首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Isoflurane and sevoflurane are both inhalation anesthetics,but in clinical application,sevoflurane has been considered to be less suitable for long-term anesthesia because of its catabolic compounds and potential nephrotoxicity.Nevertheless,recent studies have shown that these two inhalation anesthetics are similar in hepatorenal toxicity,cost,and long-term anesthetic effect.Moreover,sevoflurane possibly has less cognitive impact on young mice.In this study,C57BL/6 mice aged 8–10 weeks were exposed to 1.2%isoflurane or 2.4%sevoflurane for 6 hours.Cognitive function and memory were examined in young mice using the novel object recognition,contextual fear conditioning,and cued-fear extinction tests.Western blot assay was performed to detect expression levels of D1 dopamine receptor,catechol-O-methyltransferase,phospho-glycogen synthase kinase-3β,and total glycogen synthase kinase-3βin the hippocampus.Our results show that impaired performance was not detected in mice exposed to sevoflurane during the novel object recognition test.Contextual memory impairment in the fear conditioning test was shorter in the sevoflurane group than the isoflurane group.Long-term sevoflurane exposure did not affect memory consolidation,while isoflurane led to memory consolidation and reduced retention.Downregulation of hippocampal D1 dopamine receptors and phosphorylated glycogen synthase kinase-3β/total glycogen synthase kinase-3βand upregulation of catechol-O-methyltransferase may be associated with differing memory performance after exposure to isoflurane or sevoflurane.These results confirm that sevoflurane has less effect on cognitive impairment than isoflurane,which may be related to expression of D1 dopamine receptors and catechol-O-methyltransferase and phosphorylation of glycogen synthase kinase-3βin the hippocampus.This study was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee,Nanjing University,China on November 20,2017(approval No.20171102).  相似文献   

2.
3.
The cerebellum plays a role in conscious episodic memory retrieval   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
The cerebellum has traditionally been considered to be primarily dedicated to motor functions. Its phylogenetic development and connectivity suggest, however, that it also may play a role in cognitive processes in the human brain. In order to examine a potential cognitive role for the cerebellum in human beings, a positron emission tomography (PET) study was conducted during a "pure thought experiment": subjects intentionally recalled a specific past personal experience (consciously retrieved episodic memory). Since there was no motor or sensory input or output, the design eliminated the possibility that cerebellar changes in blood flow were due to motor activity. During silent recall of a consciously retrieved episodic memory, activations were observed in the right lateral cerebellum, left medial dorsal thalamus, medial and left orbital frontal cortex, anterior cingulate, and a left parietal region. These activations confirm a cognitive role for the cerebellum, which may participate in an interactive cortical-cerebellar network that initiates and monitors the conscious retrieval of episodic memory.  相似文献   

4.
Memory is thought to be subserved by structural and functional alteration in synaptic connectivity. But although neuronal plasticity requires gene expression, the identity of the proteins involved is largely unknown. Using the chick 1-day-old passive avoidance learning paradigm and differential display RNA fingerprinting, we identified 13 candidate genes which are upregulated in the intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale (IMHV), an area that has been correlated with the initial processing of memory formation. One of the induced genes is a new member of the cyclin family, with high homology to cyclin L (ania-6a). Analysis of the expression pattern of this gene after training revealed two time waves of induction: the first correlated with learning and initial memory process in the IMHV; the second correlated with memory consolidation, first in the IMHV, and then in the lobus paraolefactoris. There is a correlation between methylanthranilate (MeA) concentrations (the malaise substrate in the passive avoidance training procedure), the duration of memory and the expression level of cyclin S. While training chicks on low concentrations of MeA causes short-term memory and low expression level of cyclin S, high concentration of MeA induces long-term memory and high expression level of cyclin S in the IMHV. The role of cyclins in the regulation of neuronal-plasticity-related gene expression was overlooked, and it might serve as a key step in long-term memory formation.  相似文献   

5.
Function of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), which connects the anterior temporal and occipital lobes, has not been clearly demonstrated in the human brain. A 47-year-old woman with visual memory disturbance as demonstrated by the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised presented with possible brain tumor in the right temporal lobe. Diffusion tensor imaging showed partial disconnection of the right ILF, indicating that function of the ILF is highly involved in visual memory.  相似文献   

