共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
We report a series of 7 experiments examining the interaction between visual perception and action programming, contrasting 2 neuropsychological cases: a case of visual extinction and a case with extinction and optic ataxia. The patients had to make pointing responses to left and right locations, whilst identifying briefly presented shapes. Different patterns of performance emerged with the two cases. The patient with "pure" extinction (i.e., extinction without optic ataxia) showed dramatic effects of action programming on perceptual report. Programming an action to the ipsilesional side increased extinction (on 2-item trials) and tended to induce neglect (on 1-item trials); this was ameliorated when the action was programmed to the contralesional side. Separable effects of using the contralesional hand and pointing to the contralesional side were apparent. In contrast, the optic ataxic patient showed few effects of congruency between the visual stimulus and the action, but extinction when an action was programmed. This effect was particularly marked when actions had to be made to peripheral locations, suggesting that it reflected reduced resources to stimuli. These effects all occurred using stimulus exposures that were completed well before actions were effected. The data demonstrate interactions between action programming and visual perception. Programming an action to the affected side with the contralesional limb reduces "pure" extinction because attention is coupled to the end point of the action. However, in a patient with deficient visuo-motor coupling (optic ataxia), programming an action can increase a spatial deficit by recruiting resources away from perceptual processing. The implications for models of perception and action are discussed. 相似文献
2.
Prism adaptation (PA) alleviates some neglect symptoms, however, the mechanisms underlying these effects are unclear. One
brain area that may be important in generating these beneficial effects is the superior parietal lobe (SPL), a region not
typically damaged in neglect, and known to be important for attention, visuomotor control, and eye movements. We examined
the effects of rightward PA on covert attention in CF, a patient with bilateral SPL lesions, compared to a group of controls
( N = 26) who underwent sham adaptation. In contrast to previous work in neglect, there was no reduction in CF’s leftward disengage
deficit, or rightward attentional bias following PA. These results suggest that the SPL plays an important role in generating
the beneficial after-effects of prisms on attention.
Both Laure Pisella and James Danckert have contributed equally to this work. 相似文献
3.
Furosemide (Furo) is a potent natriuretic drug that is often used experimentally to investigate the brain mechanisms underlying salt appetite. Within this experimental paradigm, however, Furo also has anorectic activity that has received only modest attention. In Experiment 1 we varied two things-administering a 10-mg dose of Furo in a single or a divided dose and preinjection exposure to a Na-free diet. In the 24 h after Furo, all four groups of rats reduced ingestion of Na-free diet. Both the division of the Furo dose and the preexposure to Na-free diet reduced the amount of food consumed even more than a single dose or continuous access to normal chow did. The fact that preexposure to Na-free diet increased the post-Furo anorexia implied an associative component to the phenomenon. Experiments 2 and 3 investigated the ability of Furo (2 and 10 mg) to serve as an unconditioned stimulus in taste aversion learning using 0.2 M sucrose as the conditioned stimulus. A saline (Sal) injection group served as control in both experiments. The results show that animals avoided sucrose when its ingestion was immediately followed by 10 mg Furo but not with 2 mg Furo or Sal. An aversion to sucrose did not develop when 10 mg Furo was administered the day prior to sucrose access. Thus, the suppressive effects of high-dose Furo on food intake might be due to a conditioned response. 相似文献
4.
We report a series of 7 experiments examining the interaction between visual perception and action programming, contrasting 2 neuropsychological cases: a case of visual extinction and a case with extinction and optic ataxia. The patients had to make pointing responses to left and right locations, whilst identifying briefly presented shapes. Different patterns of performance emerged with the two cases. The patient with “pure” extinction (i.e., extinction without optic ataxia) showed dramatic effects of action programming on perceptual report. Programming an action to the ipsilesional side increased extinction (on 2-item trials) and tended to induce neglect (on 1-item trials); this was ameliorated when the action was programmed to the contralesional side. Separable effects of using the contralesional hand and pointing to the contralesional side were apparent. In contrast, the optic ataxic patient showed few effects of congruency between the visual stimulus and the action, but extinction when an action was programmed. This effect was particularly marked when actions had to be made to peripheral locations, suggesting that it reflected reduced resources to stimuli. These effects all occurred using stimulus exposures that were completed well before actions were effected. The data demonstrate interactions between action programming and visual perception. Programming an action to the affected side with the contralesional limb reduces “pure” extinction because attention is coupled to the end point of the action. However, in a patient with deficient visuo-motor coupling (optic ataxia), programming an action can increase a spatial deficit by recruiting resources away from perceptual processing. The implications for models of perception and action are discussed. 相似文献
5.
