首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
When two clicks are presented within 500 ms and the clicks are separated by several seconds, a typical finding is a suppression of the amplitude of the P50 component of the middle-latency auditory-evoked response. In the present study, we investigated whether only the P50 or also the earlier components Po, Na, Pa and Nb, and the exogenous components N100 and P200 exhibit an amplitude suppression to the second click. In addition, we studied the suppression behaviour of the auditory-evoked 40-Hz gamma-band response in the time and frequency domain. We found a significant amplitude suppression to the second click for all components of the auditory-evoked potential following Po, which was most pronounced at electrode Cz. When testing the successive peaks and troughs of the evoked 40-Hz gamma-band response in the time domain, we found a significant amplitude suppression for peaks and troughs with the same latency and polarity as the middle-latency components following Po, which was most pronounced at electrodes Fz and Cz. Consequently, the amplitude of the 40-Hz evoked gamma-band response in the frequency domain paralleled the findings of the time domain, with a significant amplitude suppression to the second tone, which was most pronounced at electrodes Fz and Cz. Results are discussed with reference to the early sensory-gating hypothesis.  相似文献   

2.
Summary We have studied the effect of selective listening on the neuromagnetic evoked activity of the human auditory cortex. In the word categorization experiment the stimuli were 5-letter words, each beginning with /k/. Half of them were targets, i.e., names of animals or plants, and half other meaningful Finnish words. In the duration discrimination experiment equiprobable tones of 425 ms (targets) or 600 ms duration were presented. In both experiments the interstimulus interval (ISI) was 2.3 s and the stimuli of the two classes were presented randomly. Subjects either ignored the stimuli (reading condition) or counted the number of targets (listening condition). The magnetic field over the head was measured with a 7-channel 1st-order SQUID-gradiometer. The stimuli evoked a transient response followed by a sustained field. The transient response did not differ between the two conditions but the sustained field was significantly larger in the listening than reading condition; the increase began 120–200 ms after stimulus onset and continued for several hundred milliseconds. The equivalent source locations of both transient and sustained responses agreed with activation of the supratemporal auditory cortex. In the dichotic listening experiment 25-ms square-wave stimuli were presented randomly and equiprobably either to the left or to the right ear at an ISI of 0.8–1 s, either alone or in presence of a speech masker. Counting the stimuli of either ear resulted in differences between responses to relevant and irrelevant sounds. The difference began 140–150 ms after stimulus onset and peaked at 200–240 ms. During monaural speech masking, N100m was larger for attended than ignored stimuli. The results suggest that neural mechanisms underlying direction of attention include modification of the activity of the auditory cortex and that the mechanisms are similar for words and tones.  相似文献   

3.
1. We studied extracellular responses of low-frequency cells in the central nucleus of the inferior colliculus (ICC) to interaural time differences (ITDs) of clicks and compared their responses to ITDs of noise and tones. Most cells that displayed sensitivity to ITDs of clicks responded cyclically as a function of ITD with central peaks and troughs at the same ITDs as in response to noise. The positions of these peaks and troughs also matched those predicted from tonal ITD curves. Thus over the range of physiologically relevant ITDs, the binaural cells in the ICC showed similar sensitivity to ITDs of tones, noise, and clicks. 2. The transient nature of the response to a click allowed association of individual discharges with either the ipsilateral or contralateral stimulus when the binaural stimulus included a large ITD. We studied the influence of the click presented to one side on responses to the click presented to the other side. By examining responses to clicks with large ITDs, ranging from 2 to 3 up to 200 ms, we could identify both excitatory and inhibitory components in response to binaural clicks. 3. For many cells, there was evidence for a short-lasting excitation arising from one or both inputs of the binaural stimulus. Inhibitory interactions could also be demonstrated over a large range of ITDs. Long-lasting, late inhibitory components arose from both contralateral and ipsilateral inputs. In 87% of cells that were driven by the contralateral input, a late inhibitory component originating from the ipsilateral side was detected. In all cells that were driven by the ipsilateral side, a late inhibitory contralateral component was detected. This late inhibition of the excitatory response to one side by a leading stimulus to the other side could be evoked even when the leading stimulus was not effective in evoking an excitatory response. 4. Some cells also exhibited an early inhibitory component that preceded the excitation. An early contralateral inhibition was detected in 44% of cells that were driven by the ipsilateral input, whereas an early ipsilateral component was detected in 17% of cells driven by the contralateral input. 5. We confirmed hypotheses about the laterality and time course of the inhibitory and excitatory components by introducing interaural level differences (ILDs) into the binaural clicks and thus varying the strengths of the different components. 6. Inhibitory components may play a role in shaping the sensitivity of individual cells to ITDs of stimuli other than clicks; they were also apparent in responses to noise.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

