首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   7085篇
  免费   258篇
  国内免费   49篇
耳鼻咽喉   24篇
儿科学   215篇
妇产科学   21篇
基础医学   1965篇
口腔科学   30篇
临床医学   701篇
内科学   192篇
皮肤病学   1篇
神经病学   2367篇
特种医学   224篇
外科学   411篇
综合类   325篇
一般理论   1篇
预防医学   265篇
眼科学   20篇
药学   484篇
  1篇
中国医学   138篇
肿瘤学   7篇
  2023年   52篇
  2022年   132篇
  2021年   246篇
  2020年   228篇
  2019年   191篇
  2018年   213篇
  2017年   184篇
  2016年   156篇
  2015年   143篇
  2014年   374篇
  2013年   353篇
  2012年   293篇
  2011年   357篇
  2010年   257篇
  2009年   324篇
  2008年   347篇
  2007年   341篇
  2006年   306篇
  2005年   268篇
  2004年   260篇
  2003年   182篇
  2002年   164篇
  2001年   155篇
  2000年   198篇
  1999年   155篇
  1998年   110篇
  1997年   104篇
  1996年   91篇
  1995年   97篇
  1994年   79篇
  1993年   69篇
  1992年   79篇
  1991年   79篇
  1990年   55篇
  1989年   56篇
  1988年   64篇
  1987年   67篇
  1986年   45篇
  1985年   63篇
  1984年   57篇
  1983年   33篇
  1982年   42篇
  1981年   31篇
  1980年   37篇
  1979年   47篇
  1978年   39篇
  1977年   29篇
  1976年   28篇
  1973年   24篇
  1972年   21篇
排序方式: 共有7392条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
In this study the organization of information processing in a selective search task was examined by analyzing event-related potentials. This task consisted of searching for target letters in a relevant (attended) color. The ERPs revealed two different effects of attention: an early occipital negativity (+/- 150 ms) reflecting feature-specific attention, and a later, central N2b component (+/- 240 ms) reflecting covert orienting of attention. A later, prolonged negativity (search-related negativity) (+/- 300 ms), maximal at Cz, was related to controlled search to letters in the attended color. Detection of relevant targets resulted in a parietal P3b component. Depending on stimulus presentation conditions an earlier response to both attended and unattended targets was found: an N2 component (+/- 250 ms). In these same conditions, C'3-C'4 asymmetries (Corrected Motor Asymmetries--CMA) suggested motor activation at +/- 300 ms, in the same time range as search-related negativity. It was argued that N2 and CMA suggest the existence of a preattentive target detection system, operating in parallel with a slower serial attentive system, as reflected by N2b and search negativity.  相似文献   
52.
Summary The aim of the study was to investigate the contribution of the primary sensory cortex in the compensation of cerebellar deficits during self-paced movements. For this purpose, monkeys were trained on motor tasks which required goal-reaching and independent finger movements. The intermediate and lateral deep cerebellar nuclei and the sensory cortex were lesioned in isolation and in sequence and the course of motor recovery was studied on the test performances. The deep nuclei were lesioned by kainic acid injections, the sensory cortex was removed by ablation. Cerebellar lesions in isolation produced obvious deficits at proximal and distal joints, affecting both slow and fast motor adjustments. Only lesions of the anterior portions of the intermediate and lateral deep nuclear complexes produced deficiencies in voluntary movements. Lesions of the posterior portions produced postural disturbances. The process of recovery following cerebellar lesions was slow and, depending on the nature of the task, was found to be differentially disruptive for motor performances requiring fast and slow motor adjustments. The deficits at distal joints appeared to be more enduring than those at proximal joints. Sensory cortical lesions in isolation produced much less severe and more transient motor deficits. They consisted of hand clumsiness and their recovery was fast and reached higher levels of performance than following cerebellar lesions. When the sensory cortex was removed secondarily to a cerebellar lesion and after recovery from the cerebellar deficits, the initially recovered motor performance became much worse again (decompensation). Removal of the sensory cortex prior to a cerebellar lesion exaggerated the cerebellar deficits and severely limited their recovery. Slow and fast motor performances were completely abolished for three weeks following sequential lesions. Signs of recovery subsequently appeared and stabilized at low levels of performance by five to seven weeks. The effects of combined, sequential cerebellar and sensory cortical lesions were much worse than expected if the effects from the two lesions were merely additive. This indicates that there is some functional interrelationship between the sensory cortex and the cerebellum, which promotes compensation. The somatosensory cortex appears to play a crucial role in the process of recovery from cerebellar motor deficits and it is likely that sensation is an important component in the process of recovery. It is suggested that the sensory cortex exerts its compensatory actions via a structure or structures which receives convergent cerebellar and sensory cortical inputs.  相似文献   
53.
