首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 525 毫秒
1.
Kelly JP  Chang S 《Vision research》2000,40(14):1887-1905
This study measured the development of contrast-sweep VEP thresholds to a range of chromatic and luminance stimuli. Subjects were 14-32 week-old infants (n=21) and three adults. Stimuli were 1 c/d sine gratings reversed at 5.6 Hz. Chromaticity was varied from the L-M axis to an achromatic axis. VEP thresholds when plotted in L- and M-cone contrasts showed that: (1) VEP thresholds did not consistently locate the psychophysical isoluminance match under the same stimulus conditions; (2) About 50% of the data were described by independent chromatic and luminance mechanism, however, thresholds were limited by the cone contrast of the stimulus, phase cancellation between visual mechanisms, and the proper sampling of thresholds in L- and M-cone contrast space and; (3) No significant changes occurred in VEP detection contours across ages but suprathreshold VEP amplitudes suggested complex developmental changes. Data from previous studies showed similar results.  相似文献   

2.
Sun H  Lee BB 《Visual neuroscience》2004,21(3):315-320
Vernier thresholds are determined by luminance rather than chromatic contrast when both are present in vernier targets. The role of luminance and chromatic mechanisms in vernier performance under equiluminant conditions remains uncertain. Temporal summation functions for vernier thresholds with luminance and red-green equiluminant gratings were compared to those for detection thresholds with similar stimuli. Vernier thresholds showed similar temporal summation for luminance and chromatic gratings, which is consistent with a single mechanism underlying vernier performance in the two conditions. However, detection thresholds showed a shorter temporal summation duration for luminance gratings than for chromatic gratings, which suggests that two different mechanisms underlie detection thresholds. Analysis of physiological data supports the hypothesis that the frequency-doubled response of ganglion cells in the magnocellular pathway can provide accurate spatiotemporal information for vernier performance at equiluminance.  相似文献   

3.
This study concerns the spatial-frequency-tuned channels underlying infants' contrast sensitivity functions (CSFs) for red-green chromatic stimuli, and their relationship to the channels underlying infants' CSFs for luminance-modulated stimuli. Behavioral (forced-choice preferential-looking) techniques and stationary stimuli were used. In experiment 1. contrast thresholds were measured in 4- and 6-month-olds, using isoluminant red-green gratings with spatial frequencies ranging from 0.27 to 1.53 c deg. In experiment 2. contrast thresholds were measured in 4-month-olds. using both red-green and luminance-modulated gratings in the same low spatial frequency range. Covariance analyses of individual differences were performed. Experiment 1 revealed one dominant covariance channel for the detection of red-green gratings, with a second channel contributing to detection of the highest spatial frequencies used. Experiment 2 revealed two to three channels serving color and luminance: but surprisingly these channels were not statistically separable for luminance versus chromatic stimuli. Thus, covariance channels for color and luminance that are independent for adults [Peterzell & Teller (2000). Spatial frequency tuned covariance channels for red-green and luminance-modulated gratings: psychophysical data from human adults. Vision Research, 40, 417-430] are apparently interdependent in infants. These data suggest that for infants, detection thresholds for chromatic and luminance-modulated stimuli may be limited by common mechanisms.  相似文献   

4.
C R Ingling  B A Drum 《Vision research》1973,13(6):1151-1163
Gouras has reported two types of ganglion cell receptive fields which have the properties required by the conventional achromatic and chromatic visual channels of psychophysics. These receptive fields are treated as spread functions, and, in a manner analogous to that used for predicting brightness contrast phenomena by convoluting targets with a spread function, Gouras's receptive fields are used to predict responses to hue and brightness edges. These predictions are correlated with the results of psychophysical experiments on heterochromatic brightness matches, minimum border matches, chromatic gratings of uniform luminance and chromatic gratings with high achromatic contrast.  相似文献   

