首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
Although many studies have indicated information processing deficits in schizophrenic patients, the precise nature and underlying causes of these deficits remain largely uncertain. One prominent hypothesis is that these patients show insufficient attentional inhibition. This deficit to inhibition has been linked to certain cognitive disorders in schizophrenic patients, including attention deficits, as well as to some clinical symptoms, especially those involving delusional thought, hallucinations,and poor contact with reality. The hypothesis of deficient attentional inhibition, although attractive in some ways, is difficult to work with, because it is not easy to directly measure "attentional inhibition". Several studies involving normal subjects have linked attentional inhibition with performance on a task demanding the suppression of distracting information: the presumption is that efficient attentional inhibition will permit rapid responses because the distracting information will be quickly suppressed, allowing undistracted processing of the target information. The present study measures schizophrenic patients' performance on a task demanding suppression of rapidly-presented visual information. An important methodological feature of this study is that performance is measured in terms of "percent correct responses" rather than the reaction time measures typically used in tasks demanding distractor suppression, such as Stroop-like selective attention tasks. Since reaction times are not considered, the results cannot be interpreted in terms of deficient response organization and execution. Schizophrenic (18) and normal (18) subjects underwent trials in which a visual target was the second of two stimuli presented in rapid succession. Interference produced by a non-target significantly impaired perception of the target for schizophrenic patients. This effect persisted longer in the schizophrenic subjects possibly because of deficient attentional inhibition.  相似文献   

2.
J Everett  L Laplante 《L'Encéphale》1991,17(3):171-178
Since McGhie & Chapman's (1961) pioneering work, there have been continual attempts to clarify the link between attentional disturbances observable in schizophrenics and their schizophrenic symptoms. Venables (1964, 1977) claims that this diminution is a consequence of a badly controlled arousal, resulting in an inadequate filtering at the level of sensory input. Although the notion of a faulty sensory filter is simple and intellectually satisfying, this idea rapidly met with articulate opposition. Broadbent himself, in 1971, revised his model to take into account the way in which semantic aspects influence selective attention, and proposed the existence of a second stage of information processing, coming after the sensory filter. It became increasingly clear that progress in understanding selective attention would depend upon more centrally-directed approaches. In 1935, Stroop developed an ingenious way of studying selective attention. In his task, an "automatic", overlearned task (reading short words aloud) is put into conflict with a slightly more difficult task (colour-naming): the subject has to name the colour of the ink in which a word is printed, but the word itself is the name of a different colour. For instance, when the word "blue" is written in red ink, the correct response is "red". Typically, subjects find it difficult to inhibit the reading response, and take significantly more time to name a colour that forms an incompatible word than take to name the colour of a row of letters "x". This increased reaction time for the incompatible condition is attributed to an "interference" caused by the verbal information contained in the double stimulus.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)  相似文献   

3.
The purpose of this study was to investigate how underlying cognitive deficits such as a defect in processing speed or in selective attention contributed to different types of memory impairment observed in schizophrenia (superficial vs deep encoding). 49 schizophrenic patients and 40 normal controls were administered a verbal memory task. Superficial encoding was assessed by the ability to recall items in their serial order. Deep encoding was assessed by the ability to organise words into semantic categories. Two measures of processing speed (Digit Symbol Substitution Test and Stroop colour time) and one measure of selective attention (Stroop test) were used. Regression analyses were carried out. In the patient group, processing speed contributed to both superficial and deep encoding, and to a global verbal memory score. Selective attention only contributed to the superficial encoding processes. Thus, slowing of processing speed in schizophrenia seems to be more crucial for memory performance, since it affects memory in a pervasive way.  相似文献   

