首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The present study investigates executive functioning in schizotypic college students and control subjects using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). Inhibitory control and working memory, two aspects of executive functioning, were examined in deviantly high scorers on the Perceptual Aberration and Magical Ideation Scales (n=97), high scorers on the revised Social Anhedonia Scale (n=58), and in control subjects (n=104). The schizotypic groups displayed significantly more perseverative errors and achieved fewer categories than the control group. The two schizotypic groups did not differ from each other. We identified a subset of schizotypic individuals who also produced clinically deviant WCST profiles. The findings support the hypothesis that executive function deficits may precede the onset of schizophrenia and related illnesses.  相似文献   

2.
This study evaluates the ability of several Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Psychological Assessment Resources, 1990) variables to detect malingering in mild traumatic brain injury (TBI). The sample consisted of 373 TBI patients and 766 general clinical patients. Classification accuracy for seven indicators is reported across a range of injury severity and scores levels. Overall, most WCST scores were ineffective in discriminating malingering from non-malingering mild TBI patients. Failure-to-Maintain-Set, the Suhr & Boyer formula, and the King et al. formula detected about 30% of malingerers at cutoffs associated with a false positive error rate of ≤11%. The clinical interpretation and use of these indicators are discussed.  相似文献   

3.
目的 初步探讨精神病司法鉴定中威斯康星卡片分类测验(WCST)的应用。方法 将我院2004~2005年鉴定中的刑事案例,分成具有精神病诊断的研究组54例和正常组32例,收集一般资料,完成WCST,并对数据进行统计学处理。结果 两组WCST结果不存在显著性差异;研究组中的精神分裂症患者与正常组无显著性差异;研究组中完全责任者、限定责任者、无责任者之间WCST结果的两两比较也不存在显著性差异。结论 WCST结果受多种因素影响,在精神病司法鉴定中,WCST的运用有待进一步研究。  相似文献   

4.
5.
The purpose of this study was to explore the relationship between regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) and problem-solving thinking in negative schizophrenia. Twenty-one negative schizophrenic patients and 12 normal controls were studied with single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT). Measures of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) were taken both at rest and during a prefrontal activation task using Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). Compared with controls, poor performances on the WCST of total trials category (TT), perseverative errors (PE) and non-perseverative errors (NE) were found in negative schizophrenic (P < 0.05). During WCST activation, patients showed interhemispheric differences in the prefrontal region, but under rest conditions, no such differences manifested. The negative schizophrenia group had a significantly lower rCBF change rate in profrontal lobe during stimulant WCST than those in normal controls (P < 0.05). The negative schizophrenic patient has executive function deficits and lower rCBF perfusion in left profrontal lobes, which suggest that the negative schizophrenic patient has dysfunction of the left profrontal region.  相似文献   

6.
The leading aim of the present study was to examine developmental trends in performance on a computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) in Turkish children. Participants were 449 children aged between 8 and 11. Findings suggested that children's WCST scores improve with age, and developmental changes in executive functions follow distinct processes as assessed by different components of the WCST. The most striking improvement was noted in perseverative tendencies around age 10. This study also explored the effects of parental education on WCST performances of children, and maternal education emerged as the most important predictor.  相似文献   

7.
INTRODUCTION: It is generally assumed that executive dysfunctions in Parkinson's disease (PD) are caused by degeneration of the basal ganglia or frontal cortex or both. However, there have been few studies investigating the relationship between executive dysfunctions and cerebral pathological change. The objective of this study was to evaluate various cognitive functions in non-demented patients with PD, and to compare the fractional anisotropy (FA) values of PD patients with and without executive dysfunction. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Twenty-one consecutive non-demented patients with PD were enrolled in this study. Patients were divided into two groups on the basis of their Wisconsin Card Sorting Test score. RESULTS: There was significant FA reduction in the left parietal white matter in the group in which the number of categories achieved was 2. CONCLUSION: Accumulating evidence suggests that conventional 'frontal' tasks correlate with both frontal lobe and parietal lobe function, and we suggest that pathological changes in the left parietal lobe may cause, in part, disturbances in executive tasks in PD.  相似文献   

