首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   997篇
  免费   228篇
  国内免费   38篇
耳鼻咽喉   2篇
儿科学   9篇
妇产科学   1篇
基础医学   122篇
口腔科学   5篇
临床医学   81篇
内科学   91篇
皮肤病学   5篇
神经病学   467篇
特种医学   57篇
外科学   34篇
综合类   194篇
预防医学   78篇
眼科学   5篇
药学   44篇
中国医学   64篇
肿瘤学   4篇
  2024年   16篇
  2023年   43篇
  2022年   42篇
  2021年   97篇
  2020年   59篇
  2019年   74篇
  2018年   60篇
  2017年   66篇
  2016年   59篇
  2015年   70篇
  2014年   78篇
  2013年   82篇
  2012年   57篇
  2011年   39篇
  2010年   41篇
  2009年   35篇
  2008年   46篇
  2007年   42篇
  2006年   38篇
  2005年   38篇
  2004年   33篇
  2003年   29篇
  2002年   22篇
  2001年   16篇
  2000年   16篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   7篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   3篇
  1978年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1263条查询结果,搜索用时 562 毫秒
1.
目的研究和开发支持中医和现代生物医学本体和术语集的语义标注系统。方法以MedPortal本体库和中医临床术语集等为术语资源库,设计语义标注系统工作流程和功能框架,并开发Web应用系统。结果构建了一个基于Web的中医药文献语义标注系统,支持语料库管理与维护、术语词典管理、语义标注和语义检索等功能,既可以为基于机器学习的信息抽取算法研究提供训练集,又能实现语义层面的多来源数据集成与知识融合。结论该中医药文献语义标注系统设计方案已经过实际项目验证,可为其他同类系统研发提供参考。  相似文献   
2.
3.
4.
Fluency deficits have been found in different neuropsychological populations, and dissociation of letter initial and category fluency has been demonstrated in right-brain-damaged and Alzheimer patients. Studies on normal controls also indicate that letter initial and category fluency are supported by different brain regions. We studied letter initial and category fluency in 79 left-brain-damaged patients, 18 non-aphasic and 61 aphasic. Our results indicate that brain-damaged patients are significantly impaired in both fluency tasks compared to normal controls, and a difference between letter initial and category fluency emerges from our data. Firstly, only letter initial fluency is more impaired in aphasic than in non-aphasic patients; secondly, many aphasics have normal category fluency and pathological letter initial fluency but the reverse is seldom true and thirdly, only category fluency scores correlate with confrontation-naming scores. It is suggested that strategies to recover words based on a semantic or a phonological prompt are different. A semantic strategy is coherent with the organizational structure of the semantic system and corresponds to the normal way in which we recover words whereas a phonological strategy is quite unusual and it is not used for actual communication.  相似文献   
5.
Three representations of the conditional independences due toMendelian segregation of genes in a pedigree are proposed. Thecomputational costs of performing calculations using the techniqueof peeling with each of these representations is compared byconsidering the weights of triangulations of the graph producedby each representation.  相似文献   
6.
Memory function in normal aging   总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4  
Basic findings obtained on memory functions in normal aging are presented and discussed with respect to five separate but interacting memory systems. These systems are: episodic memory, semantic memory, short-term memory, perceptual representation system and procedural memory. All available evidence from cross-sectional research shows that there is a linear, decreasing memory performance as a function of age for episodic memory. Longitudinal studies suggest, however, that this age deficit may be an overestimation, by showing a relatively stable performance level up to middle age, followed by a sharp decline. Studies on semantic memory, short-term memory, perceptual representation system, and procedural memory show a relatively constant performance level across the adult life span, although some tasks used to assess short-term memory and procedural memory have revealed an age deficit. Disregarding the mixed results for these latter two memory systems, it can be concluded that episodic memory is unique in showing an age deficit. Episodic memory is also unique in the sense that it is the only memory system showing gender differences in performance throughout the adult life span with a significantly higher performance for women.  相似文献   
7.
Graph algorithms previously developed by the authors are adapted to simulate pedigrees similar to those used in genetic linkage studies which associate disease phenotypes with specific genomic locations. Pedigrees are chosen uniformly at random from the set of those with specified numbers of individuals and matings and which contain no loops. Summary statistics from pedigrees generated in this way can be used to check real pedigrees for anomalies due to biased sampling or phenotypic effects on the pedigree structure.  相似文献   
8.
目的:探讨不同时间睡眠剥夺(sleep deprivation,SD)对内隐记忆的影响。方法:将32名青年男性随机分为4组:对照组、SD21、SD45和SD69组,每组8名。采用补笔测验和组词测验对4组被试进行测试。结果:SD 后无论知觉启动还是语义启动,启动量降低,并随SD 时间延长而减少。同一组内,两种测验进行比较,除对照组外,其他SD 组两两比较,语义启动的启动量大于知觉启动(P<0.05)。知觉启动中,SD45同SD69 相比无显著差异(P=0.245),其他两两比较差异均有统计学意义(P<0.01);语义启动中,SD21同对照组相比差异无统计学意义(P=0.316),其他两两比较差异均有统计学意义(P<0.01)。结论:SD 后内隐记忆受损,并同SD 时间有关;SD 后语义启动和知觉启动出现分离,知觉启动更受SD 影响。  相似文献   
9.
ERPs were elicited by two types of orthographically legal, pronounceable nonwords. One nonword set was derived from and resembled real words, whereas the other set did not. Nonwords derived from related root words elicited N400 semantic priming effects similar to those obtained for words, indicating semantic activation of the root words. N400 repetition priming effects from nonderived nonwords were similar to those obtained for words. The elicitation of N400 by only derived nonwords would have suggested it was generated by the activation of word meanings, per se. However, both types of nonwords produced an N400, and an N400 priming effect. Because nonderived nonwords are not associated with meaning, the N400 cannot be generated by semantic activation per se. Rather, the N400 appears to be generated by orthographic/phonological analysis and is attenuated by the top-down feed of semantic information to the orthographic/phonological level.  相似文献   
10.
Visual evoked potentials (VEPs) elicited by foveal presentation of words were compared to brain potentials evoked by the same words in a condition where subjects had to make a saccadic eye movement in order to perceive the words (saccade-related brain potentials, SRPs). Subjects had to categorize the words responding with a button press to stimuli belonging to the target (infrequent, P = 0.2) category. The VEP and SRP waveforms showed divergences in the early (up to 250 ms) components, but a marked similarity between the late components. Principal Component Analysis also revealed the same relationship between the two types of brain responses. Peak latency of the late SRP components measured from saccade offset showed an apparent processing advantage over the corresponding late components of VEPs. The N3 component, indexing semantic processing of visual patterns, peaked between 310 and 375 ms in the SRPs, while in the VEPs it appeared between 410 and 470 ms. The P4 component, associated with final stimulus evaluation, showed a similar latency benefit in favour of SRPs (420-500 ms vs 530-590 ms in VEPs). The mean reaction time was 74 ms shorter in the eye movement condition (measured from saccade offset) than in the VEP condition (703 vs 777 ms). The question of what kind of processes may contribute to the differences in mean RTs and to the latencies of the late components between the two conditions are discussed. We suggest that the late components (P3, N3 and P4) of the VEP and the SRP, respectively, index identical brain processes.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号