4.
Although attentional dysfunction is considered a predominant feature of schizophrenia, it is seldom quantitatively assessed in clinical practice. A simple paper-and-pencil test of selective attention (Ruff et al., 1986) provides a 5-min assessment with theoretical relevance for schizophrenia. Thirty schizophrenic subjects performed the 2 and 7 Selective Attention Test, which measures speed and accuracy, and compares controlled and automatic information processing. Ninety percent of the subjects displayed automatic processing. Speed was severely impaired, and accuracy was less so. Sixty-seven percent of the schizophrenic subjects performed in the normal range (± 1 SD) for accuracy, but only 23% scored in the normal range for speed. Speed and accuracy were moderately correlated. Although theories of a lateralized deficit were not supported, the results are consistent with a hypothesized frontal lobe dysfunction. The automatic processing demonstrated may represent old learning. Poor controlled processing may preclude the acquisition of automatic functioning. This selective attention test may provide a measure of severity of cognitive impairment relevant to daily life functioning.  相似文献   

5.
In two experiments, functional laterality and interhemispheric transfer was investigated in schizophrenic patients (n=14) and healthy controls (n=17). In Experiment 1, words and pseudowords were presented either to the left or right ear (monaural condition) or simultaneously to both ears (binaural condition). In Experiment 2, subjects had to discriminate two tones differing in frequency during monaural and binaural stimulation.Healthy controls showed a right ear advantage (REA) for word stimuli, indicating left-hemispheric superiority for word processing. The same lateralization pattern was found in schizophrenic patients, indicating unimpaired functional lateralization of auditory language processing. In both groups, no REA was found for pseudowords resulting in significant WordnessxEar interactions. When presented binaurally, auditory processing of words and pseudowords did not differ significantly from any of the two monaural conditions. Tone discrimination did not lead to any ear asymmetry. The results show normal patterns of functional asymmetry during auditory language processing and tone discrimination in schizophrenic patients.  相似文献   

6.
Repetition effect in event-related potentials (ERPs) was studied in 10 non-thought-disordered (non-TD) patients with schizophrenia, 8 thought-disordered (TD) patients with schizophrenia, and 10 normal control subjects while they performed a semantic categorization task with incidental word repetitions. All patients were in a stable or partially remitted stage. Although both healthy control and non-TD groups produced more positive ERPs to the repeated words than to the new words (ERP repetition effect) for 250-500 ms, the TD group did not show the ERP repetition effect. These findings suggest that the abnormal attenuation of the ERP repetition effect during semantic processing may be more prominent in schizophrenic patients with thought disorder than in those without the symptom.  相似文献   

7.
The present study investigated whether schizophrenic patients could develop appropriate visual orientation and motor set under precuing conditions which contrasted attentional (input selective) and intentional (output selective) information. The aim was to evaluate perceptual performance in processing visuospatial information, and executive performance in response preparation. Stimuli and/or elicited responses were controlled for selective hemispheric engagement. Age, sex and handedness matched groups of 33 chronic schizophrenic patients and 33 normal subjects were tested on choice reaction time (RT) tasks in which warning signals were manipulated regarding either where a target stimulus would occur (selective attention) or which hand to use for responding (response preparation). All subjects benefited from precued information regarding subsequent responses. However, schizophrenic patients were not able to use intentional cues as effectively as control subjects did. Interhemispheric asymmetry of spatial attention was found in patients with schizophrenia, with slowing of responses to uncued targets presented in the right visual field. There was also a decreased advantage of within-hemisphere stimulus-response conditions in the schizophrenic group. Our results support the notion that a dysfunction involving parietal and premotor areas has potential importance in the schizophrenic illness. We replicated findings which indicate that deficits of information processing in schizophrenia may affect left hemispheric mechanisms to a larger extent. The results also point toward a possible abnormal connectivity between frontal and parietal circuits in schizophrenia.  相似文献   

8.
In the present study, we focused on the ability of suppression of processing in schizophrenic patients, using event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded during a selective attention task. During the task, subjects were required to focus on one ear, counting deviant stimuli, those deviating in duration from a sequence of standard stimuli. We compared amplitude data of two positive components differing in latency elicited by standard stimuli, which reflect suppression of stimulus processing, between schizophrenic patients and normal controls. Significant between-group differences were obtained specifically in the right ear attended condition, suggesting impaired suppression of processing in schizophrenics mediated in the left hemisphere.  相似文献   

