首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到10条相似文献,搜索用时 140 毫秒
1.
Spinal rats exposed to an instrumental avoidance routine in a counterbalanced Horridge paradigm were able to achieve successively higher criteria. Both experimental and yoked control animals were capable of instrumental avoidance conditioning to incremental criteria; experimental animals exhibited retention of the task when tested. During acquisition, naive experimental animals were superior in performance to previous control animals. Due to the use of a counterbalanced Horridge paradigm, the effects of sensitization and response variability are probably not sufficient to explain all of the results of this experiment. The data suggest that both graded acquisition and retention occur at the spinal level.  相似文献   

2.
Prior work has demonstrated that spinal cord neurons, isolated from the brain through a spinal transection, can support learning. Spinally transected rats given legshock whenever one hindlimb is extended learn to maintain the shocked leg in a flexed position, minimizing net shock exposure. This capacity for learning is inhibited by prior exposure to an uncontrollable stimulus (e.g., intermittent tailshock). The present experiments examined whether spinal cord neurons are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of uncontrollable stimulation after spinal cord injury. Experiment 1 confirmed that uncontrollable shock inhibits subsequent learning in transected rats. Rats that received uncontrollable stimulation prior to transection did not exhibit this effect, suggesting that brain systems exert a protective effect. Experiment 2 showed that this protective effect was removed if subjects received a dorsolateral funiculus lesion prior to shock exposure. Subsequent experiments were designed to determine the identity of the neurochemical systems that protect spinal plasticity. Intrathecal application of serotonin (5-HT) or a 5-HT 1A/7 agonist (8-OH DPAT) in transected rats had a protective effect that blocked the adverse effect of uncontrollable stimulation (Experiment 3). The alpha-2 noradrenergic agonist, clonidine, also protected plasticity (Experiment 4), but this effect was linked to cross-reactivity at the 5-HT 1A receptor (Experiment 5). Microinjection of a 5HT 1A antagonist (WAY 100635) into the spinal cord before intact rats received uncontrollable stimulation blocked the brain-dependent protection of spinal cord neurons. The findings indicate that serotonergic systems normally protect spinal cord plasticity from the deleterious effects of uncontrollable stimulation.  相似文献   

3.
The effect of two chronic motor training paradigms on the ability of the lumbar spinal cord to perform an acute instrumental learning task was examined in neonatally (postnatal day 5; P5) spinal cord transected (i.e., spinal) rats. At approximately P30, rats began either unipedal hindlimb stand training (Stand-Tr; 20-25min/day, 5days/week), or bipedal hindlimb step training (Step-Tr; 20min/day; 5days/week) for 7 weeks. Non-trained spinal rats (Non-Tr) served as controls. After 7 weeks all groups were tested on the flexor-biased instrumental learning paradigm. We hypothesized that (1) Step-Tr rats would exhibit an increased capacity to learn the flexor-biased task relative to Non-Tr subjects, as locomotion involves repetitive training of the tibialis anterior (TA), the ankle flexor whose activation is important for successful instrumental learning, and (2) Stand-Tr rats would exhibit a deficit in acute motor learning, as unipedal training activates the ipsilateral ankle extensors, but not flexors. Results showed no differences in acute learning potential between Non-Tr and Step-Tr rats, while the Stand-Tr group showed a reduced capacity to learn the acute task. Further investigation of the Stand-Tr group showed that, while both the ipsilateral and contralateral hindlimbs were significantly impaired in their acute learning potential, the contralateral, untrained hindlimbs exhibited significantly greater learning deficits. These results suggest that different types of chronic peripheral input may have a significant impact on the ability to learn a novel motor task, and demonstrate the potential for experience-dependent plasticity in the spinal cord in the absence of supraspinal connectivity.  相似文献   