Understanding the physiological and psychological factors that contribute to healthy and pathological balance control in man has been made difficult by the confounding effects of the perturbations used to test balance reactions. The present study examined how postural responses were influenced by the acceleration–deceleration interval of an unexpected horizontal translation. Twelve adult males maintained balance during unexpected forward and backward surface translations with two different acceleration–deceleration intervals and presentation orders (serial or random). “SHORT” perturbations consisted of an initial acceleration (peak acceleration 1.3 m s−2; duration 300 ms) followed 100 ms later by a deceleration. “LONG” perturbations had the same acceleration as SHORT perturbations, followed by a 2-s interval of constant velocity before deceleration. Surface and intra-muscular electromyography (EMG) from the leg, trunk, and shoulder muscles were recorded along with motion and force plate data. LONG perturbations induced larger trunk displacements compared to SHORT perturbations when presented randomly and larger EMG responses in proximal and distal muscles during later (500–800 ms) response intervals. During SHORT perturbations, activity in some antagonist muscles was found to be associated with deceleration and not the initial acceleration of the support surface. When predictable, SHORT perturbations facilitated the use of anticipatory mechanisms to attenuate early (100–400 ms) EMG response amplitudes, ankle torque change and trunk displacement. In contrast, LONG perturbations, without an early deceleration effect, did not facilitate anticipatory changes when presented in a predictable order. Therefore, perturbations with a short acceleration–deceleration interval can influence triggered postural responses through reactive effects and, when predictable with repeated exposure, through anticipatory mechanisms.  相似文献   
54.
Nine young infants were followed longitudinally from 4 to 15 months of age. We recorded early spontaneous movements and reaching movements to a stationary target. Time-position data of the hand (endpoint), shoulder, and elbow were collected using an optoelectronic measurement system (ELITE). We analyzed the endpoint kinematics and the intersegmental dynamics of the shoulder and elbow joint to investigate how changes in proximal torque control determined the development of hand trajectory formation. Two developmental phases of hand trajectory formation were identified: a first phase of rapid improvements between 16 and 24 weeks of age, the time of reaching onset for all infants. During that time period the number of movement units per reach and movement time decreased dramatically. In a second phase (28–64 weeks), a period of fine-tuning of the sensorimotor system, we saw slower, more gradual changes in the endpoint kinematics. The analysis of the underlying intersegmental joint torques revealed the following results: first, the range of muscular and motiondependent torques (relative to body weight) did not change significantly with age. That is, early reaching was not confined by limitations in producing task-adequate levels of muscular torque. Second, improvements in the endpoint kinematics were not accomplished by minimizing amplitude of muscle and reactive torques. Third, the relative timing of muscular and motion-dependent torque peaks showed a systematic development toward an adult timing profile with increasing age. In conclusion, the development toward invariant characteristics of the hand trajectory is mirrored by concurrent changes in the control of joint forces. The acquisition of stable patterns of intersegmental coordination is not achieved by simply regulating force amplitude, but more so by modulating the correct timing of joint force production and by the system's use of reactive forces. Our findings support the view that development of reaching is a process of unsupervised learning with no external or innate teacher prescribing the desired kinematics or kinetics of the movement.  相似文献   
55.