4.
The auditory magnetic event-related fields (ERF) qualitatively change through the child development, reflecting maturation of auditory cortical areas. Clicks presented with long inter-stimulus interval produce distinct ERF components, and may appear useful to characterize immature EFR morphology in children. The present study is aimed to investigate morphology of the auditory ERFs in school-age children, as well as lateralization and repetition suppression of ERF components evoked by the clicks. School-age children and adults passively listened to pairs of click presented to the right ear, left ear or binaurally, with 8–11 s intervals between the pairs and a 1 s interval within a pair. Adults demonstrated a typical P50m/N100m response. Unlike adults, children had two distinct components preceding the N100m–P50m (at ~65 ms) and P100m (at ~100 ms). The P100m dominated the child ERF, and was most prominent in response to binaural stimulation. The N100m in children was less developed than in adults and partly overlapped in time with the P100m, especially in response to monaural clicks. Strong repetition suppression was observed for P50m both in children and adults, P100m in children and N100m in adults. Both children and adults demonstrated ERF amplitude and/or latency right hemispheric advantage effects that may reflect right hemisphere dominance for preattentive arousal processes. Our results contribute to the knowledge concerning development of auditory processing and its lateralization in children and have implications for investigation of the auditory evoked fields in developmental disorders.  相似文献   

5.
Responses to tones, clicks, and noise were recorded from chinchilla auditory-nerve fibers (ANFs). The responses to noise were analyzed by computing the zeroth-, first-, and second-order Wiener kernels (h0, h1, and h2). The h1s correctly predicted the frequency tuning and phases of responses to tones of ANFs with low characteristic frequency (CF). The h2s correctly predicted the frequency tuning and phases of responses to tones of all ANFs, regardless of CF. Also regardless of CF, the kernels jointly predicted about 77% of the features of ANF responses to "frozen" samples of noise. Near-CF group delays of kernels and signal-front delays of responses to intense rarefaction clicks exceeded by 1 ms the corresponding basilar-membrane delays at both apical and basal sites of the chinchilla cochlea. This result, confirming that synaptic and neural processes amount to 1 ms regardless of CF, permitted drawing a map of basilar-membrane delay as a function of position for the entire length of the chinchilla cochlea, a first for amniotic species.  相似文献   

6.
To examine the influence of stimulus significance on more sustained as well as transient aspects of electrodermal, cardiac (HR), and eyeblink response, a 21-sec tone was sounded in one ear or the other. A click occurred during many tones, and a light followed offset by 9 sec. Four groups were studied: one pressed a pedal immediately on hearing any click; another only on click during tone in a specified ear; a third also responded only to the specified ear, but withheld press until the light; a fourth listened without any response. Results confirmed the important role of stimulus significance in each system whether between- or within-subject comparisons were made. Sustained responses were seen only when a significant signal was sought, involving in each case sustained HR deceleration, slowed blink rate, and heightened electrodermal level. Transient response to click and light also appeared only when there were significant signals. Response to tone-onset gave more ambiguous results. ANOVAs of response magnitude suggested that onset of nonsignificant tones might have elicited ORs, while binomial tests indicated these were not elicited with better than random frequency anywhere but on those trials occurring more frequently at the experiment's onset. Interpretations consistent with both the significance hypothesis and with a distinction between automatic and voluntary ORs can be made only here. Motor response had no effect on electrodermal or eyeblink response, and on HR was associated only with increased acceleration 1-2 sec after pedal-press. Studies using small motor responses to establish stimulus significance are therefore not likely to be substantially biased by the response itself.  相似文献   