5.
D J McKeefry 《Vision research》2001,41(15):2005-2025
Visually Evoked Potentials (VEPs) were recorded in response to the onset of chromatic and luminance motion gratings of 1 cpd and luminance 40 cd m(-2) subtending a 7 degrees field. At slow speeds (< or =2 cycles s(-1)) the motion onset response exhibits a clear amplitude minimum at isoluminance. Over the Michelson contrast range tested (0.05-0.75) the chromatic response at 2 cycles s(-1) possesses a linear response function compared to the saturating function of the luminance response and the contrast dependency of the former is a factor of 5-6 times greater than for the latter. These differences are suggestive of different neural substrates for the chromatic and luminance motion VEPs at slow speeds. At 10 cycles s(-1) the chromatic motion onset VEP exhibits no amplitude minimum at isoluminance and becomes more like its luminance counterpart in terms of its saturating contrast response function. Furthermore, the contrast dependency of the chromatic and luminance responses differs by only a factor of 1.6 at this faster rate. These findings are consistent with the idea of separate motion mechanisms that operate at fast and slow speeds, the latter having separate channels for colour and luminance motion.  相似文献   

6.
《Vision research》1996,36(21):3557-3566
We have investigated whether ageing affects selectively the responses to equiluminant patterns of pure colour contrast. In two groups of subjects (mean ages 29 and 72 yr) contrast thresholds were measured psychophysically for the detection and for the discrimination of the direction of motion of drifting gratings. The gratings were modulated either in pure luminance contrast (and uniform colour), or pure chromatic contrast (red-green equiluminant gratings). In subjects of the same age groups, visual evoked potentials (VEP) were recorded in response to gratings with either pure luminance contrast or pure colour contrast sinusoidally reversed in contrast at various temporal frequencies. It was shown that psychophysical contrast sensitivity for equiluminant patterns deteriorates significantly with age, and VEP latency increases. However, these effects of ageing on the responses to patterns of pure colour contrast are substantially the same as those observed in the same subjects for stimuli with pure luminance contrast. The results suggest that ageing causes a small and unspecific decline of the response of the visual system to luminance and colour contrast. Copyright © 1996 Elsevier Science Ltd.  相似文献   

7.
A number of recent studies have explored the role of the chromatic system in motion processes using the isoluminance paradigm. A major concern when using such methodological procedures is potential artefacts produced by chromatic aberrations. In the present study we address the problem of optically induced luminance artefacts produced by transverse chromatic aberrations (TCA), which may contaminate the results obtained in chromatic motion-nulling experiments. Results show that different TCA levels artificially increase chromatic motion sensitivity values to varying degrees above 0.5 cpd for red/green gratings. The data also suggest the notion that naturally occurring TCA can decrease motion-nulling thresholds for chromatic gratings at high spatial frequencies. Furthermore, our data show that the motion-nulling paradigm for chromatic gratings may in fact be an efficient functional method for assessing the amount of TCA produced by optical factors.  相似文献   

8.
The idea of a largely segregated processing of color and form was initially supported by observations that geometric-optical illusions vanish under isoluminance. However, this finding is inconsistent with some psychophysical studies and also with physiological evidence showing that color and luminance are processed together by largely overlapping sets of neurons in the LGN, in V1, and in extrastriate areas. Here we examined the strength of nine geometric-optical illusions under isoluminance (Delboeuf, Ebbinghaus, Hering, Judd, Müller-Lyer, Poggendorff, Ponzo, Vertical, Z?llner). Subjects interactively manipulated computer-generated line drawings to counteract the illusory effect. In all cases, illusions presented under isoluminance (both for colors drawn from the cardinal L-M or S-(L+M) directions of DKL color space) were as effective as the luminance versions (both for high and low contrast). The magnitudes of the illusion effects were highly correlated across subjects for the different conditions. In two additional experiments we determined that the strong illusions observed under isoluminance were not due to individual deviations from the photometric point of isoluminance or due to chromatic aberrations. Our findings show that our conscious percept is affected similarly for both isoluminance and luminance conditions, suggesting that the joint processing for chromatic and luminance defined contours may extend well beyond early visual areas.  相似文献   

