首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 62 毫秒
1.
The effect of hippocampal aspiration lesions on trace eyeblink conditioning was examined in young, freely-moving F1 hybrid rats (Fisher 344 x Brown Norway). Rats which received either bilateral neocortical or bilateral hippocampal aspiration lesions were compared with each other or with sham lesioned control rats. The rats were trained with a 250 ms tone conditioning stimulus (CS), a 250 ms stimulus free trace interval and a 100 ms corneal airpuff unconditioned stimulus (US). Rats with lesions of the hippocampus were significantly impaired relative to the neocortical and sham lesioned control rats. Analyses of different behavioral parameters (e.g. percent conditioned responses, amplitude, and area of response) indicated that all of the measures for the conditioned response were significantly impaired by the hippocampal lesion. The unconditioned response was not significantly affected by the lesion, and there was no significant difference among the groups after 2 days of subsequent conditioning with the delay paradigm (zero trace interval). We conclude that the hippocampus is required for rats to learn the association between a tone CS and an airpuff US when a 250 ms trace interval is interposed between the two stimuli.  相似文献   

2.
Mice lacking the glutamate receptor subunit delta2 (GluRdelta2) are deficient in cerebellar long-term depression (LTD) at the parallel fibre-Purkinje cell synapses. We conducted delay and trace eyeblink conditioning with these mice, using various temporal intervals between the conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US). During trace conditioning in which a stimulus-free trace interval (TI) of 250, 100 or 50 ms intervened between the 352-ms tone CS and 100-ms US, GluRdelta2-mutant mice learned as successfully as wild-type mice. Even in the paradigm with TI = 0 ms, in which the end of CS and onset of US are simultaneous, there was no difference between the GluRdelta2-mutant and wild-type mice in their acquisition of a conditioned response. However, in the delay paradigm in which the 452-ms CS overlapped temporally with the coterminating 100-ms US, GluRdelta2-mutant mice exhibited severe learning impairment. The present study together with our previous work [Kishimoto, Y., Kawahara, S., Suzuki, M., Mori, H., Mishina, M. & Kirino, Y. (2001) Eur. J. Neurosci., 13, 1249-1254], indicates that cerebellar LTD-independent learning is possible in paradigms without temporal overlap between the CS and US. On the other hand, GluRdelta2 and cerebellar LTD are essential for learning when there is CS-US temporal overlap, suggesting that the cerebellar neural substrates underlying eyeblink conditioning may change, depending on the temporal overlap of the CS and US.  相似文献   

3.
The rate and level of classical nictitating membrane (NM)/eyelid conditioning in rabbits established by pairing a pontine nucleus stimulation conditioned stimulus (CS) with an air puff unconditioned stimulus (US) were studied at 6 interstimulus intervals (ISIs). Similar to earlier studies which used peripheral CSs, an inverted U-shaped function relating ISI and conditioning was generated. Interstimulus intervals of 250 and 500 ms produced the highest levels of conditioning, 100, 1000 and 2000 ms ISIs resulted in lower levels of conditioning, and no conditioning was established with a 50 ms ISI. These results demonstrate that a normal ISI function can be established when direct activation of cerebellar mossy fibers is used as a CS instead of conventional peripheral CSs.  相似文献   

4.
The hippocampus is believed to be an important structure for learning tasks that require temporal processing of information. The trace classical conditioning paradigm requires temporal processing because the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US) are temporally separated by an empty trace interval. The present study sought to determine whether the hippocampus was necessary for rats to perform a classical trace fear conditioning task in which each of 10 trials consisted of an auditory tone CS (15-s duration) followed by an empty 30-s trace interval and then a fear-producing floor-shock US (0.5-s duration). Several weeks prior to training, animals were anesthetized and given aspiration lesions of the neocortex (NEO; n = 6), hippocampus and overlying neocortex (HIPP; n = 7), or no lesions at all (control; n = 6). Approximately 24 h after trace conditioning, NEO and control animals showed a significant decrease in movement to a CS-alone presentation that was indicative of a conditioned fear response. Animals in the HIPP group did not show conditioned fear responses to the CS alone, nor did a pseudoconditioning group (n = 7) that was trained with unpaired CSs and USs. Furthermore, all groups except the HIPP group showed conditioned fear responses to the original context in which they received shock USs. One week later, HIPP, NEO, and control animals received delay fear-conditioning trials with no trace interval separating the CS and US. Six of seven HIPP animals could perform the delay version, but none could perform the trace version. This result suggests that the trace fear task is a reliable and useful model for examining the neural mechanisms of hippocampally dependent learning. Hippocampus 1998;8:638–646. © 1998 Wiley-Liss, Inc.  相似文献   

