首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Female rats were given access to casein, cornstarch, and fat in separate cups or a composite diet identical in composition to that chosen by self-selecting rats. After 2 weeks, all rats received granular sucrose ad lib in addition to these other foods. Sucrose availability resulted in increased caloric intake and increased body weight gain in composite-fed rats, but not in self-selectors. Self-selectors maintained caloric homeostasis by decreased consumption of all foods initially, but protein intake recovered to control levels by the third week of treatment.  相似文献   

2.
Adult female rats were given access to cornstarch, fat, and 2 cups of casein ad lib. Sucrose octaacetate was added to one or both casein cups in concentrations of 0, 0.1, or 2.5%. The lower concentrations of SOA had no effect on total caloric intake or food choices, although a taste aversion experiment indicated that the rats could detect SOA at this level. The 2.5% concentration had no effect on total caloric intake. If only one casein cup was treated with SOA, the animals consumed 70% of their protein from the unadulterated casein supply, but total protein intake was unchanged. If both cups were treated with 2.5% SOA, protein intake decreased, but carbohydrate consumption showed a compensatory increase. Fat intake was unchanged.  相似文献   

3.
Though not universally observed, moderately low-protein diets have been found to increase caloric intake and body fat. It appears that animals overeat in calories in order to obtain more dietary protein. For animals to control protein intake, they must be able to distinguish between two isocaloric diets containing different percentages of protein and make the appropriate dietary selection on the basis of their previous history of protein intake. Experiment 1 examined the 24-h diet selection (5 vs. 35% casein) of Sprague-Dawley rats that had been previously fed diets containing various percentages of dietary protein (5, 10, 20, 35, or 60% casein). Animals fed 5, 10, or 20% dietary protein showed a preference for the higher protein selection diet. In contrast, no significant diet preference was found in animals pre-fed the two higher levels of dietary protein (35 or 60% casein). In this study, daily food intake and body fat of rats fed the low-protein diets (5 and 10% casein) were similar to rats fed the 20% casein diet. Experiment 2 examined the effects of the level of methionine supplementation on rats fed 10% casein. In this study, food intake and body fat were increased by approximately 20% in rats fed 10% casein diets, regardless of the level of methionine supplementation (0.3 vs. 0.15%). Together, the results suggest that the presence of low-protein-induced hyperphagia helps maintain body protein levels in the face of moderately low dietary protein and promotes an increase in the amount of body fat and energy.  相似文献   

4.
Melatonin, a hormone secreted in a rhythmic manner over 24 h mainly by the pineal gland, is used to alleviate the symptoms of jetlag and treat sleeping problems. The objective of the present study was to examine the effects of a 7-h phase-shift from the natural peak of melatonin secretion on total food intake and macronutrient selection. Forty-eight adult Wistar rats of both sexes were divided in three dietary groups, each group offered a simultaneous and different choice of a carbohydrate- and a protein-rich diet. Macronutrient intakes following intraperitoneal administration of four doses of melatonin (3000, 6000, 10 000 and 15 000 pg/ml blood) at dark onset were examined. Melatonin increased short- (4 h postinjection) and long-term (12 h postinjection) nocturnal total food intake in both male and female rats, mainly with the two highest doses. This effect of melatonin was mainly due to a short-term increase of intake across all carbohydrate-rich diet preparations (dextrin/cornstarch, cornstarch, and sucrose/cornstarch) and across genders. This consistent effect of melatonin on the intake of carbohydrate-rich diets with contrasting sensory attributes rules out the possibility that melatonin acts on sensorymotor pathways, thus suggesting that melatonin's effect on food intake is controlled by the carbohydrate content of the diet. In contrast, melatonin could be affecting some sensory or motor processes peculiar to the ingestion of protein since it increased protein-rich diet intake inconsistently across the various preparations (casein, soy isolate, and egg protein) as well as genders. This evidence supports the view that melatonin acts as a time indicator, reinforcing the animals with a “night cue”, and favors predominant carbohydrate intake normally occurring at the beginning of the activity period.  相似文献   

