首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
The effects of pre-session and post-session daily ethanol injections on the development and loss of tolerance to ethanol's effects on fixed ratio operant performance in rats was assessed using a cumulative dosing procedure. Daily pre-session ethanol administration produced a greater decrease in ethanol sensitivity than did daily post-session ethanol. Both tolerance effects persisted for at least 1 month after the chronic injection phase. No changes in ethanol sensitivity were apparent in the saline control group and no changes in estimated blood ethanol levels were found after the chronic treatments. The post-session ethanol groups displayed a performance decrement during the initial segment of the chronic injection period, but improved significantly across the chronic phase. These data suggest that some delayed effect of ethanol initially impaired performance but that tolerance to this ethanol effect also occurred and probably contributed to the decline in ethanol sensitivity seen in these groups. Compensatory learning as the mechanism for tolerance development in the pre-session and post-session ethanol groups was supported by the finding of no change in ethanol sensitivity in rats exposed to comparable daily ethanol without any concurrent operant task on which the direct, immediate, or indirect, delayed ethanol effects could operate.  相似文献   

2.
Behavioral factors in development of tolerance to ethanol's effects   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Dose-effect analyses were used to monitor the development of tolerance for ethanol's effects on FR30 operant performance in rats under different conditions of chronic ethanol exposure: (a) pre-session ethanol injections (PRE) vs. post-session ethanol injections (POST) in Experiment 1; and (b) an ethanol liquid diet (ED) vs. a control diet (CD) in Experiment 2. The PRE and ED groups developed tolerance at the conclusion of the chronic regimens, which declined by six months but not to baseline levels. These data suggest that tolerance results from learned compensatory adjustments (through intoxicated practice) to ethanol's disruptive effects. The POST, but not the CD, group developed a progressively increasing degree of tolerance after several ethanol challenge tests. These results suggest that some threshold level of passive ethanol exposure in the POST group interacted with their limited intoxicated practice. Finally, the tolerance developed under intoxicated practice conditions did not appear to reflect a generalized tolerance to rate-reducing properties of drugs, changes in ethanol kinetics, or age-related changes.  相似文献   

3.
The purpose of this study was to determine whether repeated treatment (15 days) with d-amphetamine (AMP) or fenfluramine (FEN), administered after a daily 3 h feeding session (e. g. post-session), would result in tolerance or crosstolerance to the decrement in food consumption induced by treatment with either drug before feeding (e. g. pre-session). Groups of male rats were treated IP with 0.5 ml saline, 1.0, 2.0, or 4.0 mg/kg AMP, or 2.5, 5.0, or 10.0 mg/kg FEN prior to a 3 h feeding session. For the next 15 sessions, the respective groups were treated post-session with saline (0.5 ml), AMP (4.0 mg/kg), or FEN (10 mg/kg). Following the 15 day postsession phase, each group again received this pre-session treatment. The initial pre-session treatment with all dosages of these two drugs produced a significant decrease in food consumption. Tolerance to the food intake suppressant effect of FEN, but not AMP, resulted from repeated post-session treatment with the same agent. Repeated post-session treatment with AMP resulted in a significant decrement in the suppressant activity of FEN on food intake, whereas the corresponding post-session treatments with FEN did not alter the pre-session effects of AMP, except for an enhancement seen with higher AMP doses.  相似文献   

4.
Two experiments were performed to test the role of dose interval in the development of tolerance to methylphenidate. Rats were trained to consume sweetened milk and then were given methylphenidate in a dose that decreased milk intake by approx. 50%. For the next 23 sessions they received either saline; methylphenidate daily, immediately post-session; or pre-session methylphenidate, either daily, every-other-day, or every-four-days. The next session, all groups received methylphenidate pre-session. The 3 groups treated on a chronic basis with methylphenidate pre-session returned to baseline levels of milk intake and differed significantly from the daily saline and daily post-session methylphenidate groups, which did not become tolerant. In a second experiment, rats injected presession daily or every fourth day with 15mg/kg methylphenidate developed tolerance to the disruption of milk consumption. As compared to rats treated chronically with saline, the 2 groups given methylphenidate showed a shift of their dose-effect curves to the right and cross-tolerance to d-amphetamine. These results demonstrate that tolerance can occur to the disruptive effects of amphetamine-type drugs even when three drug-free days intervene between administrations. This tolerance is characterized by a shift in the dose-effect curve as well as cross-tolerance to a drug with similar pharmacological properties.  相似文献   