7.
Summary We have studied the effects of masking sounds on auditory evoked magnetic fields (AEFs) of healthy humans. The AEFs were elicited by 25-ms tones presented randomly to the left or to the right ear, and the responses were recorded over the right auditory cortex. Without masking, the 100-ms deflection (N100m) was of somewhat higher amplitude and of shorter latency for contrathan ipsilateral stimuli. Continuous speech, music, or intermittent noise, delivered to the left ear, dampened N100m to stimulation of both ears without correlated changes in sensation. Intermittent noise had a weaker effect on N100m than speech or music. Continuous noise fed to the left ear dampened both the sensation of and the responses to the left-ear stimuli, with no significant effect on the responses to the right-ear stimuli. The results suggest that the masking effects of continuous noise, seen at the auditory cortex, derive mainly from the periphery whereas the effects of sounds with intensity and frequency modulations take place at more central auditory pathways.  相似文献   

8.
Summary Evoked responses from the cochlea and cochlear nucleus in the rat were studied using two types of stimuli: (1) bursts of tones or noise, and (2) continuous tones or noise that were amplitude modulated with pseudorandom noise. While the responses to the first type of stimuli were averaged only in the conventional way, the responses to the continuous and amplitude modulated sounds were averaged over one period of the pseudorandom noise. This average was then cross correlated with one period of the noise. The morphology of these cross correlation functions was in many ways similar to the response to transient sounds.Recordings from the round window of the cochlea and the cochlear nucleus showed that the latencies of these peaks in the responses to tone bursts and those of the cross correlation functions obtained from the continuous tones modulated with pseudorandom noise were similar. However, the latencies of the peaks in the cross correlation functions were slightly shorter and showed less dependency on the stimulus intensity than did the peaks in the responses to tone bursts. When the responses to noise bursts and the responses to noise that was amplitude modulated were compared, it was found that the latencies of the peaks in the cross correlation functions were nearly independent of the stimulus intensity. However, the peaks in the averaged responses to noise bursts showed a decrease in latency with increasing sound intensity.Supported in part by a grant from the Deafness Research Foundation  相似文献   

9.
Perception of the temporal structure of acoustic signals contributes critically to vocal signaling. In the aquatic clawed frog Xenopus laevis, calls differ primarily in the temporal parameter of click rate, which conveys sexual identity and reproductive state. We show here that an ensemble of auditory neurons in the laminar nucleus of the torus semicircularis (TS) of X. laevis specializes in encoding vocalization click rates. We recorded single TS units while pure tones, natural calls, and synthetic clicks were presented directly to the tympanum via a vibration-stimulation probe. Synthesized click rates ranged from 4 to 50 Hz, the rate at which the clicks begin to overlap. Frequency selectivity and temporal processing were characterized using response-intensity curves, temporal-discharge patterns, and autocorrelations of reduplicated responses to click trains. Characteristic frequencies ranged from 140 to 3,250 Hz, with minimum thresholds of -90 dB re 1 mm/s at 500 Hz and -76 dB at 1,100 Hz near the dominant frequency of female clicks. Unlike units in the auditory nerve and dorsal medullary nucleus, most toral units respond selectively to the behaviorally relevant temporal feature of the rate of clicks in calls. The majority of neurons (85%) were selective for click rates, and this selectivity remained unchanged over sound levels 10 to 20 dB above threshold. Selective neurons give phasic, tonic, or adapting responses to tone bursts and click trains. Some algorithms that could compute temporally selective receptive fields are described.  相似文献   

10.
Although many aspects of sensory processing are qualitatively similar in awake and anesthetized subjects, important state-dependent differences are known to exist. To investigate the effects of anesthesia on temporal processing in rat auditory cortex, multi-unit neural responses to trains of broadband clicks were recorded prior to, 15 min following, and 5 h following the administration of a ketamine-based anesthetic. While responses to clicks in isolation were relatively stable between states, responses to subsequent clicks exhibited increases in latency, peak latency, response duration, and post-onset suppression under anesthesia. Ketamine anesthetic reduced the maximum rate at which multi-unit clusters entrained to repeated clicks. No multi-unit clusters entrained to stimulus presentation rates greater than 33 Hz under anesthesia, compared with 85% and 81% in the pre- and post-anesthetic condition, respectively. Anesthesia also induced oscillatory activity that was not present in awake subjects. Finally, ketamine anesthesia abolished all tonic excitatory and suppressive nonsynchronous responses to click trains. The results of this study suggest that ketamine-based anesthesia significantly alters neural coding of broadband click trains in auditory cortex.  相似文献   