Coding of reaching in the cerebral cortex is based on the operation of distributed populations of parietal and frontal neurons,
whose main functional characteristics reside in their combinatorial power, i.e., in the capacity for combining different information
related to the spatial aspects of reaching. The tangential distribution of reach-related neurons endowed with different functional
properties changes gradually in the cortex and defines, in the parieto-frontal network, trends of functional properties. These
visual-to-somatic gradients imply the existence of cortical regions of functional overlaps, i.e., of combinatorial domains,
where the integration of different reach-related signals occurs. Studies of early coding of reaching in the mesial parietal
areas show how somatomotor information, such as that related to arm posture and movement, influences neuronal activity in
the very early stages of the visuomotor transformation underlying the composition of the motor command and is not added “downstream”
in the frontal cortex. This influence is probably due to re-entrant signals traveling through fronto-parietal-association
connections. Together with the gradient architecture of the network and the reciprocity of cortico-cortical connections, this
implies that coding of reaching cannot be regarded as a top-down, serial sequence of coordinate transformation, each performed
by a given cortical area, but as a recursive process, where different signals are progressively matched and further elaborated
locally, due to intrinsic cortical connections. This model of reaching is also supported by psychophysical studies stressing
the parallel processing of the different relevant parameters and the “hybrid” nature of the reference frame where they are
combined. The theoretical frame presented here can also offer a background for a new interpretation of a well-known visuomotor
disorder, due to superior parietal lesions, i.e., optic ataxia. More than a disconnection syndrome, this can now be interpreted
as the consequence of the breakdown of the operations occurring in the combinatorial domains of the superior parietal segment
of the parieto-frontal network. 相似文献
6.
The substantial forward projection from hippocampal area CA1 to the subiculum has been comprehensively described, both anatomically and neurophysiologically. There are few data, however, regarding the existence of a backward projection from the subiculum to area CA1. We present here new electrophysiological evidence for the existence of this projection. We demonstrate a positive-going deflection in the evoked synaptic response in area CA1 following stimulation in dorsal subiculum. We also found a small, but significant, paired-pulse facilitatory effect at a 100-ms interstimulus interval. We were unable to induce long-term potentiation following high-frequency stimulation, but were able to induce short-term potentiation. 相似文献
7.
Response inhibition is considered a core dimension in alcoholism and its co-existing disorders. The major objective of this study is to compare the magnitude and spatial distribution of ERP components during response activation and inhibition in alcoholics (N = 30) and normal controls (N = 30) using a visual Go/No-Go task. The results indicate that alcoholics manifest a decreased P3(00) amplitude during Go as well as No-Go conditions. The difference between Go and No-Go processing was more evident in controls than in alcoholics. The topography of current source density in alcoholics during the P3 response was found to be very different from that of normals, suggesting that alcoholics perhaps activated inappropriate brain circuitry during cognitive processing. The significantly reduced No-Go P3 along with the relatively less anteriorized CSD topography during No-Go condition suggests poor inhibitory control in alcoholics. It is proposed that the No-Go P3, the electrophysiological signature of response inhibition, can be considered as an endophenotypic marker in alcoholism. 相似文献
9.
An inbred family is described in which three sibs have congenital glaucoma and two of them also have an ataxia indistinguishable from Friedreich's ataxia. The association between these two disorders has not previously been reported. The genetic mechanisms of this association are discussed. 相似文献
10.
The avoidance model in patients with knee and hip osteoarthritis (OA) hypothesizes that pain and psychological distress lead to avoidance of activities, and thereby to muscle weakness and activity limitations. This paper systematically reviews the scientific evidence for the validity of this avoidance model. A qualitative data synthesis was used to identify levels of evidence. Sixty studies were included. In knee OA, strong evidence was found that avoidance of activities is associated with activity limitations via muscle weakness (mediation by muscle weakness), strong evidence was found for an association between muscle weakness and activity limitations, and weak evidence was found that pain and psychological distress are associated with muscle weakness via avoidance of activities (mediation by avoidance). In hip OA, weak evidence was found for mediation by muscle weakness; and strong evidence was found for an association between muscle weakness and activity limitations. More research is needed on the consecutive associations between pain or psychological distress, avoidance of activities and muscle weakness, and to confirm causal relationships. 相似文献
11.
In vitro studies support the existence of adult neural stem cells in the rostral migratory stream (RMS). The evidence supporting this possibility in vivo is scarce. We then explore this issue by taking advantage of a rat model in which a physical barrier implanted in the brain interrupted the migration of neuroblasts derived from the SVZ along the RMS at the level of its vertical limb. The presence of local stem cells and neurogenesis were then established by estimating the number of nuclei labeled with bromo-deoxyuridine (BrdU), the number of doublecortin-positive neuroblasts and the existence of cells displaying co-localization of BrdU and Sox-2 immunoreactivity along the RMS, at different time points following barrier implantation. Estimations of the number of the granular and periglomerular neurons integrated into the corresponding layers of the olfactory bulb of implanted rats established that stem cells in the RMS give rise predominantly to periglomerular neurons. Our results then support the notion that the RMS is indeed a region in which neurogenesis is taking place in the adult brain. They also support that the relative location of the neurogenic niche might imprint, at least in some degree, the identity and lineage of the neuroblasts arising from them. 相似文献
14.