6.
Sympathetic neurons release both urokinase plasminogen activator (uPA) and tissue plasminogen activator (tPA). A number of inhibitors of serine proteases have been tested to determine their effects on neurite outgrowth from rat sympathetic neurons. Some inhibitors increase neurite outgrowth while others have little or no effect on outgrowth. Inhibition of plasminogen activator (PA) activity but not other serine protease activity correlates with the increase in neurite outgrowth (uPA, r = 0.89; tPA, r = 0.86; plasmin, r = 0.015; thrombin, r = 0.025). Antibodies that inhibit uPA activity increase neurite outgrowth, while antibodies that bind to uPA but do not inhibit activity do not alter outgrowth. Time-lapse videomicroscopy of neurite outgrowth indicates that about 85% of the neurites increase their rate of outgrowth following exposure to inhibitors of PA. Routinely, 1-2 min after exposure of a growth cone to an inhibitor, there is an increase in lamellipodial activity at the leading edge of the growth cone and a decrease in lamellipodial activity on the sides and base of the growth cone. The increase in the rate of outgrowth combined with the decrease in lamellipodial activity on the sides of the growth cones results in neurites being very long and straight in the presence of inhibitors (persistence time P = 3.7 and 15.3 hr for controls and in the presence of inhibitors of PA, respectively). PAs released from sympathetic neurons and PC12 cells interact with 3 different binding sites on the cell surface: (1) an inhibitor of serine proteases (including uPA and tPA) is bound to the surface via a heparinase-sensitive site; (2) a uPA-selective binding site is present in patches on the bottom surface of PC12 cells; and (3) a tPA-selective binding site with high affinity (KD = 23 +/- 10 nM) and high capacity (340,000 +/- 130,000 sites/neuron) for 125I-tPA is homogeneously distributed over the entire surface. Data in the present study are consistent with PA being involved in neurite outgrowth and open the possibility of other PA-dependent functions occurring when tPA and/or uPA interacts with cell surface binding sites.  相似文献   

7.
8.
Memory formation has been associated with structural and functional modifications of synapses. Cell adhesion molecules are prominent modulators of synaptic plasticity. Here, we investigated the involvement of the cell adhesion molecules, NCAM, its polysialylated state (PSA-NCAM) and L1 in spatial learning-induced synaptic remodeling and memory storage. A differential regulation of these adhesion molecules was found in the hippocampus of rats submitted to one training session in the spatial, but not cued, version of the Morris water maze. Twenty-four hours after training, synaptic expression of NCAM and PSA-NCAM was increased, whereas L1 appeared markedly decreased. The regulation of these molecules was spatial learning-specific, except for L1 reduction, which could be attributed to swimming under stressful conditions rather than to learning. Subsequent psychopharmacological experiments were performed to address the functional role of NCAM and PSA-NCAM in the formation of spatial memories. Rats received an intracerebroventricular injection of either a synthetic peptide (C3d) aimed to interfere with NCAM function, or endoneuraminidase, an enzyme that cleaves polysialic acid from NCAM. Both treatments affected acquisition of spatial information and lead to impaired spatial memory abilities, supporting a critical role of the observed learning-induced up-regulation of synaptic NCAM expression and polysialylation on spatial learning and memory. Therefore, our findings highlight NCAM as a learning-modulated molecule critically involved in the hippocampal remodeling processes underlying spatial memory formation.  相似文献   