9.
Displacement thresholds with incremental chromatic and luminance edges were measured on different backgrounds. Above 3% luminance contrast, thresholds were always similar. At luminance contrasts below 3%, luminance edges could not be detected, but chromatic edges were still visible. At these low contrasts displacement thresholds for chromatic edges increased to a high level. We interpret these data in terms of multiple mechanisms; above 3% contrast a luminance mechanism determines thresholds, but when, at lower contrasts, chromatic mechanisms support detection, they also support the spatial task. Physiological data were consistent with the different mechanisms originating at the retinal ganglion cell level.  相似文献   

10.
D T Lindsey  D Y Teller 《Vision research》1990,30(11):1751-1761
Subjects viewed a 2.3 x 2.3 deg patch of a moving 1.3 c/deg, 3.75 Hz sinusoidal grating, centered 1.8 deg from fixation. Two-alternative forced-choice contrast thresholds were measured along the luminance axis and 10 chromatic axes at isoluminance for three tasks: detection (D), form discrimination (F), and discrimination of upward from downward motion (M). F/D threshold ratios averaged approx. 1:1 on all axes. M/D ratios were approx. 1:1 on the luminance axis, but varied from 3:1 to indeterminately large with chromatic axis at isoluminance. We conclude that under the present conditions there are large, highly specific losses of direction-of-motion information at isoluminance. The results imply the existence of chromatic channels that are labeled for form but not for direction of motion at threshold. The pattern and significance of variations in M/D ratios within the isoluminant plane is also discussed.  相似文献   

11.
M Gur  V Akri 《Vision research》1992,32(7):1253-1262
Visual performance is greatly impaired when tested with heterochromatic isoluminant stimuli. It is thus concluded that the chromatic system contribution to many visual tasks is limited. We suggest that unless color and luminance are shown to be processed independently, such experiments do not demonstrate shortcomings of the chromatic system but rather the inadequacy of using isoluminant stimuli for isolating that system. We hypothesize that color vision has evolved not only to encode color per se but also to enhance luminance-based visual processing, so that for color information to be fully effective, luminance as well as chromatic variations should be present in the stimulus. The hypothesis was tested by studying the contribution of color to spatial vision. The human contrast sensitivity function (CSF) was studied using luminance, isoluminance (color) and combined luminance/color sinusoidal gratings. It is found that luminance contrast sensitivity is enhanced when luminance contrast is accompanied by color contrast and vice versa. The nature of the interaction is best described by an additive single analyzer model. Color opponent cells which respond to both chromatic and achromatic stimuli may be identified as the analyzer.  相似文献   

12.
Luminance signals mediated by the magnocellular (MC) pathway play an important role in vernier tasks. MC ganglion cells show a phase advance in their responses to sinusoidal stimuli with increasing contrast due to contrast gain control mechanisms. If the phase information in MC ganglion cell responses were utilized by central mechanisms in vernier tasks, one might expect systematic errors caused by the phase advance. This systematic error may contribute to the contrast paradox phenomenon, where vernier performance deteriorates, rather than improves, when only one of the target pair increases in contrast. Vernier psychometric functions for a pair of gratings of mismatched contrast were measured to seek such misestimation. In associated electrophysiological experiments, MC and parvocellular (PC) ganglion cells' responses to similar stimuli were measured to provide a physiological reference. The psychophysical experiments show that a high-contrast grating is perceived as phase advanced in the drift direction compared to a low-contrast grating, especially at a high drift rate (8 Hz). The size of the phase advance was comparable to that seen in MC cells under similar stimulus conditions. These results are consistent with the MC pathway supporting vernier performance with achromatic gratings. The shifts in vernier psychometric functions were negligible for pairs of chromatic gratings under the conditions tested here, consistent with the lack of phase advance both in responses of PC ganglion cells and in frequency-doubled chromatic responses of MC ganglion cells.  相似文献   

