients with hippocampal lesions (H) or large medial temporal lobe (MTL) lesions, including patients where neurohistological information was available. Recognition of novel faces was unequivocally intact in H patients but only at a short retention interval. Recognition memory for words, buildings, inverted faces, and famous faces was impaired. For MTL patients, recognition memory was impaired for all materials and across all retention intervals. These results indicate that structures other than the hippocampus, perhaps the perirhinal cortex, can support face recognition memory in H patients under some conditions. The fact that the faces were novel when recognition memory was intact does not fully account for our findings. We propose that the role of the hippocampus in recognition memory is related to how recognition decisions are accomplished. In typical recognition tasks, participants proceed by forming an association between a study item and the study list, and the recognition decision is later made based on whether participants believe the item was on the study list. We suggest that face recognition is an exception to this principle and that, at short retention intervals, participants can make their recognition decisions without making explicit reference to the study list. Important features of faces that might make face recognition exceptional are that they are processed holistically and are difficult to verbally label.The hippocampus and related medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures are essential for the formation of long-term declarative memory. Damage to these structures has traditionally been thought to result in a broad memory impairment that extends across all sensory modalities, across all domains of material (e.g., scenes, words, objects), and across different testing methods (e.g., recall and recognition) (1–3).An early hint that the impairment might not be so pervasive came from a study of recognition memory in a mixed group of memory-impaired patients (4). Three patients thought to have hippocampal lesions were impaired at word recognition but not at face recognition. Subsequently, recognition scores on the same test were reported for a larger group (n = 6, including the earlier three patients). Again, word recognition was poor, but face recognition was variable and not significantly impaired (P < 0.08) (5). Because of this marginal finding, and the finding that face recognition was unequivocally impaired at a retention delay of 24 h (5, 6), the possibility that face recognition might be special in some way was not noted. Face recognition also drew no comment when it was reported to be intact in a single patient (patient BE) (7).An early review of the literature suggested that the performance of hippocampal patients was good for face recognition but concluded that performance was also good for recognition memory more broadly (8). Similarly, an extensive study of hippocampal patient YR emphasized the relative sparing of recognition memory, including face memory, against a background of impaired recall (9). Other studies suggested that what was spared was recognition of nonverbal material (10) or single-item recognition, compared with associative recognition (11).Perhaps the first proposal that the capacity for face recognition itself deserved special attention came from the study of a patient with hippocampal lesions and spared face recognition memory but impaired recognition memory for words and buildings (12). Subsequently, three single-case studies and one group study (n = 3) also reported spared memory for faces after hippocampal lesions and impaired memory for other kinds of nonverbal material (e.g., buildings or scenes) (13–16). The evidence for sparing of face recognition became even stronger when scores on the Recognition Memory Test [RMT, a standard memory test for faces and words (17)] were brought together for 10 patients with hippocampal lesions from earlier studies (18). The findings were unmistakable. Recognition memory was impaired for words but intact for faces. The authors suggested that hippocampal lesions might spare recognition memory for material that, like faces, was unfamiliar to patients before testing.In five experiments, we explored the conditions under which face recognition memory might be spared in patients with hippocampal lesions (H) or larger lesions of the MTL. We first examined scores on a standard test (the RMT). For four patients (three H patients and one MTL patient) neurohistological information was available to characterize the lesions. For six patients the lesions were characterized by quantitative neuroimaging (Table S1). Testing occurred both immediately after study and 24 h later (Exp. 1). Next, for the six patients still living (five H patients, one MTL patient), we assessed recognition memory for faces, buildings, and words across four retention intervals from immediate to 1 d (Exp. 2). We then tested face recognition memory again after matching the difficulty of the faces and buildings tests (Exp. 3). Next, we tested recognition memory for faces that were potentially familiar to participants before testing (i.e., famous faces from before 1970; Exp. 4). Finally, we tested recognition memory for upside-down faces (Exp. 5). 相似文献
15.
Dentate gyrus-specific manipulation of beta-Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent kinase II disrupts memory consolidation 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Cho MH Cao X Wang D Tsien JZ 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2007,104(41):16317-16322
Although the functions of alpha-Ca(2+)/calmodulin-dependent kinase II (CaMKII) have been studied extensively, the role of betaCaMKII, a coconstituent of the CaMKII holoenzyme in synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory has not been examined in vivo. Here we produce a transgenic mouse line in which the inducible and reversible manipulation of betaCaMKII activity is restricted to the hippocampal dentate gyrus, the region where long-term potentiation was originally discovered. We demonstrate that betaCaMKII activity in the dentate gyrus selectively impaired long-term potentiation in the dentate perforant path, but not in the CA1 Schaffer collateral pathway. Although the transgenic mice showed normal 1-day memories, they were severely impaired in 10-day contextual fear memory. Systematic manipulations of dentate betaCaMKII activity during various distinct memory stages further reveal the initial day within the postlearning consolidation period as a critical time window that is highly sensitive to changes in betaCaMKII activity. This study provides evidence not only for the functional role of betaCaMKII in the dentate gyrus plasticity and hippocampal memory, but also for the notion that the mismatch between the actual learning pattern and reactivation patterns in the dentate gyrus circuit can underlie long-term memory consolidation. 相似文献
16.