5.
The present study was designed to characterize the degree of cross-tolerance between the response rate-decreasing effects of morphine and three mu opioids with varying relative intrinsic efficacies at the mu receptor, buprenorphine, butorphanol and fentanyl, and a non-opioid (+) amphetamine, in a behavioral-tolerance paradigm. Lever pressing of rats was maintained by a fixed-ratio 20-schedule of food presentation, and dose-effect curves for each drug were obtained prior to, during, and after daily administrations of morphine in separate groups of rats administered morphine either before (pre-session) or after (post-session) experimental sessions. Each of the mu opioids and the non-opioid (+)-amphetamine dose-dependently decreased response rates. In the pre-session group, daily administration of morphine shifted the morphine dose-effect curve 0.33 log unit rightward, indicating that tolerance had developed, and shifted the butorphanol dose-effect curve 0.96 log unit rightward. Daily pre-session administrations of morphine did not shift the dose-effect curves for buprenorphine, fentanyl, or (+)-amphetamine. In the post-session group, daily administration of morphine did not shift the morphine, butorphanol, buprenorphine, fentanyl, or (+)-amphetamine dose-effect curves. These data suggest that pharmacological variables, such as the drug's relative intrinsic efficacy at the mu receptor, can play a role in behavioral tolerance and cross-tolerance.  相似文献   

6.
Consistent with human literature, previous studies identified attention-enhancing effects of nicotine in rats, using a 5-choice task. The present study addressed the influence of repeated exposure to nicotine on these effects. Over six weeks, the effects of nicotine (0.0, 0.05 and 0.2 mg/kg) given ten min before sessions were tested each week. In addition, rats were injected daily two hours after sessions. In the first week, when these post-session injections were of saline for all rats, pre-session nicotine had profound rate-disruptive effects at the larger dose. In weeks 2-6, when half the rats received post-session injections of saline and the other half of nicotine (0.4 mg/kg), the disruptive effects of pre-session nicotine had disappeared and it enhanced attentional performance on all response indices. These effects did not differ significantly between post-session treatment groups or weeks, although they appeared less pronounced in the last two weeks. When tested under modified task parameters in weeks 9 and 10, nicotine (0.1 mg/kg) robustly enhanced performance in both groups despite continuing daily post-session administration of nicotine or saline. These studies provide evidence that, following tolerance to initial disruptive effects, the nicotine-induced attentional enhancement is stable across lengthy periods of chronic exposure. This is important for potential therapeutic applications of the drug and indicates that these effects can be a continuous motive for smoking behavior.  相似文献   

7.
A modified before-after design was used to assess the influence of behavioral contingencies on the development of tolerance to ethanol, monitored with both a behavioral (moving belt, shock avoidance) task and a physiological (body temperature) response measure. Male Long-Evans rats received ethanol (2.0 g/kg IP) and daily moving belt training under one of four conditions: (1) daily injection of ethanol before moving belt sessions; (2) daily injection of ethanol after belt sessions, with intermitten intoxicated practice; (3) daily injection of ethanol after belt sessions, with no intoxicated practice; (4) daily injections of saline, with intermittent intoxicated practice. After a 28-day tolerance acquisition phase, all groups received a final moving belt test and temperature assessment after injection of ethanol. Development of tolerance to the motor effects of ethanol was seen only in animals receiving daily injections before behavioral training. By contrast, tolerance to the hypothermic effect of ethanol developed equally in all groups. The augmented development of tolerance to the motor effects of ethanol may not, therefore, indicate a generalized state of neuronal adaptation; rather, it appears to reflect a fairly specific response acquired by the organism.  相似文献   