5.
Kung JC  Su NM  Fan RJ  Chai SC  Shyu BC 《Brain research》2003,970(1-2):58-72
The emotional component of nociception is seldom distinguished from pain behavioral testing. The aim of the present study was to develop a behavioral test that indicates the emotional pain responses using the classical conditioning paradigm. The role of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in the process of this pain conditioning response was also evaluated. In laser-pain conditioning, free moving rats were trained to associate a tone (conditioned stimulus, CS) and short CO2 laser pulsation (unconditioned stimulus, US). Monotonous tone (800 Hz, 0.6 s) was delivered through a loud-speaker as CS. CO2 laser pulses (5 W at 50 or 100 ms in duration) applied to the hind paw was adopted as US. The CS–US interval was 0.5 s. Laser-pain conditioning was developed during 40 CS–US pairings. CS and US pairing with 100-ms laser pulse stimuli was more effective in establishing conditioning responses than that of 50-ms stimuli. The conditioning responses remained, tested by presenting CS alone, immediate to and 24 h subsequent to training. The performance of laser-pain conditioning was significantly reduced after bilateral lesioning of the ACC. Similar results were also obtained by bilateral lesions of the amygdala. The conditioning responses were also diminished following morphine treatment. The association between a neutral stimulus and a noxious stimulus could be demonstrated in a Pavlovian conditioning test in free moving rats. Thus, the conditioned response may be employed as a measure of the emotional component of the nociception. It is also suggested that the ACC may play an important role in mediating this conditioning effect.  相似文献   

6.
The present study sought to determine whether post-training excitotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus would disrupt retention of fear conditioned using a trace procedure. Rats were trained using one of six procedures. Forward trace conditioning consisted of 10 trials in which a 16-s tone conditional stimulus (CS) was followed by a 28-s stimulus-free trace interval and then a mild footshock unconditional stimulus (US). We used two forms of delay conditioning where the tone and footshock co-terminated. Short delay used a 16-s tone and long delay used a 46-s tone. Backward trace conditioning was the same as forward trace, except that the order of the CS and US was reversed. CS-only and US-only were similar to forward trace except that the footshock or tone, respectively, was eliminated. One day later, animals received either an N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA)-induced lesion of the dorsal hippocampus or sham surgery. One week later, the rats were tested for freezing to the tone in a novel context. The next day, they were tested for freezing to the original training context. Hippocampal lesioned trace conditioned rats showed significantly less freezing during the tone compared with their sham lesioned controls. The lesion did not affect freezing during the tone in delay conditioning, nor in the other training conditions. During the 1-min period after tone offset, there was a trend in all hippocampal lesioned animals toward a deficit in freezing, compared with their corresponding sham lesioned controls, although only short delay, forward and backward trace groups showed a significant deficit. Hippocampal lesions also attenuated contextual conditioning. Thus, the hippocampus is critical for the consolidation and/or expression of a trace fear conditioned stimulus.  相似文献   

7.
To what extent the hippocampus is required for contextual conditioning remains a matter of debate. The present experiments examined the effects of ibotenate hippocampal lesions on discriminative fear conditioning to context in mice using measures of freezing in two conditioning paradigms. In both paradigms animals received foot shock as the unconditional stimulus (US) when placed in the (conditioning) context and no foot-shock when placed in the other (neutral) context. In both contexts, animals were presented with a tone as the conditioned stimulus (CS). In the conditioning context there was either no interval (delay condition) or a 30-s interval (trace condition) between tone CS end and shock US onset. These two paradigms were used because theory predicts that in the trace condition animals would learn more about contextual cues as predictors, or not, of shock US occurrence than in the delay condition. In agreement with this, we observed that sham-operated mice learned the context discrimination faster in the trace than in the delay condition. Lesions of the hippocampus significantly retarded, but did not prevent, the acquisition of the context discrimination in the trace condition. In contrast, lesions produced an opposite (facilitatory) effect in the delay condition, which was mainly observed during tone CS presentation. The data suggest that mice used two distinct competing strategies in solving this discrimination task: (i) a strategy relying on the processing of background contextual stimuli allowing direct establishment of context-US associations of different strengths, and (ii) a conditional cue (tone)-based strategy. Hence, hippocampal lesions may impair the use of the former strategy while exacerbating (unmasking) the use of the latter.  相似文献   