5.
Adult female rats were allowed to self-select their diet from separate sources of fat, protein, and carbohydrate (starch). Other rats were fed a composite diet that matched the nutrient composition chosen by the self-selecting rats (50% fat, 28% protein, 22% carbohydrate) or a low-fat, high-carbohydrate chow diet. Half of the rats in each diet condition were given access to a 32% sucrose solution for 30 days. Sucrose availability increased total caloric intake (approximately 20%) and body weight gain in all three groups compared to control groups not fed the sucrose solution. The selection animals compensated for their sucrose intake by reducing their fat intake, and to a lesser degree, their starch intake; protein intake was the least affected by sucrose availability. The selection rats consumed less sucrose than the chow-fed rats and displayed a smaller increase in weight, relative to controls, than the chow-fed rats. These differences were attributed to the high-fat intake of the selection animals since similar results were obtained with the rats fed the composite diet. In particular, both the selection and composite diets produced mild obesity in the absence of sucrose. The results demonstrate that sucrose-induced overeating and overweight is not an artifact of restraining the diet choices of rats to a pure sugar and a nutritionally complete diet.  相似文献   

6.
Effect of type of protein on food intake of rats fed high protein diets   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The effects of type of protein on intake of a high protein diet after adapting rats to a low protein diet were examined in rats trained to eat a 5.2% (N X 6.25) protein diet containing a mixture of casein, lactalbumin, egg white and soy protein, in a single 3-hour period per day. Food intake was measured from 0-15, 15-30, 30-90, and 90-180 minutes. After a 2-week adjustment period, rats were presented with a 40% (N X 6.25) protein purified diet containing only one of the 4 proteins mentioned above or a mixture of these 4 proteins. During the first 15-minute interval, rats eating diets containing protein mixture, lactalbumin, egg white or soy protein depressed their intake significantly compared with the average intake of the 3-day pre-test period, whereas rats eating casein diet increased their intake. During the last 90-minute interval of the first day, all rats depressed their intake, those rats eating casein the least and those rats eating egg white the most. On the second day, rats offered lactalbumin depressed their intake 52.5% for the 3-hour period and rats offered casein depressed their intake 34.3%. Rats eating soy protein, egg white and protein mixture increased their intake from day 1 to day 2. These experiments show that type of protein affects rats' initial intake when they are offered a high protein diet.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

7.
2-Deoxy-D-glucose (2DG) and mercaptoacetate (MA) are antimetabolic agents that reduce the metabolism of glucose and fatty acids, respectively, and stimulate feeding. The present study compared the effects of MA and 2DG on macronutrient self-selection. Because 2DG and MA have different metabolic actions and appear to activate different neural pathways, our hypothesis was that 2DG and MA would elicit different patterns of macronutrient selection. The first experiment examined macronutrient selection in response to 2DG, MA, and 0.9% saline in rats maintained on a three-macronutrient self-selection diet consisting of cornstarch, casein, and vegetable oil. Subsequently, one macronutrient source was replaced in each of three similar experiments with Polycose, albumin, or solid vegetable shortening. Finally, 2DG and MA tests were conducted in which only one macronutrient (cornstarch, casein, or oil) was available during the test. Results show that MA and 2DG elicit different macronutrient preferences. 2DG elicits intake of all three macronutrients in the same relative proportion consumed during spontaneous feeding across a number of dietary conditions, suggesting that glucoprivation activates interoceptive signals and neural pathways similar to those involved in normal hunger. MA elicits a selective intake of protein. Conditions in which carbohydrate palatability is enhanced or protein palatability is diminished lead to a relative increase in carbohydrate intake in response to MA. However, MA did not increase the intake of fat. Results suggest that intake of each macronutrient is subject to separate neural or endocrine control, and that these controls are linked to metabolic cues.  相似文献   

8.
During a 15-day experiment, weanling rats fed a 10% casein diet adulterated daily by one of four aversive taste stimuli ate 12–16% less than their controls fed ad lib on the unadulterated diet. They also demonstrated an inhibition in feed efficiency (g body weight gain/g food intake) during the first 8 days. A second control group, pair-fed to the group receiving the adulterated diet, also showed a reduction in feed efficiency. By Day 9, a compensatory process was evident in the rats fed the unpalatable diet such that feed efficiency increased above that observed for the ad lib control group. By the end of the experiment all three groups exhibited the same efficiency. The ratio of urinary nitrogen to total nitrogen intake was higher in rats fed the unpalatable diet ad lib compared to those offered the unadulterated diet ad lib. This observation suggests that the rats on the unpalatable diet were using more of the dietary protein as a caloric source than were their controls.  相似文献   