13.
In this paper we use a dynamic noise-masking paradigm to explore the nature of the mechanisms mediating the motion perception of drifting isoluminant red-green gratings. We compare contrast thresholds for the detection and direction discrimination of drifting gratings (1.5 cpd), over a range of temporal frequencies (0.5-9 Hz) in the presence of variable luminance or chromatic noise. In the first experiment, we used dynamic luminance noise to show that direction thresholds for red-green grating motion are masked by luminance noise over the entire temporal range tested, whereas detection thresholds are unaffected. This result indicates that the motion of nominally isoluminant red-green gratings is mediated by luminance signals. We suggest that stimulus-based luminance artifacts are not responsible for this effect because there is no masking of the detection thresholds. Instead we propose that chromatic motion thresholds for red-green isoluminant gratings are mediated by dynamic luminance artifacts that have an internal, physiological origin. We have termed these "temporal chromatic aberration". In the second experiment, we used dynamic chromatic noise masking to test for a chromatic contribution to red-green grating motion. We were unable to find conclusive evidence for a contribution of chromatic mechanisms to the chromatic grating motion, although a contribution at very high chromatic contrasts cannot be ruled out. Our results add to a growing body of evidence indicating the presence of dynamic, internal luminance artifacts in the motion of chromatic stimuli and we show that these occur even at very low temporal rates. Our results are compatible with our previous work indicating the absence of a chromatic mechanism for first order (quasi-linear) apparent motion [Vision Res. 40 (2000) 1993]. We conclude that previous conclusions based on the motion of chromatic red-green gratings should be reassessed to determine the contribution of dynamic luminance artifacts.  相似文献   

14.
The use of chromatic patterns that are equated for luminance has become increasingly popular in psychophysical and neurophysiological studies of visual processing. The currently available techniques for equating different colors for brightness rely upon human reports of perceptual events that are reduced at some luminance ratio. We report here the results of a study using a technique we have recently developed that produces a vivid and compelling motion percept only at isoluminance. That is, unlike previous methods, this technique relies upon a perceptual event (motion) that actually becomes more salient at isoluminance. We also observed that the optokinesis generated by the moving pattern mirrors the perceptual reports at all luminance ratios. If used in this manner, the technique can provide an estimate of chromatic isoluminance in a variety of species and can be used to corroborate a human subject's perceptual experience.  相似文献   

15.
Prolonged viewing of sinusoidal luminance gratings produces elevated contrast detection thresholds for test gratings that are similar in spatial frequency and orientation to the adaptation stimulus. We have used this technique to investigate orientation and spatial frequency selectivity in the processing of color contrast information. Adaptation to isoluminant red-green gratings produces elevated color contrast thresholds that are selective for grating orientation and spatial frequency. Only small elevations in color contrast thresholds occur after adaptation to luminance gratings, and vice versa. Although the color adaptation effects appear slightly less selective than those for luminance, our results suggest similar spatial processing of color and luminance contrast patterns by early stages of the human visual system.  相似文献   

16.
Vimal RL 《Vision research》2002,42(5):599-611
In this study, we have compared foveal SF discriminations for luminance and color-defined stimuli using two different tasks (criteria): in criterion-A, the discrimination is based on spatial (size of the stimuli) and/or spatial frequency; in criterion-B, it is based on apparent motion (contraction/expansion). We used high contrast (75%) spatially localized D6 stimuli and cosine gratings (0.25-9.5 cpd). The SF discrimination was measured by the method of constant stimuli with a two-interval forced-choice procedure. Data show that: (i) for criterion-A, the discrimination thresholds for color stimuli were lower than that for luminance stimuli at low SFs, but similar or higher at higher SFs; for criterion-B, the thresholds to chromatic stimuli were higher than that to achromatic stimuli for all SFs; (ii) SF discrimination was best at inter-stimulus-interval (ISI) of about 200 ms for color stimuli and at ISI of 0 ms for luminance stimuli; (iii) SF discrimination got better with stimulus duration and reached to plateau at 200 ms (or more) for color stimuli and at 67 ms (or more) for luminance stimuli; (iv) SF discrimination threshold (mean Delta(f)=0.19 octaves) is about one-tenth of the full bandwidth (mean=1.96 octaves) of SF tuned mechanisms and is in hyperacuity range; both (discrimination and hyperacuity) can be explained by the relative activities within a population of tuned mechanisms. We conclude that color and luminance SF discrimination thresholds have a different SF dependence. While color appears to perform better than luminance vision at low SFs, this effect is lost or even reversed at high SFs. Data imply that color and form interact, but color and motion are largely segregated (i.e. they weakly interact).  相似文献   