8.
During classical eyeblink conditioning, animals acquire adaptive timing of the conditioned response (CR) to the interstimulus interval (ISI) between the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US). To investigate this coding of the timing by the cerebellum, we analyzed Purkinje cell activities during acquisition of new timing after we shifted the ISI. Decerebrate guinea pigs were conditioned to an asymptotic level of learning using a delay paradigm with a 250-ms ISI. A 350-ms tone and a 100-ms electrical shock were used as the CS and US, respectively. As reported previously in other species, Purkinje cells in the simplex lobe exhibited three types of responses to the CS: excitatory, inhibitory, or a combination of the two. After we increased the ISI to 400 ms, the frequency of the CR stayed at an asymptotic level, but the latency of the CR peak became gradually longer. Two types of cells were observed, based on changes in the nature of their response to the CS; one changed its type of response in parallel with learning the new timing, while the other did not. There was no correlation between the type of response before and after we changed the ISI. In some cells, the peak latency of activities became longer or shorter, while the type of response did not change. These results suggest that some Purkinje cells code the timing of the CR, but do not play a consistent role in shaping the CR over a range of ISIs.  相似文献   

9.
Trace conditioning relies on the maintained representation of a stimulus across a trace interval, and may involve a persistent trace of the conditioned stimulus (CS) and/or a contribution of contextual conditioning. The role of hippocampal structures in these two types of conditioning was studied by means of pretraining lesions and reversible inactivation of the hippocampus in rats. Similar levels of conditioning to a tone CS and to the context were obtained with a trace interval of 30 s. Neurotoxic lesions of the whole hippocampus or reversible muscimol inactivation of the ventral hippocampus impaired both contextual and tone freezing in both trace- and delay-conditioned rats. Dorsal hippocampal injections impaired contextual freezing and trace conditioning, but not delay conditioning. No dissociation between trace and contextual conditioning was observed under any of these conditions. Altogether, these data indicate that the ventral and dorsal parts of the hippocampus compute different aspects of trace conditioning, with the ventral hippocampus being involved in fear and anxiety processes, and the dorsal hippocampus in the temporal and contextual aspects of event representation.  相似文献   

10.
The first experiment in this study used the classical heart rate (HR) conditioning paradigm to determine if rabbits could associate an auditory conditioned stimulus (CS) and a fear-producing shock-unconditioned stimulus (US) separated by an empty 10-s trace interval. Trace conditioned rabbits (n = 7) acquired significant bradycardiac conditioned HR responses on CS-alone test trials during a single 35-trial conditioning session. Control animals (n = 7) which received unpaired CSs and USs did not show HR conditioning. During a retention session of CS-alone trials 24 h after the conditioning session, some trace-conditioned animals showed conditioned HR responses immediately following CS onset (n = 3), while others showed responses appropriately timed to the US onset (n = 4) used in trace conditioning 24 h earlier. Thus, rabbits remember the duration of the long 10-s trace interval 24 h after a single day of training. The second part of this study sought to determine if cells in the dorsal hippocampus play a role in trace HR conditioning. Rabbits were given bilateral ibotenic acid lesions in the neocortex (n = 7) or dorsal hippocampus (n = 8). During trace conditioning and retention, neocortical animals showed conditioned HR responses to the CS, whereas the hippocampal group showed no significant HR conditioning. One week after trace conditioning, the same animals received a delay HR conditioning session where no trace interval separated the CS and US. During delay conditioning, hippocampal animals showed significant conditioned HR responses to the CS that were similar to the neocortical group. Thus, the dorsal hippocampus plays a critical role in rabbit HR conditioning when the CS and US are separated by a 10-s trace interval. This paradigm may be ideal for in vivo electrophysiological recording studies because rabbits are easily immobilized during the testing procedure, and learning occurs during a single day of training.  相似文献   