9.
Verbal and visual recognition tasks were administered to 40 patients with schizophrenia and 40 healthy comparison subjects. The verbal recognition task consisted of discriminating between 16 target words and 16 new words. The visual recognition task consisted of discriminating between 16 target pictures (8 black-and-white and 8 color) and 16 new pictures (8 black-and-white and 8 color). Visual recognition was followed by a spatial context discrimination task in which subjects were required to remember the spatial location of the target pictures at encoding. Results showed that recognition deficit in patients was similar for verbal and visual material. In both schizophrenic and healthy groups, men, but not women, obtained better recognition scores for the colored than for the black-and-white pictures. However, men and women similarly benefited from color to reduce spatial context discrimination errors. Patients showed a significant deficit in remembering the spatial location of the pictures, independently of accuracy in remembering the pictures themselves. These data suggest that patients are impaired in the amount of visual information that they can encode. With regards to the perceptual attributes of the stimuli, memory for spatial information appears to be affected, but not processing of color information.  相似文献   

10.
BACKGROUND: Disturbances in language associations were among the first clinical symptoms reported for individuals described as schizophrenic (Bleuler 1911/1950). Currently, associative language disturbance is a diagnostic feature of schizophrenia (American Psychiatric Association 1994); however, the mechanisms that produce this symptom remain unknown. In the present study, two candidate psychological functions were examined: sensitivity to semantic context and expectancy (attention). METHODS: Visual event-related potentials were recorded during a lexical decision task in which semantic relationship and expectancy (relatedness proportions) were varied. Semantic priming processes were compared between 34 male normal control subjects tested once and 37 male schizophrenic inpatients evaluated during their participation in a double-blind haloperidol maintenance therapy and placebo replacement protocol. RESULTS: Schizophrenic patients failed to discriminate between associated and unassociated words, as measured by the amplitude of the N400 component (i.e., absence of the N400 priming effect); however, the overall mean amplitude of N400 did not differ between patients and control subjects. In addition, patients and control subjects did not differ significantly in the amplitude of N400 elicited to associated words or to unassociated words. Finally, the effect of expectancy-based processing on the magnitude of the N400 priming effect did not differ between patients and control subjects. CONCLUSIONS: On the basis of these findings, a tentative hypothesis is suggested that schizophrenic patients are characterized by a pattern of indiscriminate or random spread of activation in their semantic network during the processing of single-word semantic contexts.  相似文献   

11.
The Stroop Word-Color Test was used to study selective attention deficits in schizophrenic and nonschizophrenic psychiatric patients, compared with nonpsychiatric control subjects. Parts of the Weschler Memory Test and a shortened version of the Stroop Test were administered to test the hypotheses that the attentional deficit could be explained by problems of memory or cognitive fatigue. All patients showed deficits on all of the Stroop scales, but closer analysis of the results permitted discrimination of schizophrenic from nonschizophrenic patients. Schizophrenic patients showed as much difficulty as nonschizophrenic subjects on a limited-duration selective-attention task, but deteriorated significantly more when selective attention had to be maintained. The results could not be attributed to memory problems in the schizophrenic group. The results support the existence of two separable selective attention deficits in schizophrenic patients: a difficulty in selectively attending to the salient aspect of a complex stimulus, a difficulty shared with nonschizophrenic subjects, and a difficulty in maintaining selective attention over time that seems to be more marked in schizophrenic subjects.  相似文献   

12.
The memory profile of acute and chronic schizophrenic patients was examined according to the information processing model, with which encoding, retention and retrieval processes of these patients were compared. The effects of an external organizational strategy on their verbal learning and memory were also examined. Twenty chronic and 20 acute schizophrenic patients were tested with a list learning task consisting of a random (i.e., words presented randomly) and a blocked (i.e., words presented in clusters) word-list. The schizophrenic patients, as compared with age- and education-matched normal control subjects, demonstrated impaired learning, and the duration of their illness was not a significant factor in the severity of their learning impairment. However, the acute and chronic schizophrenic patients seem able to retain most of the newly acquired materials, regardless of the presentation format, after 30 min. In addition, the learning and subjective organizational strategy of the chronic patients, but not that of the acute patients, improved significantly by the blocked presentation. However, semantic organization could facilitate both the chronic and acute schizophrenic patients to retain more newly learned items.  相似文献   