Summary This study was undertaken in order to determine the time course of the process by which information derived from a visual target is used to accurately set the amplitude of a simple motor response. We refer to this process as response specification. Separate auditory and visual cues were given to the subjects in order to independently control the moment of response initiation and the time available for processing amplitude information from the target. Six subjects initiated impulses of isometric force in synchrony with the last of predictable series of regular tones. Response amplitudes were to match one of three visual target steps occurring at random times between 0 and 400 ms before the response-synchronizing tone. Using these separate auditory and visual cues, we were able to systematically vary the time interval between target presentation and response onset, termed here Stimulus-Response or S-R interval. Target steps were presented in blocks of either predictable (simple condition) or unpredictable (choice condition) amplitudes. The peak forces and the peaks of their time derivatives were analyzed to determine how subjects achieved accuracy under the different conditions and at different S-R intervals. The trajectories of responses produced in the simple condition were independent of the S-R interval. In contrast, when targets were presented in unpredictable order, the distribution of the peak forces of the subjects' responses depended on the S-R interval. At short S-R intervals (<125 ms), subjects made responses whose peak forces were distributed around the center of the range of target steps. These responses formed a unimodal, but broad distribution which was independent of actual target amplitude. With increasing S-R interval (>125 ms), the distributions of peak forces gradually shifted toward the correct target amplitudes, with the means reaching the appropriate amplitudes at S-R intervals of 250–400 ms. At S-R intervals comparable to a reaction time, the range of peak forces was constricted to a similar extent as previously observed in a reaction time task (Hening et al. 1988). We found that the gradual improvement of accuracy was not achieved through changes in trajectory control: at all S-R intervals, subjects utilized a pulse-height control policy (Gordon and Ghez 1987a). Different peak forces were achieved by varying the rate of rise of force, while force rise time was held relatively invariant. We did find, however, that within the constraints imposed by rise time regulation, compensatory adjustments to the force trajectories (Gordon and Ghez 1987b) were greatest during the period of specification. We conclude that (1) subjects can initiate their responses independent of the degree of specification achieved and that the normal process of specification of amplitude begins earlier and continues longer than the latency of responses in a reaction time task; (2) before target presentation, subjects prepare a default response whose amplitude is biased by prior experience with the targets presented in the task. We hypothesize that the central mechanisms that specify response amplitude do so by a progressive adjustment of the default parameters.  相似文献   
56.
Summary Corticomotor (CM) neurones were identified in three conscious macaque monkeys by the presence of post-spike facilitation (PSF) in spike-triggered averages of e.m.g. recorded from intrinsic hand and forearm muscles during performance of a precision grip task. Post-spike effects were compared with those produced by single-pulse intracortical microstimulation (ICMS), with strengths of 4–20 A, delivered at the site of 47 CM cells. Most muscles facilitated by a CM cell were also facilitated by ICMS at the site of the cell. ICMS effects were stronger: at 10 A, the amplitude of ICMS-evoked facilitation was on average 2.8 times greater than PSF, and 6.9 times greater at 20 A. Onset latency of ICMS-evoked facilitation was consistently longer (by 1.7 and 1.3 ms at 10 and 20 A respectively) than PSF, and it is suggested that this results from the indirect, trans-synaptic excitation of CM cells by ICMS. Post-spike suppression was rarely seen (7/421 compared to 105/421 cases of PSF). In contrast, suppression and facilitation were equally common in response to ICMS. The synaptic mechanisms underlying these effects were explored in 5 anaesthetised macaque monkeys. ICMS facilitated a greater proportion of the tested muscles than did the CM cell recorded at the stimulus site. The results suggest the juxtaposition in the motor cortex of CM neurones with different muscle fields. The merits of STA and ICMS for exploring cortical organisation are discussed.  相似文献   
57.
Summary The corticospinal projections from areas 4 and 6 were investigated in the raccoon using the horseradish peroxidase (HRP) retrograde tracing technique. Multiple injections of lectin bound HRP and HRP were made into either the cervical or lumbar cord in 7 anesthetized raccoons. Retrogradely labeled neurons were observed throughout a wide extent of areas 4 and 6a. The HRP positive cells were most numerous within the dorsal bank of the cruciate sulcus within area 4 and continued around the fundus to occupy the lateral two-thirds of the ventral bank of the cruciate sulcus within area 6a. No labeled cells were observed in the more medially located area 6a. Although the HRP positive cells observed following the lumbar cord injections were situated slightly more medial and caudal to those observed following the cervical cord injections, considerable overlap between the two projections was noted. The corticospinal projections arising from areas 4 and 6a in the raccoon largely correspond in location to the regions functionally defined as the primary motor cortex and the supplementary motor area, respectively.  相似文献   
58.