11.
Summary The possibility that olfactory input is transmitted to specific subregions of the hippocampal formation via the entorhinal cortex was investigated electrophysiologically by analyzing the laminar profiles of potentials evoked in the hippocampal formation by stimulation of the lateral olfactory tract (LOT). LOT stimulation resulted in long latency (14–20 ms) evoked responses in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampal formation ipsilateral to the stimulation. The variable long latency of these responses and their inability to follow stimulus rates of 40/s suggested that these potentials reflected polysynaptic activation. Analysis of the laminar profiles of the evoked potentials indicated that the responses originated from a synaptic field localized in the outer portion of the stratum moleculare of the dentate gyrus, a terminal distribution which overlaps that of the lateral entorhinal cortical (LEC) projection to the dentate gyrus. Lesions of the LEC eliminated the long latency responses in the dentate gyrus evoked by LOT stimulation. In addition, a conditioning pulse delivered either to the LOT or to the LEC produced paired pulse potentiation of the response elicited by subsequent stimulation of the other structure. No evidence was found to indicate that responses were generated in regio superior of the hippocampus proper following LOT stimulation. Taken together, these results suggest that stimulation of the LOT activates the dentate gyrus of the hippocampal formation by multisynaptic pathways which relay through the lateral portion of the entorhinal area. This finding is discussed with regard to entorhinal cortical organization and the known olfactory projections to the LEC.Some of this material was presented in abstract form at the 7th Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, 1977  相似文献   

12.
The aim of this study was to demonstrate, if possible, vestibulospinal reflex responses in soleus using a stimulus known to be capable of exciting vestibular afferents, namely 100-dB (NHL) clicks. We were able to show short-latency electromyographic (EMG) responses after clicks in five of eight normal subjects, and then we compared these responses with those after transmastoid galvanic stimulation (12 normal subjects). Stimulation of the side towards which the head was rotated (i.e. the side facing backwards) with either clicks or the cathode (anode applied to the opposite side) gave an initial excitatory response in soleus, while click or cathodal stimulation of the opposite side (i.e. the side facing forwards) gave an initial inhibitory response. Onset latencies and modulation with changes in postural task were identical for both click- and galvanic-evoked responses. In addition, there was a significant correlation between the amplitudes of the responses in soleus after click and galvanic stimulation (R 2=0.72). These similarities suggest that the earliest reflex responses in soleus after clicks and galvanic stimulation may be mediated by a common central pathway. In contrast, there was no correlation between the amplitudes of responses evoked by 100-dB clicks in soleus and those evoked by the same stimulus in the sternocleidomastoid. We conclude that vestibular activation by clicks can evoke reflex responses in lower-limb muscles and these responses have similar characteristics to the earliest responses evoked by galvanic vestibular stimulation.  相似文献   