8.
Objectives:  Patients experiencing an acute mood episode of bipolar disorder are impaired on cognitive tests of executive functioning when compared with healthy subjects. However, it is unclear to what extent executive dysfunction exists (i) during syndromal remission, when mood symptoms are attenuated, and (ii) during the early course of bipolar disorder.
Methods:  To clarify these issues, we examined retrospective data on a standard clinical version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) administered to 25 euthymic, 21 first-episode manic or mixed, and 34 multiple-episode manic or mixed patients with bipolar I disorder, and 48 healthy comparison subjects. These groups were compared on five WCST measures of executive ability.
Results:  On all but one measure the healthy group's performance was superior to that of both first- and multiple-episode groups. The euthymic group outperformed the multiple-episode group, but performed similarly to the first-episode group on all but one measure. The healthy and euthymic groups did not differ significantly on any measure. Effect sizes ranged from small to moderate.
Conclusions:  Executive dysfunction may be a mediating vulnerability indicator of bipolar disorder that is strongly related to mood state, but only modestly related to chronicity of illness during the early disease course.  相似文献   

9.
The aim of the present study was to assess mental flexibility and set maintenance of a group of individuals with Asperger syndrome (AS) or high-functioning autism (HFA) (N = 13; mean age 16,4), as compared with a matched group of typically developing children and adolescents (N = 13; mean age 15,6) on the computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). The participants in the AS/HFA group performed less well than the controls on all categories of the WCST, but the differences did not reach conventional statistical significance on most categories of the WCST. On the category failure to maintain set, however, the AS/HFA participants performed significantly less well than the controls, suggesting a deficit of focused attention.  相似文献   

10.
Impaired working memory (WM) performance is considered as a central feature of schizophrenia. Divided into two components, verbal and spatial, WM has been shown to involve frontal and parietal regions. Verbal WM can be tested either visually or aurally. The present study aimed to test schizophrenic patients in both visual and auditory verbal WM in order to assess a possible distinct pattern of alteration of these two modalities. Twenty-four schizophrenic patients and 24 healthy controls were compared with 2-back continuous visual and auditory verbal WM testing. Both groups were also tested on a neuropsychological battery including Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). Schizophrenic patients were less efficient in both verbal WM tests. When taking age and educational level as covariates and both WM modalities as dependent variables, there was no differential effect of modalities across groups. In further exploratory analyses, partial correlations brought association between verbal WM and psychosocial adaptation, WCST and length of illness. These results suggest a similar pattern of alteration of both modalities of verbal WM in schizophrenic patients. The implications of this finding are discussed. Received: 16 February 1999 / Accepted: 20 December 1999  相似文献   

11.
《Brain & development》2022,44(7):438-445
ObjectiveChildren with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) often experience difficulties with emotional control and a consequent inability to perform tasks. To clarify the effects of emotional behavior on cognitive functions, we aimed to determine the association between emotional changes and executive functions in children with ADHD by measuring the pupil diameter changes associated with emotional changes.Participants and methodsThis study included 14 children with ADHD and 10 typically developing children (TDC) aged between 10 and 16 years. During the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), which is related to context formation and task switching among executive functions, changes in pupil diameter and frontal oxygenated hemoglobin (oxy-Hb) using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) were recorded simultaneously. Pupil diameter changes during “cognitive shift” and “consecutive correction” were compared between both groups.ResultsDuring cognitive shift, the pupils of children with ADHD contracted, whereas those of the TDC were mydriatic. During consecutive correction, the pupils of children with ADHD were mydriatic, whereas those of the TDC tended to contract. These results correlated with WCST performance. Moreover, during cognitive shifts, changes in bilateral frontal blood flow were increased in TDC, but not in children with ADHD.ConclusionThe locus coeruleus-norepinephrine (LC-NE) system plays an important role in pupillary diameter response. These results suggest that the LC-NE system may be dysfunctional in children with ADHD, and the system’s abnormality may lead to affective abnormalities in such patients, which results in poor performance on WCST (i.e., impaired executive functions).  相似文献   