8.
This experiment examined whether development of tolerance to the rate-decreasing effects of morphine can be modulated by the magnitude of the initial effect of morphine. Lever pressing by rats was maintained under a fixed-ratio 30 schedule of food delivery in daily 30 min sessions. Subjects were assigned to two groups, which did not differ in initial sensitivity to morphine, and given daily injections of morphine under dosing schedules chosen to have different initial effects. Group 1 received daily pre-session injections of 10 mg/kg morphine, which initially suppressed lever pressing completely, and post-session injections of saline. Group 2 received morphine in two injections, one pre-session and one post-session. The initial daily doses 10 mg/kg pre- and 9 mg/kg post-session) decreased rates by 30% or less. The proportion of the 10 mg/kg dose administered pre-session was increased gradually over 10 weeks. Group 2 developed greater tolerance than did group 1, as assessed by changes in the dose of morphine required to suppress response rates by > 50%. After 3 months of repeated treatment, the ED(50) of morphine increased 2.5-fold in group 2, but only 1.4-fold in group 1. When the daily dose of morphine was raised to 20 mg/kg (10 mg/kg pre- and 10 mg/kg 14 h post-session) for all subjects, the ED(50) of morphine increased 6.7-fold in group 2, but only 2.6-fold in group 1. During repeated treatment, the groups did not differ in the dose of maloxene required to suppress response rates or elicit weight loss. Following termination of morphine treatment, the ED(50) of morphine returned to original values in group 2, but was 1.8-fold lower than initial values in group 1. Thus, a pronounced [??103] decreasing effect of morphine retarded development of tolerance, perhaps by a conditioning process.  相似文献   

9.
Much of the research implicating learning in the development of tolerance to ethanol-induced impairment has used an experimental design in which different groups receive drug either before or after an opportunity to perform an instrumental or operant task. The stronger tolerance observed in subjects who perform while intoxicated is most often attributed to the reinforced practice of a learned compensatory response. Using an experimental procedure modeled after Chen (1979), the present study examined an alternative theoretical basis for tolerance in the before-versus-after design. Specifically, the effects of Pavlovian context-drug pairings were assessed under circumstances that precluded reinforced practice of the operant response. Three groups of food-deprived rats were initially trained to barpress for sucrose on an FR15 schedule. After 30 sessions, the bar was retracted and the dipper was covered for a 3-day tolerance acquisition phase. During this phase, each group received an IP injection 15 min before and 45 min after each session. The Paired group received ethanol (1.2 g/kg) before and saline after the session, thus pairing ethanol with cues of the test chamber. The Unpaired group received saline before and ethanol after the session, while the No-Drug group always received saline. During a final test phase, all groups received ethanol (1.5 g/kg) before access to sucrose on the FR schedule. The Paired group completed the first FR15 sequence more rapidly than either control group, indicating that context-ethanol pairings enhanced tolerance to the drug's disruptive effect on the initiation of operant responding. However, there was little evidence of conditioned stimulus control over tolerance to ethanol's thermal effect. Overall, these data suggest that stimulus-drug contingencies occurring in operant procedures may play an important role in development of tolerance to ethanol's behavioral effects.  相似文献   

10.
Learning has been claimed to be of major importance in the development of tolerance to ethanol. In the present study we investigated the influence of learning on tolerance to ethanol-induced inhibition of a spinal reflex (tail-flick response) in intact and spinal rats. On day 1 and 9, groups of rats were injected with either ethanol 2.5 g/kg IP or saline 30 min prior to tail-flick testing. On days 2–8 the groups were treated differently in order to reveal the importance of the drug alone, the test alone and the combination of the two on development of tolerance. On day 10, the rats rendered tolerant in the home room were transferred to a new test room to be tested. Both in intact and spinal rats development of tolerance was observed only if the animals were repetitively tested while intoxicated. Tolerance acquired in the home room was not attenuated by transfer to a new environment. Results in the spinal rats suggested that adaptive mechanisms leading to tolerance may also be located in the spinal cord. The tolerance observed may be regarded as learned from practice while intoxicated.  相似文献   