13.
1. Outer hair cells (OHCs) have active micromechanical properties that are thought to be the origin of evoked otoacoustic emissions (EOAEs). In the present study, click-evoked otoacoustic emissions were recorded in humans with or without various contralateral acoustic stimulations. A previous study, concentrating on contralateral stimulation with broadband noise, had shown a decrease of the EOAE amplitude in humans. Results support a role for the efferent system in cochlear mechanics; indeed, medial efferent neurons of the olivocochlear bundle terminate on the OHCs. To obtain a better understanding of the medial efferent system functioning in humans, the present study looked at the contralateral suppressive effect as a function of stimulus parameters. 2. The study of the input-output function of the EOAE amplitude with and without a 50-dB SPL contralateral broadband noise showed that the suppressive effect was equivalent to a mean reduction of 3.77 dB. 3. For the EOAEs to tone pips, the contralateral suppressive effect was strongest when the contralateral ear stimuli were narrow bands that were centered around the central EOAE frequency. This frequency specificity disappeared for contralateral narrow band noise levels greater than 50 dB SPL. 4. The contralateral suppressive effect was also observed with transient contralateral sounds (nonfiltered clicks). Significant reductions of the EOAE amplitude were seen with contralateral click levels as low as 17.5 dB SL. Above this level, the EOAE amplitude decreased as the contralateral stimulus level increased. This effect was still present in subjects without any stapedial reflex, but absent in total unilateral hearing-loss subjects. Therefore this suppressive effect is unlikely to be due to alteration of the middle ear function or to transcranially conducted sound. 5. When the contralateral interclick interval exceeded 14.2 ms. the suppressive effect was smaller. With contralateral stimulus level maintained subjectively constant, the effect was found to disappear when the interclick interval was greater than 49.9 ms. A saturation of the contralateral suppressive effect was observed for click rates greater than 70/s (interclick interval less than 14.2 ms). 6. Our study confirms and specifies the contralateral sound suppression effect on cochlear mechanisms in humans, assessing the equivalent reduction, showing a frequency specificity and extending these findings to contralateral transient sounds. Any influence of the acoustic crosstalk was eliminated. A role played by middle ear muscles cannot be absolutely ruled out but is not necessary to produce such a contralateral suppressive effect (the effect being found in subjects after surgical removal of the stapedius muscle) and could not explain the frequency specificity.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

14.
The auditory evoked magnetic fields to very high frequency tones   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
We studied the auditory evoked magnetic fields (AEFs) in response to pure tones especially at very high frequencies (from 4000 Hz to 40,000 Hz). This is the first systematic study of AEFs using tones above 5000 Hz, the upper audible range of humans, and ultrasound. We performed two experiments. In the first, AEFs were recorded in 12 subjects from both hemispheres under binaural listening conditions. Six types of auditory stimulus (pure tones of five different frequencies: 4000 Hz, 8000 Hz, 10,000 Hz, 12,000 Hz, 14,000 Hz, and a click sound as the target stimulus) were used. In the second experiment, we used 1000 Hz, 15,000 Hz, and two ultrasounds with frequencies of 20,000 Hz and 40,000 Hz. The subjects could detect all stimuli in the first experiment but not the ultrasounds in the second experiment.We analyzed N1m, the main response with approximately 100 ms in peak latency, and made the following findings. (1) N1m responses to the tones up to 12,000 Hz were clearly recorded from at least one hemisphere in all 12 subjects. N1m for 14,000 Hz was identified in at least one hemisphere in 10 subjects, and in both hemispheres in six subjects. No significant response could be identified to ultrasounds over 20,000 Hz. (2) The amplitude of the N1m to the tones above 8000 Hz was significantly smaller than that to 4000 Hz in both hemispheres. There was a tendency for the peak latency of the N1m to be longer for the tones with higher frequencies, but no significant change was found. (3) The equivalent current dipole (ECD) of the N1m was located in the auditory cortex. There was a tendency for the ECD for the tones with higher frequencies to lie in more medial and posterior areas, but no significant change was found. (4) As for the interhemispheric difference, the N1m amplitude for all frequency tones was significantly larger and the ECDs were estimated to be located more anterior and medial in the right hemisphere than the left. The priority of the right hemisphere, that is the larger amplitude, for very high frequency tones was confirmed. (5) The orientation of the ECD in the left hemisphere became significantly more vertical the higher the tones. This result was consistent with previous studies which revealed the sensitivity of the frequency difference in the left hemisphere.From these findings we suggest that tonotopy in the auditory cortex exists up to the upper limit of audible range within the small area, where the directly air-conducted ultrasounds are not reflected.  相似文献   