9.
The sulfhydryl alkylating reagent N-ethylmaleimide (NEM) blocks opioid receptor binding and receptor/G-protein coupling. Sodium partially restores [(3)H]naloxone binding after inhibition by NEM to reveal sodium-dependent [(3)H]naloxone sites, defined as binding in the presence of 50-100 mM NaCl after treatment of membranes or sections with 750 microM NEM. In the present study, receptor autoradiography of [(3)H]naloxone binding in control and NEM-treated tissue was used to examine the anatomical distribution of sodium-dependent [(3)H]naloxone sites in rat brain. In brain membranes, the pharmacology of sodium-dependent [(3)H]naloxone sites was consistent with that of mu opioid receptors. Relatively high IC(50) values for agonists and lack of effect of Gpp(NH)p on DAMGO displacement of [(3)H]naloxone binding in NEM-treated membranes indicated that the sodium-dependent sites were low affinity sites, presumably uncoupled from G-proteins. Autoradiograms revealed that NEM treatment dramatically reduced [(3)H]naloxone binding in all brain regions. However, [(3)H]naloxone binding was increased in specific regions in NEM-treated sections in the presence of sodium, including bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, interpeduncular nucleus, periaqueductal gray, parabrachial nucleus, locus coeruleus, and commissural nucleus tractus solitarius. Sodium-dependent [(3)H]naloxone binding sites were not found in other areas that exhibited [(3)H]naloxone binding in control tissue, including the striatum and thalamus. These studies revealed the presence of a subpopulation of [(3)H]naloxone binding sites which are sodium-dependent and have a unique regional distribution in the rat brain.  相似文献   

10.
The hypothesis that hippocampal activity at encoding is causally related to subsequent declarative memory expression is tested in the mouse, by using lidocaine inactivation of the hippocampus in combination with c-fos neuroimaging analysis. We employed a two-stage radial maze paradigm of spatial discrimination, which was previously shown to dissociate between declarative and nondeclarative expression of memory related to the same acquired material. In Stage 1 (encoding), mice learnt the constant location of food among a set of six arms (three baited, three unbaited) by being submitted repeatedly to discontiguous experiences with each arm separately ("go/no-go" discrimination). In Stage 2 (test-session), they are challenged with novel presentations of the arms, which are either combined into pairs of opposite valence ("two-choice" discrimination), or opened all six together ("six-choice" discrimination). Previous experiments have demonstrated that the "two-choice" situation is a critical test for declarative memory while "six-choice" discrimination may rely on procedural memory. We observed that (i) hippocampal activity measured by c-fos mRNA expression was increased by "go/no-go" learning, and this activation was blocked by pre-training local infusions of lidocaine; (ii) when performed just before each session of Stage 1, such inactivation spared the acquisition of "go/no-go" discrimination but produced, subsequently, a selective deficit in the "two-choice" test (not in the "six-choice" test). This study indicates that the hippocampus is "spontaneously" engaged in encoding processes necessary for long-term storage of discontiguous experiences under a form enabling flexible declarative memory expression.  相似文献   

11.
Alterations of excitant amino acid (EAA) action are implicated in seizure susceptibility in the genetically epilepsy-prone rat (GEPR). The inferior colliculus (IC) is critical for audiogenic seizure (AGS) initiation in the GEPR. The present study observed that bilateral microinjection into the IC of L-canaline, a glutamate synthesis inhibitor, decreased AGS severity in the GEPR and also decreased potassium-evoked release of glutamate from IC slices. Bilateral microinjection of NMDA receptor antagonists, 2-amino-7-phosphonoheptanoate (AP7) or 3-((+/-)-2-carboxypiperazin-4-yl)-propyl-1-phosphonate (CPP) into IC blocked AGS, and an antagonist at non-NMDA EAA receptors, 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (CNQX), also blocked AGS. NMDA receptor antagonists were 5-200 times more effective than CNQX. Microinjection of a non-competitive NMDA receptor antagonist, dizocilpine (MK-801), into IC had little effect except with very high doses. Microinjection of CPP or AP7 into the IC blocked AGS at considerably lower doses as compared to pontine reticular formation (PRF). However, MK-801 attenuated AGS when microinjected into PRF at doses that were ineffective in IC. Systemically administered CPP blocked AGS and significantly reduced IC neuronal firing in the behaving GEPR, suggesting an important action of systemically administered NMDA receptor antagonists on brainstem auditory nuclei critical to AGS. The present results support a critical role for glutamate acting, in part, through NMDA receptors in IC in initiation of AGS.  相似文献   