11.
Sensory and affective components are included in the overall behavioral manifestation in a nocifensive reaction. We have developed a behavioral model using classical conditioning to differentiate the affective component from the sensory responses following a thermal noxious stimulus. In laser-pain conditioning, free moving rats were trained to associate a tone (conditioned stimulus, CS) and short CO2 laser pulsation (unconditioned stimulus, US). A monotonous tone (800 Hz, 0.6 s) was delivered through a loudspeaker as the CS. CO(2) laser pulses (5 W at 100 ms in duration) applied to the hind paw were adopted as the US. The CS-US interval was 0.5 s. The conditioned responses as quantitatively measured by their body movement were developed over a period of 40 CS-US pairings. These conditioned responses were found retained when the rats were tested by presenting CS alone, immediate to and 24 h subsequent to training. The conditioned responses however diminished significantly following both morphine and buspirone treatment. This method demonstrated that neutral auditory stimuli could form association with unlearned nocifensive responses evoked by noxious CO2 laser pulses stimuli. Thus, the assessment of conditioned response may be a valuable tool for the measurement of the affective component of nociception.  相似文献   

12.
We examined the effects of acute injections of competitive N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist 2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (APV) into the dorsal hippocampus on contextual fear conditioning and classical eyeblink conditioning in C57BL/6 mice. When injected 10 to 40 min before training, APV severely impaired contextual fear conditioning. Thus, APV injection under these conditions was sufficient to suppress hippocampal NMDA receptors. To investigate the role of hippocampal NMDA receptors on eyeblink conditioning, we carried out daily training of mice during 10-40 min after injection of APV. In the delay eyeblink conditioning, in which the unconditioned stimulus (US) is delayed and terminates simultaneously with the conditioned stimulus (CS), APV-injected mice acquired the conditioned responses (CRs) as well as artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF)-injected control mice did. However, in the trace eyeblink conditioning, in which the CS and US were separated by a stimulus-free trace interval of 500 ms, APV-injected mice showed severe impairment in acquisition of the CR. There was no significant difference in pseudo-conditioning between APV- and aCSF-injected mice. These results provide evidence that the NMDA receptor in the dorsal hippocampus is critically involved in acquisition of the CR in long trace eyeblink conditioning.  相似文献   

13.
It has been hypothesized that the amnesic effects of alcohol are through selective disruption of hippocampal function. Delay and trace fear conditioning are useful paradigms to investigate hippocampal-dependent and independent forms of memory. With delay fear conditioning, learning of explicit cues does not depend on normal hippocampal function, whereas learning explicit cues in trace fear conditioning does. In both delay and trace fear conditioning, the hippocampus is involved in learning to contextual cues, but it may not be entirely necessary. The present study investigates the effects of alcohol on the acquisition of delay and trace fear conditioning in mice, using freezing as a measure of learning. Male C57BL/6J mice were injected with 0.8 or 1.6 g/kg of 20% v/v alcohol and were immediately exposed to eight tone-footshock pairings in which the conditional stimulus (CS) either coterminated with a footshock unconditional stimulus (US) (delay conditioning) or was separated from the footshock by a 30-s trace interval (trace conditioning). During trace, but not delay fear conditioning, 0.8 g/kg alcohol impaired learning to a tone CS. This dose also impaired context-dependent learning in both procedures (although only slightly for trace fear conditioning). The 1.6 g/kg alcohol exerted a nonselective impairment on learning. The impairment by alcohol of learning to a tone CS when it is hippocampus-dependent, but not when it is hippocampus-independent provides further support for the hypothesis that alcohol exerts a selective effect on hippocampus-dependent learning.  相似文献   