13.
Miniussi C  Marzi CA  Nobre AC 《Neuropsychologia》2005,43(10):1514-1528
We investigated the ability of subjects to shift dynamically between selective task sets, using informative trial-by-trial cues. Two tasks were used which involved non-overlapping neural systems and different hemispheric specialization. In a verbal task, subjects decided whether a letter string was a real word or a non-word. In a spatial task, subjects decided whether an angle was acute or obtuse. A behavioural experiment showed that performance improved when cues predicted the upcoming task (80% validity), compared to when neutral cues did not afford selective task sets. Event-related potentials (ERPs) revealed brain activity related to forming selective task expectations, to switching tasks, and to the modulation of target processing as a function of such expectations and switches. Activity predicting the probable task started over parietal electrodes 160 ms after cue presentation, while activity related to task switching started at frontal electrodes around 280 ms. Both types of activities developed before target onset. Target processing was significantly influenced by the validity of the cue prediction, including strong modulation of language-related potentials. These results show that it is possible to switch dynamically between task sets involving distinct neural systems, even before the appearance of an imperative target stimulus, and that the nature of the task sets can influence neural activity related to task-set reconfiguration. Selective task sets can in turn modulate the processing of target stimuli. The effects also apply to the case of foveally presented words, whose processing has often been hypothesized to be automatic and outside the influence of selective attention.  相似文献   

14.
Schizophrenic subjects were administered the span of apprehension task, which is a measure of visual information processing; two neuropsychological tests; and measures of specific aspects of thought disorder and general clinical state. The measures were administered both when patients were acutely disturbed and when they were partially recovered. Normal control subjects were tested over a comparable 12-week interval. Improvements in both overall clinical condition and specific aspects of thought disorder occurred in the schizophrenic patients during this time. The patients, however, continued to show impaired information processing, indicating that the span of apprehension task is sensitive to schizophrenic dysfunction across wide variations in clinical state and, therefore, may be a marker of vulnerability to schizophrenia. The span of apprehension task was found to be significantly correlated with a measure of thought disorder that assesses resistance to associative distractors and two neuropsychological tests--the Trail-Making Test from the Halstead-Reitan battery and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test--during the testing session conducted while the subjects were partially recovered.  相似文献   

15.
Attentional performance in positive- and negative-symptom schizophrenia   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Two tasks designed to measure selective attention were administered to schizophrenics, patients with bipolar disorder, and normal subjects. Schizophrenics were divided into three subgroups: positive-, negative-, and mixed-symptom patients. Positive-symptom schizophrenics showed significant deficits on a digit-span task when compared to normal subjects. Furthermore, the positive group was the only one to show a significant performance decrement in the distraction condition of the digit-span task. There were no significant group differences in performance on a dichotic listening test. The results of the present study are contrary to the hypothesis that selective attention deficits are characteristic of negative-symptom schizophrenia. Instead, the findings suggest that positive symptoms are associated with greater susceptibility to distraction.  相似文献   

16.
Brain activations associated with semantic processing of visual and auditory words were investigated using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). For each form of word presentation, subjects performed two tasks: one semantic, and one nonsemantic. The semantic task was identical for both auditory and visual presentation: single words were presented and subjects determined whether the word was concrete or abstract. In the nonsemantic task for auditory words, subjects determined whether the word had one syllable or multiple syllables. In the nonsemantic task for visual words, subjects determined whether the word was presented in lower case or upper case. There was considerable overlap in where auditory and visual word semantic processing occurred. Visual and auditory semantic tasks both activated the left inferior frontal (BA 45), bilateral anterior prefrontal (BA 10, 46), and left premotor regions (BA 6) and anterior SMA (BA 6, 8). Left posterior temporal (middle temporal and fusiform gyrus) and predominantly right‐sided cerebellar activations were observed during the auditory semantic task but were not above threshold during visual word presentation. The data, when averaged across subjects, did not show obligatory activation of left inferior frontal and temporal language areas during nonsemantic word tasks. Individual subjects showed differences in the activation of the inferior frontal region while performing the same task, even though they showed similar response latency and accuracy. Hum. Brain Mapping 7:15–28, 1999. © 1999 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.  相似文献   