Prior research showed that attentional deficits are observed in Alzheimer's disease (AD). These deficits can further impair other cognitive processes. The present experiment was designed to study the shifts in attention induced by a noradrenergic drug (S 12024-2) through their electrophysiological correlates in 12 outpatients with mild AD, using an auditory oddball paradigm. The P3a component, known to be related to automatic attentional processing, was increased by the drug, whereas no changes occurred either in PN or in P3b, which are considered to reflect conscious processing. These results point to an involvement of the noradrenergic system in the modulation of automatic attentional processing, and provide evidence for weakening of the orienting reflex in AD, due to a possible noradrenergic deficit in patients with mild AD. 相似文献
15.
Neurotransmission by serotonin (5-HT) is tightly regulated by several autoreceptors that fine-tune serotonergic neurotransmission through negative feedback inhibition at the cell bodies (predominantly 5-HT(1A)) or at the axon terminals (predominantly 5-HT(1B)); however, more subtle roles for 5-HT(1D) and 5-HT(2B) autoreceptors have also been detected. This review provides an overview of 5-HT autoreceptors, focusing on their contribution in animal behavioral models of stress and emotion. Experiments targeting 5-HT autoreceptors in awake, behaving animals have generally shown that increasing autoreceptor feedback is anxiolytic and rewarding, while enhanced 5-HT function is aversive and anxiogenic; however, the role of serotonergic activity in behavioral models of helplessness is more complex. The prevailing model suggests that 5-HT autoreceptors become desensitized in response to stress exposure and antidepressant administration, two seemingly opposite manipulations. Thus there are still unresolved questions regarding the role of these receptors-and serotonin in general-in normal and pathological states. 相似文献
17.
Introduction: Bálint’s syndrome involves bilateral damage to the parieto-occipital area. The extent of the effect of unilateral damage on the Bálint’s triad (oculomotor apraxia, optic ataxia, and simultanagnosia) remains unknown. Methods: We examined a 63-year-old, right-handed woman who developed right hemianopia, oculomotor apraxia, optic ataxia, simultanagnosia, and hemispatial neglect (HSN) for the right after a cerebral infarction, with detailed neuropsychological tests, magnetic resonance imaging, and single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT). Results: Neuropsychological examination showed that oculomotor apraxia, optic ataxia, and simultanagnosia were more pronounced in the right hemi-space, probably due to the limited eye movement in the right visual field, whereas HSN was restricted to the right hemi-space. Diffusion-weighted MR images revealed hyperintensity in the left parieto-temporo-occipital region, and several spotty areas of the bilateral frontal and parietal subcortical regions. SPECT revealed hypoperfusion in the left parieto-occipital region and frontal operculum and small areas of the right superior parietal lobule. Conclusions: The case suggests that asymmetric (more pronounced in the right hemi-space) oculomotor apraxia, optic ataxia, and simultanagnosia occur in an extensive lesion of the left parieto-occipital cortices. Although HSN is not a prerequisite for simultanagnosia, the coexistence of HSN aggravates simultanagnosia in the hemi-space opposite the lesion. 相似文献
18.
Spinocerebellar ataxia type 7 (SCA7) is an autosomal dominant cerebellar ataxia caused by CAG repeat expansion. We found expansion at SCA7 locus in only two out of 235 Indian families clinically diagnosed for ataxia. In one of the families, a de novo mutation was observed wherein a paternal allele in intermediate range of 31 CAG repeats expanded to 59 in the offspring leading to the disease. No expanded alleles were observed in the sperm of the transmitting parent by small pool PCR. This suggests that de novo expansion by a pre-zygotic event is unlikely and could be post-zygotic. SCA7 expanded alleles from the two families were present on different genetic backgrounds, indicating multiple origins of the mutation. 相似文献
19.
Daily living often requires pedestrians and drivers to adapt their behavior to the displacement of other objects in their
environment in order to avoid collision. Yet little research has paid attention to the effect of age on the completion of
such a challenging task. The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between age and collision avoidance skill
and whether a sporting activity affects this. Three age groups (20–30, 60–70, and 70–80 years) of tennis players and non-players
launched a projectile toward a target in order to hit it before it was hit by another “object” (a stimulus represented by
apparent motion of lights). If the participant judged that time-to-collision (TTC) of the moving stimulus was not long enough
for him/her to launch the projectile in time to arrive before the stimulus, the participant had to inhibit the launching.
Results showed that for the non-players the number of errors in the 70–80 year-old group was significantly higher than those
of the 20–30 and 60–70 year-old groups, which did not differ from each other. However, this increase was not observed in the
70-80 year-old tennis players, demonstrating a beneficial effect of playing tennis on collision avoidance skill. Results also
revealed that the older groups of both tennis players and non-players were subject to the typical age-related increase in
response time. Additional analyses indicated that the 70–80 year-old non-players did not adjust their actions to these age-related
changes in response time. The older tennis-playing participants, however, were more likely to adjust collision avoidance behavior
to their diminished response times.
相似文献
|