9.
We have been studying the development of hypertension in spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) fed a low protein diet. The effects of a low protein diet upon food and water intake were examined. Body weight gain, food and water intake were measured in three to twenty-three week-old SHR and Wistar Kyoto rats (WKY) fed diets containing 8%, 15% or 25% casein. Body weights of SHR and WKY fed an 8% casein diet were significantly lower at 23 weeks than rats on the higher protein diets, although both groups on the 8% diet consumed more food and water per g of body weight. In addition, SHR fed an 8% casein diet drank less water per gram of food than WKY or SHR fed 15% and 25% casein diets. These results indicate that changes in food and water intake, as a consequence of low protein diets, should be an additional consideration when examining the effects of dietary protein on the development of hypertension.  相似文献   

10.
Experiments were performed to investigate the behavioural responses of golden hamsters to manipulations of dietary protein availability. In the first experiment, hamsters were maintained on a protein-free diet and a powdered diet containing 64.8% protein (P64.8). When the P64.8 diet was progressively diluted with cornstarch, hamsters increased their intake of this diet fraction, but protein intake nevertheless declined. When the protein content of the diet was 16.2%, animals derived only 6% of total calories from protein and lost weight despite normal intake of calories. In the remaining experiments, hamsters were maintained on a self-selection regimen of high-protein chow, pure carbohydrate (sugar cubes), and pure fat (vegetable shortening). When high-protein chow was removed for either 5 or 10 days, total caloric intake and body weight declined, and hamsters selectively increased protein intake for several days after high-protein chow was returned. Hamsters allowed access to high-protein chow for only one hour each day markedly increased the amount of high-protein chow they ate during this hour as protein-restriction continued, but still consumed only about 10% of their normal daily protein intake on this schedule and lost 20% of starting body weight in two weeks; when free access to high-protein chow was restored, these animals selectively increased their protein intake above pre-restriction levels. Hamsters given access to high-protein chow only on alternate days demonstrated a relatively modest and slowly developing increase in protein intake, perhaps because they incurred only a moderate protein deficit. The results suggest that when protein intake falls below normal minimum requirements, hamsters will demonstrate an adaptive protein hunger but make only a limited adjustment to the dilution of a protein-containing diet fraction.  相似文献   

11.
Rats were trained to eat a 6% casein basal diet during a 3-hour period per day. They were then fed either the same 6% casein diet or a 44% casein diet for 3 hours. No food intake depression was observed in the rats eating 44% casein diet during the 3-hour period. Plasma ammonia and amino acids and brain amino acids were measured at 0, 4, 12 and 24 hours after presentation of the 6% or 44% casein diets. Plasma ammonia rose to 134 (p less than 0.01) and 110 micromolar (p less than 0.05) in the 44% casein fed rats at 4 and 12 hours, respectively, as compared to 67 and 53 micromolar, respectively, for the 6% casein fed rats. All plasma amino acid concentrations except methionine and glutamate were elevated (p less than 0.05) at 4 hours. In the brain, threonine, glutamine and tyrosine concentrations were elevated (p less than 0.05) at 4 hours after diet presentation. At 24 hours, valine, isoleucine, leucine, phenylalanine, and methionine concentrations were also elevated (p less than 0.05). Because intake of the 44% casein diet decreases the second day of its presentation, as noted in an earlier experiment, the increases in plasma ammonia and its possible entry into the brain as reflected by increased brain glutamine together with changes in amino acid concentrations should be considered collectively among possible metabolic signals affecting intake of high protein diets.  相似文献   

12.
Bilateral double electrolytic overlapping lesions were placed in dorsal-lateral hippocampus of male 230 g rats, and their food intake responses to the ingestion of diets containing disproportionate amounts of amino acids were examined. Rats with such lesions and intact control rats maintained their normal intakes of the 6% casein basal diet or a threonine basal amino acid diet postoperatively. However, they exhibited marked initial food intake depression, similar to that of intact rats, when fed the threonine imbalanced amino acid diet. Also, animals with lesions in certain areas of the dorsal-lateral hippocampus showed facilitated adaptation to the amino acid imbalanced diet. Similar severe reduction in food intake with relative lack of adaptation were observed in both the intact controls and rats with hippocampal lesions when fed amino acid diets completely devoid of threonine. Initial food intake of rats with hippocampal lesions was inhibited drastically as was the case with the intact controls when fed a 75% casein high protein diet. All rats, either intact or lesioned, showed similar slow adaptation patterns with the prolonged ingestion of the high protein diet. The initial food intake responses and facilitated adaptation of the animals bearing lesions in certain areas of the hippocampus suggest that such areas are not crucially involved in the inhibition of food intake of rats fed disproportionate amounts of dietary amino acids. Rather, such areas of lesions in the hippocampus may play a role in a system governing the behavioral adaptation of the intake of amino acid imbalanced diets but not of diets containing amino acids in general excess. This would also indicate that different mechanisms control the intake of amino acid imbalanced diets and diets containing amino acids in excess.  相似文献   