17.
J Wale  G Geffen 《Neuropsychologia》1986,24(4):483-496
Dichotic word monitoring tasks were given to four cases with complete forebrain comissurotomy, three cases of partial section of the corpus callosum, one case of right hemispherectomy, and control subjects. The task involved a unimanual response to target words in either ear (divided attention, Experiment 1) or to target words in one voice that changed ears unpredictably (focused attention, Experiment 2). Left-ear extinction, defined as chance level (25%) detection of left-ear targets and 75% right-ear target detection, varied both between and within subjects during divided attention. No examples of extinction were obtained with focused attention. These results highlight attentional aspects of dichotic listening. Neither the structural, hemispheric asymmetry model (Cortex 3, 163-178, 1967), nor the cognitive, attention priming model (Attention and Performance V, pp. 87-97, Academic Press, New York) could account for the obtained results. We propose an alternative account in terms of the different processing modes and attentional capabilities of the two cerebral hemispheres.  相似文献   

18.
BACKGROUND: Abnormal smooth pursuit eye movements have been found in many schizophrenic patients and in about 40% of their first-degree biological relatives. A velocity discrimination deficit has also been demonstrated in schizophrenic patients. In this study, we address the relation between deficient velocity discrimination and impaired smooth pursuit eye movements, inasmuch as the brain regions responsible for processing velocity signals are implicated in generating and maintaining smooth pursuit. METHODS: Horizontal eye movements of 15 schizophrenic patients and 8 normal controls were recorded in response to sine wave (predictable) and step-ramp (nonpredictable) targets. Smooth pursuit eye movements were assessed during both the initiation and maintenance periods. Correlations were computed between measures of smooth pursuit (qualitative rating, peak gain, saccade frequency, and initial acceleration) and contrast sensitivity for velocity discrimination. RESULTS: Contrast sensitivity for fine velocity discrimination was significantly correlated both with initial acceleration of smooth pursuit and with peak gain, but was not significantly correlated with saccade frequency and qualitative ratings of pursuit integrity. No significant correlations were found within the normal control group. CONCLUSION: Deficient processing of velocity information seems to be one component that contributes to a dysfunction in the initiation and maintenance of smooth pursuit in schizophrenia.  相似文献   

19.
We investigated the incidental and intentional learning performance of 29 patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease (PD) and 20 healthy volunteers. Measures of incidental and intentional memory were assessed with a spoken verbal recall task followed by a temporal word order task, a picture recall task followed by a spatial picture location task, and a written word recall task followed by a judgement of word frequency task. A multivariate analysis of variance and appropriate post hoc tests revealed significant differences for all intentional learning variables. In all cases, PD patients performed worse than normal controls. No group differences were found for the incidental retention of contextual information. The results indicate that PD patients have a selective problem of intentional learning, whereas incidental learning of contextual information remains intact. Elaborate processing, attentional and organizing strategies in the systematic encoding, and recollection of information have been attributed to dorsolateral prefrontal activation. Preferential dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex-associated memory processes is consistent with the previously reported uneven patterns of dopamine loss in the striatum of patients with idiopathic PD showing most severe dopamine depletion in the portion of the caudate nucleus that is anatomically connected to the dorsolateral frontal cortex.  相似文献   

20.
The Judgment of Line Orientation Test, a visuospatial processing task, was administered to normal subjects, to schizophrenic patients and to patients with delusional disorder. Significantly better performance was seen in the normal subjects than in the schizophrenic and delusional patients. Delusional patients, in turn, showed better performance than the schizophrenic patients.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号