14.
A neuron-like adaptive element with computational features suitable for classical conditioning, the Sutton-Barto (S-B) model, was extended to simulate real-time aspects of the conditioned nictitating membrane (NM) response. The aspects of concern were response topography, CR-related neuronal firing, and interstimulus interval (ISI) effects for forward-delay and trace conditioning paradigms. The topography of the NM CR has the following features: response latency after CS onset decreases over trials; response amplitude increases gradually within the ISI and attains its maximum coincidentally with the UR. A similar pattern characterizes the firing of some (but not all) neurons in brain regions demonstrated experimentally to be important for NM conditioning. The variant of the S-B model described in this paper consists of a set of parameters and implementation rules based on 10-ms computational time steps. It differs from the original S-B model in a number of ways. The main difference is the assumption that CS inputs to the adaptive element are not instantaneous but are instead shaped by unspecified coding processes so as to produce outputs that conform with the real-time properties of NM conditioning. The model successfully simulates the aforementioned features of NM response topography. It is also capable of simulating appropriate ISI functions, i.e. with maximum conditioning strength with ISIs of 250 ms, for forward-delay and trace paradigms. The original model's successful treatment of multiple-CS phenomena, such as blocking, conditioned inhibition, and higher-order conditioning, are retained by the present model.  相似文献   

15.
A long-standing question in neuroscience is how the brain controls movement that requires precisely timed muscle activations. Studies using Pavlovian delay eyeblink conditioning provide good insight into this question. In delay eyeblink conditioning, which is believed to involve the cerebellum, a subject learns an interstimulus interval (ISI) between the onsets of a conditioned stimulus (CS) such as a tone and an unconditioned stimulus such as an airpuff to the eye. After a conditioning phase, the subject’s eyes automatically close or blink when the ISI time has passed after CS onset. This timing information is thought to be represented in some way in the cerebellum. Several computational models of the cerebellum have been proposed to explain the mechanisms of time representation, and they commonly point to the granular layer network. This article will review these computational models and discuss the possible computational power of the cerebellum.  相似文献   

16.
Acquisition of the trace-conditioned eye blink response (CR) is mediated by a variety of brain structures, including the cerebellum, the hippocampus, and brain stem nuclei. We examined the effects of a neuronal sodium channel antagonist (lamotrigine) on the ability of rabbits to acquire an eye blink CR after 6.5 min of cerebral ischemia. New Zealand white rabbits (n = 31) were randomly assigned to sham (S), normothermic ischemia (N), hypothermic (30 degrees C) ischemia-(H), or lamotrigine (50 mg/kg) treated (L) groups. In the N, H, and L groups, 6.5 min of global cerebral ischemia was produced using an inflatable neck tourniquet. Trace conditioning was started on the 7th postischemic day. The conditioned stimulus consisted of a tone (85 dB, 6 kHz) presented for 100 ms. The unconditioned stimulus was an air puff (150 ms duration) directed at the cornea. The interval between the end of the conditioned stimulus and the start of the unconditioned stimulus (the trace interval, TI) was 300 ms in duration. A trace-conditioned response was defined as an eye blink that was initiated during the TI. Eighty trials were delivered daily for 15 days. Neurologic deficits were greatest in the N group, and these animals had fewer CRs (149 +/- 157) than animals in the S (509 +/- 214) or H (461 +/- 149) groups (P < 0.05 by analysis of variance). Animals in the L group had a total number of CRs (380 +/- 253) that was intermediate between the S and N groups. Histologic evidence of neural injury was greatest in the N group. This study demonstrates that a brief episode of cerebral ischemia results in the impairment of this test of neurobehavioral function. Both hypothermia and lamotrigine were able to attenuate the impairment of eye blink trace-conditioned responses produced by cerebral ischemia.  相似文献   

17.
Extracellular single neuronal firings were recorded in the auditory cortex of rats (n = 4) performing a visual reaction-time task with a warning tone (10 kHz, 10 ms duration), which preceded the imperative light stimulus by an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 1.4 s. Thirty-six neuronal firings were evoked by the warning tone, with the peak latency being between 15 and 55 ms. Among them, nine neurons (25%) showed an increased firing frequency following the evoked response during the ISI, which was, in average, 2.5 times as high as the firing frequency during the baseline period. When the tones were presented independent of the imperative stimulus, such sustained increase in neuronal firing was not observed. Activation of the sensory cortex during the ISI may constitute one of the neuronal modulations related to preparatory set.  相似文献   