4.
Previous studies have shown that spinal neurons are capable of supporting a form of instrumental conditioning. Subjects receiving a spinal transection will learn to maintain a flexion response after exposure to shock contingent on leg position. In contrast, subjects receiving shock irrespective of leg position will not show increased flexion duration. Activation of the immune system has deleterious effects on learning in intact animals, but the impact of immune system activation on learning spinal animals is not known. We found that a large dose of i.p. LPS (1.0mg/kg) significantly disrupted the acquisition of the instrumental flexion response. The LPS-induced learning deficit was not prevented by preexposure to contingent shock (i.e. immunization) (Experiment 2). Co-administration of the iNOS inhibitor L-NIL (0.1, 1.0 and 10.0 microg/microL) failed to block the deficit (Experiment 3). Co-administration of an IL-1 receptor antagonist (r-metHuIL-1ra [10.0, 30.0 and 100.0 microg/microL) prevented the LPS-induced learning deficit when given in a dose of 100.0 microg/microL(i.t.) only (Experiment 4). Findings indicate a role for spinal IL-1 in the decreased plasticity following LPS administration.  相似文献   

5.
The long-standing belief that the spinal cord serves merely as a conduit for information traveling to and from the brain is changing. Over the past decade, research has shown that the spinal cord is sensitive to response–outcome contingencies, demonstrating that spinal circuits have the capacity to modify behavior in response to differential environmental cues. If spinally transected rats are administered shock contingent on leg extension (controllable shock), they will maintain a flexion response that minimizes shock exposure. If, however, this contingency is broken, and shock is administered irrespective of limb position (uncontrollable shock), subjects cannot acquire the same flexion response. Interestingly, each of these treatments has a lasting effect on behavior; controllable shock enables future learning, while uncontrollable shock produces a long-lasting learning deficit. Here we suggest that the mechanisms underlying learning and the deficit may have evolved from machinery responsible for the spinal processing of noxious information. Experiments have shown that learning and the deficit require receptors and signaling cascades shown to be involved in central sensitization, including activation of NMDA and neurokinin receptors, as well as CaMKII. Further supporting this link between pain and learning, research has also shown that uncontrollable stimulation results in allodynia. Moreover, systemic inflammation and neonatal hindpaw injury each facilitate pain responding and undermine the ability of the spinal cord to support learning. These results suggest that the plasticity associated with learning and pain must be placed in a balance in order for adaptive outcomes to be observed.  相似文献   

6.
Previous research has shown that spinally transected rats will learn to maintain a flexion response when administered shock contingent upon leg position. In short, a contingency is arranged between shock delivery and leg extension so that Master rats exhibit an increase in flexion duration that lasts throughout the training session. Furthermore, when Master rats are later tested they reacquire the flexion response in fewer trials, indicative of some savings. As a control, a second group of spinal rats (Yoked rats) are given shock irrespective of leg position (noncontingent shock). These animals fail to show the same increase in leg flexion duration. Interestingly, when Yoked rats are later tested with a shock contingency in place, they still fail to learn (learning deficit). The present experiments were designed to determine whether both forms of instrumental learning in spinal animals require de novo protein synthesis. As such, we administered various doses of anisomycin intrathecally prior to training. Additionally, spinal rats were trained and tested either immediately or 24 h after test. We found that only the highest dose of anisomycin (125 microg/microl) had an effect in Yoked animals that were tested 24 h after training. Specifically, the highest dose of anisomycin reversed the learning deficit in those animals. Moreover, anisomycin had a similar effect when administered prior to training and immediately following training, but not 6 h after training. Finally, the results demonstrated that the observed effect of anisomycin was not due to state-dependency.  相似文献   

7.
The role of spinal cord plasticity in motor learning is largely unknown. This study explored the effects of H-reflex operant conditioning, a simple model of motor learning, on GABAergic input to spinal motoneurons in rats. Soleus motoneurons were labeled by retrograde transport of a fluorescent tracer and GABAergic terminals on them were identified by glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD)67 immunoreactivity. Three groups were studied: (i) rats in which down-conditioning had reduced the H-reflex (successful HRdown rats); (ii) rats in which down-conditioning had not reduced the H-reflex (unsuccessful HRdown rats) and (iii) unconditioned (naive) rats. The number, size and GAD density of GABAergic terminals, and their coverage of the motoneuron, were significantly greater in successful HRdown rats than in unsuccessful HRdown or naive rats. It is likely that these differences are due to modifications in terminals from spinal interneurons in lamina VI-VII and that the increased terminal number, size, GAD density and coverage in successful HRdown rats reflect and convey a corticospinal tract influence that changes motoneuron firing threshold and thereby decreases the H-reflex. GABAergic terminals in spinal cord change after spinal cord transection. The present results demonstrate that such spinal cord plasticity also occurs in intact rats in the course of motor learning and suggest that this plasticity contributes to skill acquisition.  相似文献   