12.
Achievements made over the last years have highlighted the important role of creatine in health and disease. However, studies of its effect on cognition function have been limited. In the present study, we investigated the effect of creatine on early consolidation of the spatial learning in rats. Statistical analysis showed that intrahippocampal administration of creatine (2.5 and 7.5 nmol/hippocampus) (post-training) decreased the latency for scape and mean number of errors in Barnes maze test. The involvement of polyamine binding site at NMDA receptor in creatine-induced spatial learning enhancement was investigated by co-administration of arcaine (0.02 nmol/hippocampus) or spermidine (0.02 nmol/hippocampus) with creatine (2.5 nmol/hippocampus) (post-training). Statistical analysis revealed that creatine-induced spatial learning enhancement was reverted by co-administration of arcaine (0.02 nmol/hippocampus) and intensified by spermidine (0.02 nmol/hippocampus). These results provide evidence that creatine not only seem to be involved in energy metabolism but may also play an important role in early consolidation of spatial learning in hippocampus which participation of polyamines binding site at the NMDA receptor.  相似文献   

13.
The distribution of benzodiazepine binding sites was analysed in limbic structures of rat brain by quantitative radioautography of brain sections incubated with 3H-flunitrazepam (3H-FLU). Quantitative estimation of the binding parameters was made in each range of postero-anterior sections taken. Distribution of 3H-FLU binding sites was found to be rather homogeneous in most of the structures examined but there were regional differences which resulted from variations in the densities of sites rather than in their affinities. A particular distribution pattern of 3H-FLU binding sites was observed in the cingulate cortex contrasting with the homogeneous postero-anterior distribution measured in other cortical areas in the same slices. A significantly greater density of sites was found in the anterior part of the structure as compared to the posterior part. This difference, which corresponds to a change in the density of sites without alteration of their apparent affinity and occurs at a precise anatomical level, is discussed with reference to the anatomical organization of this brain structure and to its possible functional implications.  相似文献   

14.
15.
The possibility of area-specific changes in binding sites for CGRP in response to stress was studied in cat CNS after repeated sleep-deprivation and restriction of movement. Brain sections were obtained from a cat placed under stressful conditions for 2 h the 1st day, 6 h the 2nd day and 24 h the 3rd day. Changes in CGRP binding sites were evaluated by an in vitro autoradiographic technique with 125I-Tyr-rat-CGRP as a ligand. The autoradiograms were then compared with those of control animals. The results show decreased labelling in the cortex prefrontalis and pyriformis and in some basal ganglia (n. caudatus, claustrum, n. entopedencularis). Increased CGRP binding site densities were seen in areas involved in the integration of sensory information, in the control of endocrine secretion and in those that participate in sleep-walking cycles. These changes in CGRP binding in selective CNS areas following stress suggests that CGRP plays a role in processes of adaptation.  相似文献   

16.
The selective in vivo blockade of high affinity (mu1) opioid binding sites in mice by naloxazone reduced the analgesic potency of opiates and opioid peptides, evidenced by a shift of their analgesic ED50 values. However, the extent of these shifts varied significantly between a series of opioid drugs, ranging from 12-fold for morphine to 4-fold for D-Ala2-D-Leu5-enkephalin. These findings suggested that analgesia in naloxazone-treated animals is mediated through a different subpopulation of receptors than in normal controls. Correlating these analgesic shifts for a series of opioids with their affinity for different [3H]opioid binding sites suggested an analgesic role for delta sites. Additional studies in mice with spinal transections suggested that mu1 analgesia was primarily supraspinal.  相似文献   

17.
There is evidence that sleep supports the enhancement of implicit as well as explicit memories (i.e., two memory systems that during learning normally appear to act together). Here, employing a serial reaction time task (SRTT) paradigm, we examined the question whether sleep can provide explicit knowledge on an implicitly acquired skill. At learning, young healthy subjects (n = 20) were first trained on the SRTT. Then, implicit knowledge was assessed on two test blocks, in which grammatically incorrect target positions were occasionally interspersed by the difference in reaction times between grammatically correct and incorrect target positions. To assess explicit sequence knowledge, thereafter subjects performed on a generation task in which they were explicitly instructed to predict the sequential target positions. In half the subjects, learning took place before a 9-hour retention interval filled with nocturnal sleep (sleep group), in the other half, the retention interval covered a 9-hour period of daytime wakefulness (wake group). At subsequent retesting, both testing on the generation task and the SRTT test blocks was repeated. At learning before the retention interval, subjects displayed significant implicit sequence knowledge which was comparable for the sleep and wake groups. Moreover, both groups did not display any explicit sequence knowledge as indicated by a prediction performance not differing from chance on the generation task. However, at retesting, there was a distinct gain in explicit knowledge in the subjects who had slept in the retention interval, whereas generation task performance in the wake group remained at chance level. SRTT performance in the test blocks at retesting did not indicate any further gain in skill (i.e., unchanged reaction time differences between grammatically correct and incorrect target positions) independently of whether subjects had slept or remained awake after learning. Our results indicate a selective enhancement of explicit memory formation during sleep. Because before sleep subjects only had implicit knowledge on the sequence of target transitions, these data point to an interaction between implicit and explicit memory systems during sleep-dependent off-line learning.  相似文献   