17.
Olena Bukalo Emilie Campanac Dax A. Hoffman R. Douglas Fields 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2013,110(13):5175-5180
Learning and other cognitive tasks require integrating new experiences into context. In contrast to sensory-evoked synaptic plasticity, comparatively little is known of how synaptic plasticity may be regulated by intrinsic activity in the brain, much of which can involve nonclassical modes of neuronal firing and integration. Coherent high-frequency oscillations of electrical activity in CA1 hippocampal neurons [sharp-wave ripple complexes (SPW-Rs)] functionally couple neurons into transient ensembles. These oscillations occur during slow-wave sleep or at rest. Neurons that participate in SPW-Rs are distinguished from adjacent nonparticipating neurons by firing action potentials that are initiated ectopically in the distal region of axons and propagate antidromically to the cell body. This activity is facilitated by GABAA-mediated depolarization of axons and electrotonic coupling. The possible effects of antidromic firing on synaptic strength are unknown. We find that facilitation of spontaneous SPW-Rs in hippocampal slices by increasing gap-junction coupling or by GABAA-mediated axon depolarization resulted in a reduction of synaptic strength, and electrical stimulation of axons evoked a widespread, long-lasting synaptic depression. Unlike other forms of synaptic plasticity, this synaptic depression is not dependent upon synaptic input or glutamate receptor activation, but rather requires L-type calcium channel activation and functional gap junctions. Synaptic stimulation delivered after antidromic firing, which was otherwise too weak to induce synaptic potentiation, triggered a long-lasting increase in synaptic strength. Rescaling synaptic weights in subsets of neurons firing antidromically during SPW-Rs might contribute to memory consolidation by sharpening specificity of subsequent synaptic input and promoting incorporation of novel information. 相似文献
18.
Agnihotri NT Hawkins RD Kandel ER Kentros C 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2004,101(10):3656-3661
The hippocampus is critical for formation of spatial memories. Hippocampal pyramidal neurons in freely behaving animals exhibit spatially selective firing patterns, which taken together form an internal representation of the environment. This representation is thought to contribute to the hippocampal spatial memory system. Behavioral long-term memories differ from short-term memories in requiring the synthesis of new proteins. Does the development of the internal hippocampal representation also require the synthesis of new proteins? We found that blocking protein synthesis in the brain of mice by 95% does not affect short-term stability of newly formed hippocampal place fields but abolishes stability in the long term. By contrast, inhibiting protein synthesis does not affect the retention and recall of previously established fields in a familiar environment, indicating that protein synthesis-dependent reconsolidation is not required for recall. Our results indicate that place fields parallel both behavioral memories and the late phase of long-term potentiation in requiring the synthesis of new proteins for consolidation. 相似文献
19.
Sarah Emerson Lee Stephen B. Simons Scott A. Heldt Meilan Zhao Jason P. Schroeder Christopher P. Vellano D. Patrick Cowan Suneela Ramineni Cindee K. Yates Yue Feng Yoland Smith J. David Sweatt David Weinshenker Kerry J. Ressler Serena M. Dudek John R. Hepler 《Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America》2010,107(39):16994-16998
Learning and memory have been closely linked to strengthening of synaptic connections between neurons (i.e., synaptic plasticity) within the dentate gyrus (DG)–CA3–CA1 trisynaptic circuit of the hippocampus. Conspicuously absent from this circuit is area CA2, an intervening hippocampal region that is poorly understood. Schaffer collateral synapses on CA2 neurons are distinct from those on other hippocampal neurons in that they exhibit a perplexing lack of synaptic long-term potentiation (LTP). Here we demonstrate that the signaling protein RGS14 is highly enriched in CA2 pyramidal neurons and plays a role in suppression of both synaptic plasticity at these synapses and hippocampal-based learning and memory. RGS14 is a scaffolding protein that integrates G protein and H-Ras/ERK/MAP kinase signaling pathways, thereby making it well positioned to suppress plasticity in CA2 neurons. Supporting this idea, deletion of exons 2–7 of the RGS14 gene yields mice that lack RGS14 (RGS14-KO) and now express robust LTP at glutamatergic synapses in CA2 neurons with no impact on synaptic plasticity in CA1 neurons. Treatment of RGS14-deficient CA2 neurons with a specific MEK inhibitor blocked this LTP, suggesting a role for ERK/MAP kinase signaling pathways in this process. When tested behaviorally, RGS14-KO mice exhibited marked enhancement in spatial learning and in object recognition memory compared with their wild-type littermates, but showed no differences in their performance on tests of nonhippocampal-dependent behaviors. These results demonstrate that RGS14 is a key regulator of signaling pathways linking synaptic plasticity in CA2 pyramidal neurons to hippocampal-based learning and memory but distinct from the canonical DG–CA3–CA1 circuit. 相似文献