17.
It has been demonstrated widely that at isoluminance moving chromatic stimuli are seen to be stationary or moving more slowly than their luminance counterparts. We have examined the effect on perceived velocity of adding luminance contrast to an isoluminant chromatic stimulus. We show that moving luminance contrast 'captures' colour so that a combined colour and luminance stimulus is seen moving as a unified percept. However, in the presence of colour contrast, significantly higher levels of luminance contrast are required to achieve a veridical velocity than for monochromatic stimuli with only luminance contrast. We show that this interactive effect between colour and luminance contrast cannot be fully explained by a threshold masking of luminance by colour contrast. The effect suggests that a breakdown in the veridical perception of velocity should be expected for colours with a wide range of associated luminance contrasts and not just for those at the point of isoluminance.  相似文献   

18.
Boon MY  Suttle CM  Dain SJ 《Vision research》2007,47(16):2124-2133
It has been found that humans are able to distinguish colours without luminance cues by about 2-4 months of age and that sensitivity to colour difference develops during childhood, reaching a peak around adolescence. This prolonged period of maturation is reflected by improvements in psychophysical threshold measures and by the VEP characteristics of morphology, latency and amplitude. An intra-individual comparison of VEP and psychophysical responses to isoluminant colour stimuli has not been made in children, however, and this was the aim of the present study. VEPs were recorded from 49 subjects, children (age range: 4.8-12.6 years) and adults (age range: 25.7-33.2 years). Psychophysical and VEP thresholds were both measured in 40 of those subjects. Nominally isoluminant chromatic (L-M) sinewave gratings were presented in onset-offset mode and identical stimuli were used for psychophysical and VEP recordings to allow comparison. In agreement with previous reports, morphology of the transient VEP in response to this stimulus differed considerably between children and adults. There was a significant difference between psychophysical and VEP thresholds in children, but not in adults. Our findings support and expand on previous work on maturation of the L-M chromatic pathway and indicate a larger discrepancy between VEP and psychophysical chromatic thresholds in children than in adults.  相似文献   

19.
This study investigates the contribution of color vision to global motion. We present evidence demonstrating that performance on a global motion task attains similar levels for both types of chromatic (L/M-cone opponent and S-cone opponent) and luminance stimuli at suprathreshold contrasts. We show, however, that the motion thresholds for isoluminant chromatic stimuli are luminance based, on the grounds that they are masked by luminance noise but robust to chromatic noise. Detection thresholds, on the other hand, are chromatic in origin (masked by chromatic but not luminance noise), indicating that there is no luminance artifact in the stimulus. We suggest that for color vision at isoluminance the global motion task is based on the integration of many local, luminance-based signals.  相似文献   

20.
Effect of chromatic aberration on isoluminance stereothreshold   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The results of a previous experiment have shown that stereothreshold varies as a function of the luminance difference between a target and its background. When the luminance of the target is the same as that of the background (isoluminance) the stereothreshold was elevated by a factor of 3 as compared with a situation where there was maximum luminance difference between the target and the background. Generally, if the target and the background have the same color, stereothreshold at a particular luminance difference level was lower (stereoacuity better) than if the target and the background have different colors. This indicates a possible effect of chromatic aberration on stereothreshold which could be responsible for the 3-fold increase in threshold at isoluminance. This possibility has been investigated in this experiment. The results show that even though stereothresholds are sensitive to chromatic aberration this factor cannot explain the elevated stereothreshold at isoluminance.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号