11.
The development of tolerance to the motor impairment effect of ethanol was examined in separate groups of rats receiving and not receiving intoxicated practice. Tolerance to the motor impairment effect of ethanol developed whether or not rats received intoxicated practice during chronic ethanol treatment. Depending on the treatment dosage and test dose, intoxicated practice might enhance the level of tolerance attained. Tolerance to other effects of ethanol (hypothermia and narcosis) developed as a function of the treatment dosage. Intoxicated practice on the moving belt did not modify the development of tolerance to these effects of ethanol. Tolerance to the motor impairment effect of ethanol, however, was retained much longer in the intoxicated practice group following the termination of ethanol treatment.  相似文献   

12.
This experiment was designed to detect compensatory learning that has been suggested to occur during the course of tolerance development to ethanol's effects on operant performance. The effects of presession ethanol injections on the development of tolerance to ethanol's effects on operant performance in an afternoon Fixed-Ratio (FR) task was assessed in rats that were concurrently performing in a morning DRL task. Only presession saline injections were administered for the DRL task. A cumulative dosing procedure was used to establish initial and postethanol exposure dose-effect curves for both tasks. Daily presession ethanol administration produced a 3-fold shift-to-the-right in the dose-effect curve for FR-task performance. No changes were evident in the FR-task performance of controls that received daily saline injections. However, during the period of daily ethanol injections and during subsequent cumulative dose tests, the ethanol, but not the control, group displayed dose-related increases in total DRL-task responses relative to baseline. These DRL data were interpreted as reflecting the development of rate-increasing behaviors that compensated for and contributed to the tolerance of ethanol's rate-decreasing effects on FR-task performance.  相似文献   

13.
Sham and electrolytic lesions of the dorsal, median, and dorsal+median raphe nuclei were made in different groups of rats. One week later, daily oral treatment with ethanol (5 g/kg p.o. for 25 days) was started. This treatment produced tolerance to the hypothermic and motor impairing (moving belt test) effects of ethanol. On day 26, ethanol was stopped and subcutaneous injection of either 10 μg of des-Gly9-[Arg8]vasopressin (DGAVP) in saline or saline alone was started. The retention of tolerance to ethanol was measured at 3-day intervals for both hypothermia and motor-impairment. In sham-saline groups, disappearance of tolerance took 3 days for the hypothermic effect, and 9 days for the motor-impairment effect. Tolerance to both effects, however, was still observed after 9 days in DGAVP-treated rats with either sham or dorsal raphe lesions. Peptide treatment, on the other hand, failed to maintain tolerance in rats with median or median+dorsal raphe lesions. These results suggest that an intact mesolimbic serotonin pathway is necessary for the action of DGAVP on the retention of ethanol tolerance.  相似文献   

14.
Tolerance to the suppressive effects of cocaine on milk drinking by rats was studied using a contingent tolerance experimental design. Three separate groups (n=6) of rats received 8.0, 16.0, or 32.0 mg/kg cocaine daily 15 min before a 15-min period of access to sweetened condensed milk for 20 days. Three additional groups of six rats each received the same chronic doses 15 min after access to milk. Milk, water, and food intake as well as body weight were measured daily. Tolerance effects were assessed by comparing initial acute dose-effect determinations with a probe dose-effect redetermination in which all rats again received doses of cocaine pre-session after having experienced the differential pre- or post-session chronic treatment. Behavioral tolerance on the milk intake measure was observed for the 8.0 mg/kg and 16.0 mg/kg doses, but not for the 32.0 mg/kg chronic treatment, even though the latter group exhibited evidence of tolerance in the water intake measure. Chronic treatment with 8.0 and 16.0 mg/kg produced different outcomes in that chronic exposure to 16.0 mg/kg in the presence of milk resulted in generalization of tolerance to both a lower (8.0 mg/kg) and a higher dose (32.0 mg/kg), but the group receiving 8.0 mg/kg did not exhibit generalization of tolerance to higher doses. Modest sensitization effects were observed in the rats treated post-session with either 8.0 or 16.0 mg/kg. Rats receiving 32.0 mg/kg pre-milk exhibited a dramatic lowering of food intake and concomitant loss in body weight compared to the same dose given after the milk access period, suggesting that pairing cocaine with a highly palatable substance may enhance its toxic effects. The observed behavioral tolerance effects were discussed in terms of (1) the reinforcement loss hypothesis, (2) the channeling hypothesis developed in the context of behavioral tolerance to amphetamine, and (3) the concept of dose-specific tolerance.Supported by DA 05/253  相似文献   