13.
A deleterious decrease of protein intake had been evidenced in Lou/c/jall rats during ageing. This result could be induced by an impaired regulation of feeding behaviour. Glucocorticoids inducing specific amino-acid needs for gluconeogenesis and for the synthesis of inflammatory proteins by the liver, we investigated the age-related effect of a 4-days treatment with dexamethasone (DEX) on caloric and protein intake. Males and females aged 7, 19, 25 and 31 months received 573.6 +/- 65.6 microg/(kg day) of dexamethasone via the drinking water. Body weight (BW), caloric and macronutrients intakes were monitored during treatment and during 10 days after the treatment. A strong hypophagia was seen during treatment in all groups, which was mainly due to a decrease in fat intake. In the same time, rats maintained their protein intake so that protein became the main macronutrient of the diet in most of the groups. However, older males showed a lesser efficiency in adjusting their diet. These results are in agreement with previous data obtained in a protein deprivation study. They lead to the conclusion that the loss of appetite for protein in old age probably does not reflect a loss of ability to choose the needed amount of protein. We can hypothesise that the decrease of protein intake in old rats could be due to some inadequacy of casein to the metabolic requirement of aged animals.  相似文献   

14.
Seven experiments with 324 chicks tested their ability to select a nutritionally adequate diet from separate sources of purified casein and various supplements, including gelatin (a source of two amino acids), a gelatin-creatine mixture (a source of three amino acids), and fiber (nonnutritive bulk). Nonselecting controls consumed the basal purified-casein diet or a supplemented purified-casein diet. Chicks in all selection conditions composed diets that yielded normal intake, normal body temperature, normal activity, and the maximum growth possible for their intake. They also selected components in the same percentages as in premixed diets. In all instances, their selection was nonrandom and regulated. What chicks included in their diet depended on what else was available. Although the specific percentage taken from each dietary component varied across different selection alternatives, these differences affected neither intake nor growth. Selection, per se, incurred a caloric cost. Chicks selecting from fractions of a corn-and-soy diet offset this cost by increasing intake compensatorily, but chicks with a purified-casein fraction did not, suggesting that some unspecified property of casein placed a ceiling on its intake. These findings unequivocally demonstrate that immature chicks not only can self-select nutritionally adequate diets, but can do so with unexpected precision by exploiting different but equally successful strategies. © 1996 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.. Inc.  相似文献   

15.
The effect on intake of a 75% casein diet after prefeeding for one week a 6% casein basal diet with additional 0%, 2%, 5%, 8% or 15% ammonium acetate was examined in rats trained to eat in three hours per day. Food intake was measured from 0-15, 15-30, 30-90, and 90-180 minutes for the first two days that the ammonium acetate diets were presented. Rats eating 5% and 8% or 15% ammonium acetate diet depressed their intake significantly for one day and for four days respectively. Rats eating 2%, 5%, 8%, or 15% ammonium acetate diets depressed their intake significantly from 0-30 minutes. When presented with the 75% casein diet, rats prefed 0% to 5% and 8% and 15% ammonium acetate diets ate 55% to 58% and 72% and 94% of their respective baseline intakes. It is suggested that prefeeding 15% ammonium acetate apparently induces sufficient metabolic adaptation to ammonia intake so that the rat is able to offset the metabolic consequences of intake of the 75% casein diet, thus preventing the usual food intake depressing effect of the high protein diet.  相似文献   

16.
The present study explored the influence of pelleted diets on adjustment to caloric dilution in hypothalamic obese rats. Medial hypothalamic lesioned and normal rats were maintained on a high fat diet until the static stage of hyperphagia was reached. They were given three pelleted diets which consisted of undiluted Noyes pellets and Noyes pellets diluted 25 or 50 percent with kaolin. In contrast to previous reports that hypothalamic obese rats do not compensate for caloric dilution of their diet, the obese animals showed as large an increase in food intake on the dilute pelleted diets as did the control animals. These results were interpreted by viewing texture as a dimension of palatability which influences food intake in hypothalamic obese rats.  相似文献   