15.
Summary Magnetic responses evoked by stimulation of the mixed median nerve at the wrist and its cutaneous branches on the glabrous skin of the index and middle fingers were studied. The first responses to mixed nerve stimulation peaked at 19–24 ms, and those to cutaneous nerve stimulation about 4 ms later. The responses, up to a latency of 150 ms, reversed in polarity between the upper and lower parts of the rolandic fissure. Equivalent dipoles for the mixed nerve stimulation were stronger and they lay statistically significantly deeper from the scalp than those activated by the cutaneous nerve stimulation. It is suggested that mixed nerve stimulation activates areas 3a and 3b whereas cutaneous stimulation activates mainly area 3b at the human primary somatosensory cortex. Statistical procedures were developed for comparison of different field patterns and for determining confidence limits of source model parameters. For these purposes the quality and quantity of the noise were studied. The error caused by inaccuracies in the positioning of the magnetometer was found to be minimal in comparison with the signal noise which was estimated from the standard deviation of the averaged response.  相似文献   

16.
Monaural excitatory responses of 181 single units in the central nucleus of the inferior colliculus of 15 anesthetized gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) were examined quantitatively. Pure-tone stimuli were presented monaurally through sealed, calibrated sound-delivery systems. Most units were excited only by contralateral stimulation (EO); 23% were bilaterally excitable (EE). The threshold frequency tuning curves for contralateral stimulation of EE units were significantly broader than those produced by ipsilateral stimulation of EE units and those produced by contralateral stimulation of EO units. The frequency at which threshold was lowest (best frequency), or BF) was very similar for ipsilateral and contralateral stimulation of individual EE units; however, ipsilateral BFs were slightly but significantly lower than contralateral BFs. For EE units, ipsilateral BF thresholds (mean: 29.2 dB SPL) were significantly higher than contralateral BF thresholds (mean: 14.9 dB SPL). Monotonic and nonmonotonic relationships between discharge rate and stimulus intensity at BF were observed in responses evoked both by contralateral and ipsilateral stimulation. Interestingly, for individual EE units it was not uncommon for the rate/intensity function for one monaural condition to be monotonic although the relationship for stimulation of the other ear was markedly nonmonotonic. There was no qualitative difference between rate/intensity functions evoked by contralateral stimulation in EO and EE units. Ipsilateral discharge rates were characteristically much lower than contralateral rates for a given stimulus intensity. For 50 BF tones of 100 ms duration, the median peak numbers of discharges for contralateral stimulation of EO and EE units were 361 and 339, respectively; the median for ipsilateral stimulation of EE units was 102. The dynamic range of each rate/intensity function was calculated by measuring the intensity range associated with an increase in spike count from 10 to 90% of the peak rate. No differences were detected between the distributions of dynamic range for contralateral stimulation in EO or EE units, or between contralateral and ipsilateral dynamic ranges within individual EE units. For all response types the distributions of dynamic range were approximately normal, with means near 20 dB. The minimum mean latency to the first spike at BF was generally longer for ipsilateral than for contralateral responses.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

17.
Using extracellular electrodes we studied acoustic responses in the superior colliculus (SC) of the barbiturate-anesthetized cat. Pure tonal stimuli were delivered through sealed and calibrated earphones and were presented either monaurally or binaurally with interaural intensity differences (IIDs) and interaural time differences (ITDs). Acoustically sensitive cells were found in the intermediate and deep layers of the SC throughout its rostrocaudal and mediolateral extent. Most cells (80%) discharged only at stimulus onset; the rest had more complex firing patterns. For 88% of our sample the mean first-spike latency measured at 20 dB above threshold ranged between 6 and 16 ms. The sharpness and threshold intensity of the frequency tuning curves varied widely. In the SC, the average characteristic frequency and threshold intensity were higher than in other auditory brain stem nuclei. Neurons whose characteristic frequency was low were never sharply tuned. The probability of response decreased when the repetition rate at which the stimuli were delivered increased. The mean stimulus interval at which spike count reached 50% of maximum was 360 ms. Most (83%) of the cells discharged only to monaural stimulation of the contralateral ear, 7% responded to tones applied to either ear and only 1% to only ipsilateral input. The remaining cells responded only to stimulation of both ears. With binaural stimuli, most neurons (80%) could be shown to receive input from both ears. Seventy percent of the binaural cells showed predominant binaural inhibition (BI), 25% binaural facilitation (BF), and 5% a more complex mixture. Because the majority of SC neurons had high characteristic frequencies, we examined their responses to IIDs. The spike count vs. IID functions of BI cells were monotonic and sigmoidal, those of BF cells were nonmonotonic and bell-shaped. The slopes and horizontal positions of the curves varied among neurons. IIDs favoring the contralateral ear were the most effective. For a given cell, increasing the mean binaural level extended the range of IIDs that evoked maximal discharge. A small number of cells was sensitive to physiologically significant interaural time differences of low-frequency tones or the envelopes of amplitude-modulated, high-frequency tones.  相似文献   