15.
Keypecking by seven pigeons, maintained by a fixed-ratio 30 schedule of food presentation, was decreased in rate by acute pre-session administration of cocaine. In Part 1 (four pigeons), tolerance to the rate-suppressing effects of cocaine developed during daily administration conditions. Tolerance persisted (1) when daily cocaine injections were replaced by conditions in which cocaine was administered every other day, then every fourth day, then every eighth day, then every 16th day, with all intervening sessions preceded by saline injections and (2) when daily cocaine administration was replaced abruptly by a condition in which cocaine injections were spaced 16 days apart, with all intervening sessions preceded by saline. In Part 2 (three pigeons), tolerance developed during intermittent administration conditions (e.g. cocaine injected every eighth day) for two subjects, and during daily administration for the third subject. As in Part 1, tolerance persisted when cocaine was administered only once every 16 days. These results are consistent with an interpretation of tolerance based upon operant compensatory reactions to drug-induced behavioral disruptions and suggest that a simple associationist model of tolerance to cocaine-induced response-rate suppression may be inadequate. The data also have practical implications regarding tolerance development during intermittent administration conditions similar to conventional acute dose-effect determination procedures.  相似文献   

16.
Abstract Rationale. Previous studies have indicated that the conditioned effects of environmental stimuli contribute to ethanol tolerance and abuse. Acamprosate was recently suggested to reduce the effects of environmental stimuli previously associated with ethanol administrations. This action is believed to contribute to the clinical benefits of acamprosate treatment in alcoholics. Objectives. In the present experiment, a classical drug-conditioning paradigm was used to test whether acamprosate modulates the effects of ethanol-paired environmental stimuli on spontaneous motor activity. Methods. Wistar rats were divided into three groups: cued, uncued and control. The cued group daily received ethanol injections (2.0 g/kg, IP) in a specific testing environment. The uncued group daily received ethanol injections (2.0 g/kg, IP) in their home cage but never experienced ethanol in the testing environment. The control group was injected with saline and never experienced ethanol. After 8 conditioning days, the rats were IP injected with various ethanol doses (saline, 1.0, 1.5 or 2.0 g/kg) and their spontaneous motor activity in the testing environment was recorded to investigate their respective tolerance to ethanol inhibitory effects. In the second part of the study, the same procedure was repeated with chronically acamprosate-treated rats. The chronic acamprosate treatment (400 mg/kg per day) started 2 weeks before the conditioning procedure by diluting acamprosate in the drinking bottles and was maintained throughout the whole experiment. Results. The cued rats showed a significant environment-dependent tolerance to ethanol inhibitory effects relative to the uncued and control rats. This higher ethanol tolerance of the cued rats was mainly due to a faster recovery from ethanol's inhibitory effects on spontaneous activity. Furthermore, the cued rats showed a higher level of activity in the testing environment after the saline injection. However, it is not clear whether this hyperactivity is a conditioned compensatory response or an increased exploratory behavior. Acamprosate totally abolished the environment-dependent tolerance to ethanol, whereas it did not alter the hyperactivity of the cued rats in the testing environment. Conclusions. The results of the present study suggest that acamprosate reduces ethanol-conditioned effects. Such an action may be of importance to explain the anti-relapse effects of acamprosate. Electronic Publication  相似文献   