17.
These experiments demonstrate that rats can immediately adjust their meal size in response to variations in the caloric density of a novel diet. However, this immediate caloric sensitivity only seems to appear when rats have been adapted to small, calorically insufficient meals. Rats in Experiment 1 were given timed access to unlimited quantities of an oil/water diet during baseline, and they showed no indication of compensating for changes in the caloric density of the oil/water diet during a test meal. Instead, they consumed about the same amount they had consumed during the preceding baseline meal, suggesting that a learned habit of consuming a certain volume of food controlled their meal size. In contrast, rats that were accustomed to receiving only a very small quantity of food for one of their daily meals during baseline immediately responded to the caloric density of an oil/water test diet by consuming a larger meal if the diet was dilute than if it was calorically more concentrated (Experiments 2 and 3). This immediate sensitivity to caloric density occurred whether or not the rats were exposed to the oil/water diet during baseline, suggesting that rats have some way of directly "metering" the caloric density of new foods. Thus, rats' caloric intake during a meal appears to be controlled by two factors: under certain conditions, control is by caloric learning, under other conditions control is by a caloric metering mechanism.  相似文献   

18.
Rats in a laboratory foraging paradigm had 24-hr-per-day access to a feeder where they could search, by completing a fixed number of bar presses, for an opportunity to eat one of a pair of foods differing in caloric density (2.5, 3, 3.5, or 4 kcal/g) and, in Experiment 2, the price of food pellets (10 to 50 bar presses per pellet). The rats could either accept the opportunity, and eat a meal, or reject it in favor of further search. Daily caloric intake was relatively constant. The rats always included both foods in their diet, but, for any particular food, the degree of inclusion in the diet and of acceptance of meal opportunities, the meal size, and the rate of eating were all functions not only of the price and caloric value of that food but also of the price and value of the alternately-available food. The patterns of intake for one food relative to those for the other available food were strongly correlated with the relative rate of calorie intake during consumption of that food compared to the other. Although the rats appeared to be sensitive to the local rates of calorie flow, they did not maximize daily calories consumed per time spent feeding.  相似文献   

19.
Food intake and body weight gain of male adult Wistar rats were examined in two groups of animals. One group (n = 14) was allowed to select its diet from separate sources of protein (casein, 3.1 kcal/g), fat (lard and sunflower oil, 7.9 kcal/g) and carbohydrate (CHO, starch and sucrose, 3.3 kcal/g). Another group (n = 10) received a nutritionally complete diet (3.3 kcal/g). After 2 weeks of adaptation to the diets, body weights and meal patterns were recorded for at least 4 days. The total caloric intake was nearly identical for the two groups of rats. Rats given dietary choice gained less weight over 4 days than rats fed chow and showed reduced feed efficiency. During the 24-h period, self-selecting rats consumed 20.8% of calories as proteins, 21% as fats and 58.2% as CHO. Self-selecting rats ate significantly less calories during the day than did rats given chow. The chow diet consisting of 17.3% calories as protein, 7.6% as fat and 75.1% as CHO. When comparing the self-selecting group nutrient intakes to those of chow-fed group it was observed that 24-h protein calorie intakes were identical in both groups. Fat intake was significantly higher and CHO reduced as compared to chow-fed rats. During the day, CHO intake was higher in self-selecting rats, and fat intake was not significantly reduced. During the night, protein and fat intakes were significantly higher in self-selecting rats, while CHO intake was significantly decreased, particularly in the last periods of the night.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

20.
The factors involved in the depression of food intake produced by a high-protein diet are still poorly understood. The aim of this study was to assess the role of several preingestive or preabsorptive factors likely to influence food intake when rats were fed ad libitum. Food intake was measured after modifying the composition of the high-protein diet, i.e., the type of proteins, or carbohydrates. Moreover, correlations between high-protein diet intake and the quantity of fluid ingested or stomach volume were studied. By varying the carbohydrate composition (sucrose/cornstarch) and the protein source (soy or gluten or total milk protein) of high-protein diets, we modified the orosensory properties of these diets. However, no differences in food intake were observed between these groups of rats during the transition phase or after adaptation, except during the first day of soy- or gluten-based diets when the depression of food intake was intensified. The depression of high-protein diet intake was neither the consequence of any delay necessary to increase the fluid intake induced by eating a high-protein diet nor due to a marked increase in stomach volume, which might explain enhanced satiety and decreased food intake through the activation of vagal afferent fibers. Our experiments do not indicate a preponderant role for oropharyngeal or preabsorptive factors in the depression of food intake induced by a high-protein diet.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号