18.
The trace conditioning paradigm in which the conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus (CS-US) interval is longer and the US and CS do not overlap requires retention of a ‘trace’ of the CS to associate it with the US. This task is difficult, requiring for learning about 5 times the number of trials to criterion as the standard delay task. The present study was undertaken to determine if: (1) the cerebellar interpositus nucleus is essential in trace as well as in delay conditioning; and (2) the lateral cerebellar cortex is involved when CS-US association over longer time intervals is required. Sixteen adult male New Zealand white rabbits had recording and in some cases lesion electrodes surgically implanted before training. They were trained daily with 126 paired trials of tone CS and airpuff US. The trace period between CS offset and US onset was 500 ms. Mean number of trials to criterion of 8/9 CRs was 466.9 trials. After a day of overtraining the animals had one of 3 surgeries: (1) electrolytic lesion of the left cerebellar interpositus nucleus (n= 3); (2) aspiration of the left lateral cerebellar cortex (HVI and HVIIA) and underlying cerebellar nuclei (n= 4); or (3) aspiration of HVI and HVIIA only (n= 9). Rabbits with only HVI and HVIIA removed exhibited a transient decrease in CRs but relearned with significant savings. The amplitude, area and latency of pre- and postlesion conditioned responses (CRs) was similar. When the interpositus nucleus was damaged, the animals' capacity for CRs on the side ipsilateral to the lesion was permanently abolished. It is concluded that the cerebellar interpositus nucleus but not the overlying lateral cerebellar cortex is essential for retention of trace classical conditioning. Nevertheless, the temporary abolition of CRs by removal of lateral cerebellar cortex suggests that the cerebellar cortex does play an important role in learning and retention of the trace conditioned response.  相似文献   

19.
In mice lacking glutamate receptor subunit delta 2 (GluR delta 2(-/-_ mice), cerebellar long-term depression (LTD) at the parallel fibre-Purkinje cell synapses is disrupted. Unlike the cerebellar LTD-deficient mice previously used for eyeblink conditioning, however, the abnormalities of the GluR delta 2(-/-) mice are restricted to the cerebellar cortex. In delay eyeblink conditionings (interstimulus interval of 252 and 852 ms), in which the conditioned stimulus (CS) overlaps temporally with a coterminating unconditioned stimulus (US), GluR delta 2(-/-) mice are severely impaired in learning, strongly supporting the hypothesis that cerebellar cortical LTD is essential for delay conditioning. In the trace paradigm, in which a stimulus-free trace interval of 500 ms intervened between the CS and US, GluR delta 2(-/-) mice learned as successfully as wild-type mice, indicating that cerebellar LTD is not necessary for trace conditioning. Thus, the present study has revealed a cerebellar LTD-independent learning in eyeblink conditioning.  相似文献   

20.
Hippocampal and amygdaloid neuroplasticity are important substrates for Pavlovian fear conditioning. The hippocampus has been implicated in trace fear conditioning. However, a systematic investigation of the significance of the trace interval has not yet been performed. Therefore, this study analyzed the time-dependent involvement of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors in the dorsal hippocampus in one-trial auditory trace fear conditioning in C57BL/6J mice. The NMDA receptor antagonist APV was injected bilaterally into the dorsal hippocampus 15 min before training. Mice were exposed to tone (conditioned stimulus [CS]) and footshock (unconditioned stimulus [US]) in the conditioning context without delay (0 s) or with CS-US (trace) intervals of 1-45 s. Conditioned auditory fear was determined 24 h after training by the assessment of freezing and computerized evaluation of inactivity in a new context; 2 h later, context-dependent memory was tested in the conditioning context. NMDA receptor blockade by APV markedly impaired conditioned auditory fear at trace intervals of 15 s and 30 s, but not at shorter trace intervals. A 45-s trace interval prevented the formation of conditioned tone-dependent fear. Context-dependent memory was always impaired by APV treatment independent of the trace interval. The results indicate that the dorsal hippocampus and its NMDA receptors play an important role in auditory trace fear conditioning at trace intervals of 15-30-s length. In contrast, NMDA receptors in the dorsal hippocampus are unequivocally involved in contextual fear conditioning independent of the trace interval. The results point at a time-dependent role of the dorsal hippocampus in encoding of noncontingent explicit stimuli. Preprocessing of long CS-US contingencies in the hippocampus appears to be important for the final information processing and execution of fear memories through amygdala circuits.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号