18.
Oshurkova E  Scheich H  Brosch M 《Neuroscience》2008,153(4):1289-1299
We studied encoding of temporally modulated sounds in 28 multiunits in the primary auditory cortical field (AI) and in 35 multiunits in the secondary auditory cortical field (caudomedial auditory cortical field, CM) by presenting periodic click trains with click rates between 1 and 300 Hz lasting for 2-4 s. We found that all multiunits increased or decreased their firing rate during the steady state portion of the click train and that all except two multiunits synchronized their firing to individual clicks in the train. Rate increases and synchronized responses were most prevalent and strongest at low click rates, as expressed by best modulation frequency, limiting frequency, percentage of responsive multiunits, and average rate response and vector strength. Synchronized responses occurred up to 100 Hz; rate response occurred up to 300 Hz. Both auditory fields responded similarly to low click rates but differed at click rates above approximately 12 Hz at which more multiunits in AI than in CM exhibited synchronized responses and increased rate responses and more multiunits in CM exhibited decreased rate responses. These findings suggest that the auditory cortex of macaque monkeys encodes temporally modulated sounds similar to the auditory cortex of other mammals. Together with other observations presented in this and other reports, our findings also suggest that AI and CM have largely overlapping sensitivities for acoustic stimulus features but encode these features differently.  相似文献   

19.
The auditory gamma-band transient oscillatory response has been considered to reflect early cognitive processing and attention triggering, as has been suggested of the mismatch negativity (MMN). We examined whether the auditory gamma-band response was related to sensory memory as reflected by MMN. During the electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings, approximately 2000 click sounds were presented to nine healthy adult subjects with constant SOA of 120 or 170 ms in an ignored condition. At a probability of 10%, a click sound was randomly omitted from the stimulus sequence. EEG epochs responding to omitted clicks and to click sounds were averaged for analysis, respectively, and then those were convoluted by Gabor wavelet for the gamma-band response calculation. The MMN to a deviant omission in a sequence of click sounds was elicited with SOA of 120 ms which was shorter than the duration of temporal window of integration, whereas no MMN was elicited with SOA of 170 ms. In contrast with the MMN, the transient gamma-band response clearly commenced after the stimuli but not after the omissions, regardless whether SOA was short or long. The findings indicate that the brain process underlying the transient gamma-band response should be dissociated from the sensory memory function.  相似文献   

20.
The evidence for ERP manifestations of contralateral dominance in the auditory pathways is not entirely compelling. The effect when found has involved either latency or amplitude of a variety of components obtained from averaged or subtraction waveforms. Also, the effect appears to have as much to do with hemisphere as pathway differences. The objectives of this experiment were to establish whether a specific ERP peak reflected contralateral dominance, to assess its long-term stability, and to clarify the apparent pathway-hemisphere interactions which result from monaural stimulation. Ten male subjects were tested on 6 occasions (generally weekly intervals). On each occasion, 76 monaural tones were presented while EEG was recorded at C2, P2, T3, and T4 referred to linked earlobes. P70, N120, and P200 peak amplitudes and latencies and N1–P2 amplitudes were examined. N120 amplitudes proved to be larger at the temporal site contralateral to the ear stimulated. This contralateral dominance effect was observed on all 6 occasions for right ear stimuli and on all but one occasion for left ear stimuli. N1–P2 amplitudes less reliably reflected contralateral dominance because possible hemisphere effects appeared with this measure. Methodological issues related to assessment of ERP contralateral dominance and to the variety of effects seen in the literature are discussed. The data are examined with reference to neurophysiological substrates thought to underlie auditory ERPs. Finally, the utility of a stable ERP phenomenon in assessing drug or pathology effects is evaluated.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号