Recent studies have shown that the hand-pointing movements within arm's reach remain invariant whether the trunk is recruited or not or its motion is unexpectedly prevented. This suggests the presence of compensatory arm-trunk coordination minimizing the deflections of the hand from the intended trajectory. It has been postulated that vestibular signals elicited by the trunk motion and transmitted to the arm motor system play a major role in the compensation. One prediction of this hypothesis is that vestibular stimulation should influence arm posture and movement during reaching. It has been demonstrated that galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) can influence the direction of pointing movements when body motion is restrained. In the present study, we analyzed the effects of GVS on trunk-assisted pointing movements. Subjects either moved the hand to a target or maintained a steady-state posture near the target, while moving the trunk forward with the eyes closed. When GVS was applied, the final position of the hand was deviated in the lateral and sagittal direction in both tasks. This was the result of two independent effects: a deviation of the trunk trajectory and a modification of the arm position relative to the trunk. Thus, the vestibular system might be directly involved not only in the control of trunk motion but also in the arm-trunk coordination during trunk-assisted reaching movements. Electronic Publication  相似文献   
59.
We conducted a longitudinal study about daily variation of Wistar male rats' behavior in the elevated plus-maze (EPM) evaluated in the 1st, 2nd, 3rd, 6th, 12th, and 18th months of life. Animals were submitted to the plus-maze in 12 sessions at 2-h intervals (n=72, 6 per time point). Spontaneous rest-activity rhythm of four animals was assessed by observation of 24-h videotape records. Time series were analyzed by Cosinor method. Behavioral rates on the six occasions and in light and dark phases were compared by means of two-way ANOVA with repeated measures. Exploratory behavior in EPM was smaller in the light phase and in older animals. Higher values of open and closed arms exploration were observed in the first and third months of the dark phase, and in the first month of the light phase. Adjustment to the 24-h period was significant at all stages for rest-activity data, number of entries in closed arms, and time on center, and for three to five stages for open-arm exploration. In general, 24 h variability was more pronounced in younger animals compared with older ones. The present study showed that: (1). a significant amount of total variability of the behavioral indexes analyzed could be attributed to 24 h variation, (2). light/dark phases differences in EPM exploration were present at all developmental stages, (3). older Wistar rats explored less the EPM and were less active in their home cage compared with younger ones, and (4). behavioral indexes (EPM) decrease was phase related and partially related to a reorganization of rest-activity rhythm.  相似文献   
60.
Summary Two rhesus monkeys were trained to move a handle on a two-dimensional (2D) working surface in directions specified by a light at the plane. They first captured with the handle a light on the center of the plane and then moved the handle in the direction indicated by a peripheral light (cue signal). The signal to move (go signal) was given by turning off the center light. The following tasks were used: (a) In the non-delay task the peripheral light was turned on at the same time as the center light went off. (b) In the memorized delay task the peripheral light stayed on for 300 ms and the center light was turned off 450–750 ms later. Finally, (c) in the non-memorized delay task the peripheral light stayed on continuously whereas the center light went off 750–1050 ms after the peripheral light came on. Recordings in the arm area of the motor cortex (N= 171 cells) showed changes in single cell activity in all tasks. In both delay tasks, the neuronal population vector calculated every 20 ms after the onset of the peripheral light pointed in the direction of the upcoming movement, which was instructed by the cue light. Moreover, the strength of the population signal showed an initial peak shortly after the cue onset in both the memorized and non-memorized delay tasks but it maintained a higher level during the memorized delay period, as compared to the non-memorized task. These results indicate that the motor cortex is involved in encoding and holding in memory directional information concerning a visually cued arm movement and that these processes can be visualized using neuronal population vector analysis.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号