18.
The effects of morphine and naloxone on nigrostriatal function were evaluated by their influence on rotational behavior in rats with unilateral lesions of the substantial nigra. Two different rotational syndromes which result from different lesion placements, were examined. Rats with the contraversive syndrome, when given apomorphine, rotate away from the lesioned side, while rats with the ipsiversive syndrome rotate toward the lesioned side. In both syndromes, rats rotate toward the lesioned side when given amphetamine. Morphine or naloxone, alone, was without effect in either syndrome. Morphine antagonized rotation by either apomorphine or amphetamine in both syndromes. Naloxone stimulated apomorphine-induced rotation in contraversive rats and antagonized amphetamine-induced rotation in ipsiversive rats. These findings support a functional role of endogenous opioids in this dopaminergic system. The effects of morphine and naloxone on apomorphine-induced rotation indicate that opiates act at a postsynaptic site in this system. Finally, the different responses to naloxone and morphine in the two rotational syndromes suggest that an enkephalinergic assymetry may underlie the differences in behavioral responses between these two syndromes.  相似文献   

19.
Rats (220-250 g) were bilaterally implanted with cannulae in the amygdala, trained on an inhibitory avoidance response and two weeks later, on a Y-maze discrimination response. Immediately following the training on each task, they were injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) or intra-amygdally. Retention was tested one week after training for each task. Retention of the Y-maze task was assessed by discrimination reversal training. Naloxone administered i.p. (3.0 mg/kg) significantly facilitated retention of both tasks in unoperated control rats as well as in rats implanted bilaterally with amygdala cannulae. The memory-enhancing effect of naloxone i.p. was blocked by propranolol (0.3 or 1.0 microgram) injected in the amygdala, but not when this beta-noradrenergic antagonist was injected (0.3 micrograms) into either the caudate or the cortex dorsal to the amygdala. Further, intra-amygdala injections of the beta 1-adrenoceptor blocker atenolol (0.3 or 1.0 microgram) and the beta 2-adrenoceptor blocker zinterol (0.3 or 1.0 microgram), in doses which were ineffective when administered alone, blocked naloxone-induced (3.0 mg/kg, i.p.) memory facilitation. In contrast, posttraining intra-amygdala administration (1.0 micrograms) of the alpha-antagonists prazosin (alpha 1) or yohimbine (alpha 2) did not attenuate the memory-enhancing effects of systemically administered naloxone. These findings support the view that naloxone-induced enhancement of memory is mediated by the activation of beta- but not alpha-noradrenergic receptors located within the amygdaloid complex.  相似文献   

20.
The resolution of racemic nicotine to provide optically pure (+)-nicotine and the synthesis of radiolabeled nicotine with high specific activity have facilitated the study of nicotine binding in brain. The actions of the stereoisomers of nicotine on the central nervous system are qualitatively similar in most tests but (−)-nicotine is more potent than the unnatural (+)-isomer by 10-fold or greater. Binding of radiolabeled nicotine to brain has both saturable and nonsaturable components. Only saturable binding is affected by incubation conditions such as time, temperature, pH and ion concentration. Excess concentrations of the stereoisomers are equally effective in displacing (−)-[3H]-nicotine from brain homogenates. Nevertheless, a direct comparison of (+)-[3H]-nicotine and (−)-[3H]-nicotine binding shows that the latter has a KD three times lower than the former. (−)-[3H]-Nicotine is bound to the greatest degree in hypothalamus and hippocampus, areas that also exhibited the most stereoselectivity for nicotine. However, differences in the binding affinities of the two isomers were far less than the pharmacological stereospecificity observed.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号