12.
13.
14.
This study attempted to confirm that humour recognition deficits previously found in schizophrenia are specific to the condition and not attributable to other parameters such as depression or anxiety. Secondarily, we explored any possible cognitive or social functioning correlates to humour recognition deficits. A total of 60 participants (20 outpatients with schizophrenia, 20 psychiatric control participants and 20 control participants) underwent a 64-question humour task in addition to a battery of standard cognitive tests and Social Functioning Scales. In order to compare the three groups of participants, we conducted an analysis of variance (ANOVA) and post-hoc t-tests on neuropsychological measures, social functioning measures, and the primary outcome, humour recognition. The schizophrenia group showed significant and substantial deficits in humour recognition compared to the healthy control group, t(38) = 5.1, P < 0.001, ES = −1.55 and the psychiatric control group, t(38) = 3.6, P = 0.001. In the schizophrenia group, humour recognition correlated positively with general intellectual functioning (NART) r = .45, P = 0.04, social reasoning (WAIS-III Comprehension) r = .54, P = 0.01, executive functioning (WCST-CC) r = .69, P = 0.001 and social adjustment ratings (SASS scores), r = .54, P = 0.02. These findings support the assertion that humour recognition deficits in schizophrenia are specific to the condition and not attributable to other factors such as depression or anxiety. Furthermore, humour recognition deficits in schizophrenia may perhaps be preferentially associated with deficiencies in set shifting and semantic cognition.  相似文献   

15.
目的探讨精神分裂症患者认知功能障碍的特点。方法对23例以阴性症状为主的精神分裂症、30例以阳性症状为主的精神分裂症和28名正常人进行了威斯康星卡片分类测验(WCST)。结果显示以阴性症状为主的精神分裂症患者的总测验次数、持续错误数和非持续错误数明显高于以阳性症状为主的精神分裂症患者和正常人,差异有显著性(P<0.05)。同时发现,WCST的以上测验指标在两组患者之间差异亦有显著性(P<0.05)。相关分析显示,以阴性症状为主的精神分裂症患者的Andreasen阴性症状量表总分与简明精神病评定量表的迟滞因子分和持续错误数呈显著正相关(r分别为0.4372和0.4551)。结论提示精神分裂症患者存在执行功能障碍,其中阴性症状可能与额叶功能缺陷有关。  相似文献   

16.
The present study examined spatial working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) performance in psychosis-prone individuals, either those with extremely high scores on the Social Anhedonia Scale (SocAnh; n = 49) or deviant scores on the Perceptual Aberration-Magical Ideation Scales (Per-Mag; n = 66). Sixty-three individuals with normal scores on the Chapman Psychosis-Proneness Scales served as control subjects. In order to evaluate working memory performance, participants were administered three tasks, namely, sensorimotor, degraded stimulus, and delayed-response tasks. Although the SocAnh and Per-Mag groups displayed poorer performance than control subjects on the working memory task, they did not differ significantly from each other. The SocAnh group exhibited slower reaction times on the working memory task compared to the control group. The groups did not differ in their performance on sensorimotor or degraded stimulus control tasks. Both psychosis-prone groups differed significantly from control subjects in terms of their WCST performance. Working memory performance was inversely associated with the number of perseverative errors (r = -0.17) and the number of trials to complete the first category on the WCST (r= -0.15). These findings extend the literature by indicating that some psychosis-prone individuals with social-interpersonal schizotypal deficits also display subtle spatial working memory impairments.  相似文献   