17.
According to the homeostatic model, the loss of tolerance to amphetamine-induced hypophagia requires a period of unrestricted feeding in the drug-free state, which transforms the compensatory response mediating tolerance ("hyperhunger") into a functional disturbance to homeostasis. In the absence of such a disturbance, tolerance should be retained. To test this prediction, rats tolerant to amphetamine's hypophagic effect were given a 4-week tolerance retention period during which milk intakes were restricted and deprivation levels held relatively constant. During this period the rats were assigned to one of the following drug treatment conditions: 1) saline injections both before and after daily milk tests (saline group); 2) saline injections before, and amphetamine injections after, daily milk tests (after group); 3) no injections and no milk tests (no-treatment group); or 4) amphetamine injections before, and saline injections after, milk tests (before group). Despite the restricted feeding regimen, both the saline and after groups lost tolerance. These results do not support the homeostatic model, but are consistent with the instrumental learning model, which views drinking milk in the undrugged state as analogous to receiving noncontingent reinforcement.  相似文献   

18.
An animal model of human reaction time (RT) was used to investigate the effects of age and intoxicated practice on the development of tolerance to the motor impairing effects of ethanol (EtOH). Young (8–9 month) and old (24–26 month) Fischer 344 rats were trained to release a lever in response to an auditory and visual stimulus in order to avoid mild footshock. The animals were divided into groups to receive either intoxicated (EtOH-before) or unintoxicated (EtOH-after) RT testing. Successful avoidance and response latencies were impaired in young and old rats after the initial exposure to EtOH (EtOH-before group). Tolerance developed to EtOH's effects on successful avoidance and on response latencies whether or not the rats received intoxicated RT practice; however, intoxicated practice facilitated tolerance development to EtOH's effects on successful avoidance but not on response latencies. While the initial sensitivity and the level of tolerance that developed to EtOH's effects were similar in young and old rats, the old rats were generally more sensitive to EtOH and developed tolerance at a slower rate. These results suggest that tolerance develops to the effects of EtOH on RT and that intoxicated practice can have different effects on the parameters of the behavioral response.  相似文献   

19.
Experimentally naive male, Sprague-Dawley rats maintained at 85% of their original body weight were trained to touch a retractable lever that was presented on a random interval 48-s schedule. The lever retracted when touched or after 15 s had elapsed, and one 45 mg food pellet was delivered simultaneously with lever retraction or after an 8-s delay. Rats received ten daily sessions each consisting of ten lever presentations. Nicotine (0.25–0.8 mg/kg SC) administration, either 15 min prior to (pre-session) or immediately after (post-session) the daily autoshaping sessions, caused a significant dose-related impairment of acquisition with the post-session injections having the greater effect. Low doses of nicotine (0.025–0.1 mg/kg SC) had little effect on acquisition when injected pre-session or post-session. Injections of 0.45 mg/kg nicotine either immediately (t=0) or at +5 min after the daily sessions impaired acquisition of the lever-touch response. Nicotine injected at +15, +30, +60, or +120 min had no effect on acquisition. A single intraventricular injection of the ganglionic blocker chlorisondamine (5 g) 2 weeks prior to autoshaping blocked the impairment produced by 0.45 mg/kg nicotine. Post-session injections of nicotine did not alter the lever-touch behavior of well-trained animals, but suppressed responding in animals that were partially trained. Thus, nicotine-induced impairment of the autoshaped lever-touch response is dose dependent, centrally mediated, occurs within 5 min of a SC injection, and may interfere with post-training consolidation processes. Offprint requests to: E.T. Iwamoto  相似文献   

20.
This study examines possible interactions between exposure to Ginkgo biloba extract and enriched environments on the acquisition and retention of spatial learning following massed and spaced trials. After 4 weeks of exposure to either ginkgo or vehicle, 8-week-old rats were tested using a Morris Water Maze in either massed or spaced trials. While ginkgo did not have an effect on maze acquisition or long-term retention, it did promote short-term retention of spatial memory. Following reversal training, ginkgo promoted short-term retention for two groups but impaired retention for a third. These results suggest that ginkgo has powerful effects on short-term retention that vary with training conditions.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号