8.
Recent research indicates that glial cells control complex functions within the nervous system. For example, it has been shown that glial cells contribute to the development of pathological pain, the process of long-term potentiation, and the formation of memories. These data suggest that glial cell activation exerts both adaptive and pathological effects within the CNS. To extend this line of work, the present study investigated the role of glia in spinal learning and spinal learning deficits using the spinal instrumental learning paradigm. In this paradigm rats are transected at the second thoracic vertebra (T2) and given shock to one hind limb whenever the limb is extended (controllable shock). Over time these subjects exhibit an increase in flexion duration that reduces net shock exposure. However, when spinalized rats are exposed to uncontrollable shock or inflammatory stimuli prior to testing with controllable shock, they exhibit a learning deficit. To examine the role of glial in this paradigm, spinal glial cells were pharmacologically inhibited through the use of fluorocitrate. Our results indicate that glia are involved in the acquisition, but not maintenance, of spinal learning. Furthermore, the data indicate that glial cells are involved in the development of both shock and inflammation-induced learning deficits. These findings are consistent with prior research indicating that glial cells are involved in both adaptive and pathological processes within the spinal cord.  相似文献   

9.
To study plasticity of central autonomic circuits that develops after spinal cord injury (SCI), we have characterized a mouse model of autonomic dysreflexia. Autonomic dysreflexia is a condition in which episodic hypertension occurs after injuries above the midthoracic segments of the spinal cord. As synaptic plasticity may be triggered by axonal degeneration, we investigated whether autonomic dysreflexia is reduced in mice when axonal degeneration is delayed after SCI. We subjected three strains of mice, Wld(S), C57BL, and 129Sv, to either spinal cord transection (SCT) or severe clip-compression injury (CCI). The Wld(S) mouse is a well-characterized mutant that exhibits delayed Wallerian degeneration. The CCI model is an injury paradigm in which significant the axonal degeneration is due to secondary events and therefore delayed relative to the time of the initial injury. We herein demonstrate that the incidence of autonomic dysreflexia is reduced in Wld(S) mice after SCT and in all mice after CCI. To determine if differences in afferent arbor sprouting could explain our observations, we assessed changes in the afferent arbor in each mouse strain after both SCT and CCI. We show that independent of the type of injury, 129Sv mice but not C57BL or Wld(S) mice demonstrated an increased small-diameter CGRP-immunoreactive afferent arbor after SCI. Our work thus suggests a role for Wallerian degeneration in the development of autonomic dysreflexia and demonstrates that the choice of mouse strain and injury model has important consequences to the generalizations that may be drawn from studies of SCI in mice.  相似文献   

10.
《Clinical neurophysiology》2020,131(7):1519-1532
ObjectiveThe importance of subcortical pathways to functional motor recovery after spinal cord injury (SCI) has been demonstrated in multiple animal models. The current study evaluated descending interlimb influence on lumbosacral motor excitability after chronic SCI in humans.MethodsUlnar nerve stimulation and transcutaneous electrical spinal stimulation were used in a condition-test paradigm to evaluate the presence of interlimb connections linking the cervical and lumbosacral spinal segments in non-injured (n=15) and spinal cord injured (SCI) (n=18) participants.ResultsPotentiation of spinally evoked motor responses (sEMRs) by ulnar nerve conditioning was observed in 7/7 SCI participants with volitional leg muscle activation, and in 6/11 SCI participants with no volitional activation. Of these six, conditioning of sEMRs was present only when the neurological level of injury was rostral to the ulnar innervation entry zones.ConclusionsDescending modulation of lumbosacral motor pools via interlimb projections may exist in SCI participants despite the absence of volitional leg muscle activation.SignificanceEvaluation of sub-clinical, spared pathways within the spinal cord after SCI may provide an improved understanding of both the contributions of different pathways to residual function, and the mechanisms of plasticity and functional motor recovery following rehabilitation..  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号