17.
Summary. Environmental enrichment produces changes in spontaneous and psychostimulant-induced motor activity. Dopamine in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), through the activation of D1 receptors, has been suggested to play a role in modulating motor activity. The present study investigated the effects of environmental enrichment on spontaneous motor activity, prefrontal acetylcholine release following local D1 receptor stimulation and D1 receptor expression in the PFC. Male wistar rats (3 months of age) were housed in enriched or isolated conditions during 90 days. Animals were then implanted with guide cannulae to perform microdialysis experiments in the PFC. Spontaneous motor activity and acetylcholine extracellular concentrations were monitored simultaneously. Also spontaneous motor activity was measured in an open field. On completion of the experiments, the density of D1 receptors in the PFC was studied by immunocytochemistry. Rats housed in an enriched environment showed significantly lower spontaneous motor activity in the open field compared to isolated animals. Perfusion of the D1 agonist SKF38393 (50 μM; 40 min) in the PFC produced long lasting increases of spontaneous motor activity and of local dialysate concentrations of acetylcholine in both groups of rats. However, increases of both motor activity and acetylcholine concentrations were significantly lower in enriched compared to isolated animals. Moreover, the density of D1 receptors in the PFC was significantly reduced in animals housed in an enriched environment. These results are the first evidence suggesting that environmental enrichment during adult life changes the function of D1 dopamine receptors in the PFC.  相似文献   

18.
It remains unclear how impaired insight and neurocognitive impairment are related to one another in schizophrenia. In order to examine this relationship, subjects with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder were classified as having‘impaired’(n=38) or‘unimpaired’(n=43) insight based on their insight rating on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS). Their performance on neuropsychological tests of global function, executive function, memory and vigilance was then compared. Multivariate analyses followed by Scheffe tests indicated that subjects with impaired insight performed less well on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, demonstrating poorer abstract flexibility and heightened perseveration. No differences were found between the groups with regard to global cognitive function, memory or vigilance. This suggests that insight is closely linked to deficits in executive function, and that it may be associated with the compromise of frontal lobe function.  相似文献   

19.
The clinical and neuro-cognitive correlates of the P50 and N100 auditory evoked responses gating deficits in schizophrenia have thus far eluded identification. Based on our prior results, we hypothesized that, in addition to the P50, gating of the N100 is significantly decreased in schizophrenia and that this deficit correlates with the negative symptoms dimension of schizophrenia. Amplitudes and gating measures of the P50 and N100 were compared between stable out-patients (N = 45) (mainly on atypical antipsychotics) with chronic schizophrenia and age- and gender-matched healthy controls (N = 49) and the clinical correlates examined. All subjects underwent the paired-stimulus paradigm in 3 or 4 different days. Data from day one and the mean of all days (MOAD) were examined. P50 and N100 amplitudes and gating measures were correlated with PANSS and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test data. Utilizing day one data, no amplitude or gating measures were significantly different between the groups. Utilizing MOAD data, both P50 and N100 gating were significantly decreased in schizophrenia patients. The N100 gating deficit correlated with the negative-symptoms cluster and measures of frontal lobe dysfunction. The data suggest a correlation between N100 gating deficit and the negative-cognitive deficits dimensions of schizophrenia. Data also suggest that improving the signal to noise ratio (MOAD data) increases the sensitivity for detecting gating abnormalities and assessing their clinical correlates.  相似文献   

20.
This study disentangles the prefrontal network underlying executive functions involved in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). During the WCST, subjects have to perform two key processes: first, they have to derive the correct sorting rule for each trial by trial-and-error, and, second, they have to detect when this sorting rule is changed by the investigator. Both cognitive processes constitute key components of the executive system, which is subserved by the prefrontal cortex. For the current fMRI experiment, we developed a non-verbal variant of the WCST. Subjects were instructed either to respond according to a given sorting rule or to detect the correct sorting rule, like in the original version of the WCST. Data were obtained from 14 healthy male volunteers and analysed using SPM and a random effects model. All conditions activated a fronto-parietal network, which was generally more active when subjects had to search for the correct sorting rule than when the rule was announced beforehand. Significant differences between these two conditions were seen in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the parietal lobe. In addition, the data provided new evidence for the assumption of differentiated roles of the left and right prefrontal cortex. Although the right PFC showed a general involvement in response selection and the execution of goal directed responses, based on given rules, the left PFC was only activated when inductive reasoning and feedback integration was required.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号